Abstract
Rodents use rhythmic whisker movements at frequencies between 4 and 12 Hz to sense the environment that will be disturbed when the animal touches an object. The aim of this work is to study the response adaptation to rhythmic whisker stimulation trains at 4 Hz in the barrel cortex and the sensitivity of cortical neurons to changes in the timing of the stimulation pattern. Longitudinal arrays of four iridium oxide electrodes were used to obtain single-unit recordings in supragranular, granular, and infragranular neurons in urethane anesthetized mice. The stimulation protocol consisted in a stimulation train of three air puffs (20 ms duration each) in which the time interval between the first and the third stimuli was fixed (500 ms) and the time interval between the first and the second stimuli changed (regular: 250 ms; “accelerando”: 375 ms; or “decelerando” stimulation train: 125 ms interval). Cortical neurons adapted strongly their response to regular stimulation trains. Response adaptation was reduced when accelerando or decelerando stimulation trains were applied. This facilitation of the shifted stimulus was mediated by activation of NMDA receptors because the effect was blocked by AP5. The facilitation was not observed in thalamic nuclei. Facilitation increased during periods of EEG activation induced by systemic application of IGF-I, probably by activation of NMDA receptors, as well. We suggest that response adaptation is the outcome of an intrinsic cortical information processing aimed at contributing to improve the detection of “unexpected” stimuli that disturbed the rhythmic behavior of exploration.
Significance Statement
To detect objects, rodents scan the environment by rhythmic movements of whiskers at 4-12 Hz, suggesting that this rhythmic behavior may facilitate stimulus detection. This rhythmic input in the somatosensory cortex will be disturbed when the animal touches an object. We study whether spike responses in the somatosensory cortex are sensitive to small changes in the timing of the rhythmic sensory input. We find that responses were facilitated when a modification of the stimulation interval was introduced in the stimulation sequence. This facilitation was mediated by activation of NMDA receptors and increased during periods of EEG activation induced by IGF-I application. We suggest that the detection mechanism of shifted stimulus may play a role in sensory processing.
Introduction
Animals actively gather sensory information through self-generated movements. For example, eye movements are used to foveate interesting regions of visual space. Eye movements can determine the visual sensory input that falls on the retina. Active touch is also a common behavior to discern the shape, size, and texture of objects (Szwed et al., 2003; Kleinfeld et al., 2006; Diamond and Arabzadeh, 2013; Bale and Maravall, 2018).
In rodents, tactile discrimination during exploratory behaviors is based on repetitively and rapidly swept back and forth of whiskers (whisking) across objects or surfaces in repeated rhythmic movements at frequencies between 4 and 12 Hz, scanning their surroundings to obtain tactile information about nearby objects (Carvell and Simons, 1990; Berg and Kleinfeld, 2003; Von Heimendahl et al., 2007; Jadhav et al., 2009). Whisking in free air induces spike trains in the somatosensory barrel cortex (BC; Curtis and Kleinfeld, 2009), which will be disturbed when the animal touches an object (for review, see Kleinfeld and Deschênes, 2011). In addition, spike trains suffer a reduction of the response with time in a process termed as response adaptation. The functional role of the response adaptation process may be to alter the sensitivity of neurons to encode more efficiently sensory stimuli (Sharpee et al., 2006; Maravall et al., 2007, 2013; Ganmor et al., 2010) or to improve the detectability of rare stimuli by decreasing responses to frequent stimuli (Dragoi et al., 2002; Ulanovsky et al., 2003). It is established that neuronal responses in the BC adapt robustly to repetitive whisker stimulation (Ahissar et al., 2000, 2001; Chung et al., 2002; Khatri et al., 2009; Martin-Cortecero and Nuñez, 2014). However, response adaptation during whisking may be altered when a change in the interval between stimuli occurs, for example when whiskers touch an object.
The effect of the temporal pattern in a spike train has been studied by Segundo and colleagues (Perkel et al., 1963; Segundo et al., 1963; Villa et al., 2007). Stimulation with triplets in Aplysia ganglia showed that neuronal responses were sensitive to the timing of the stimulation pattern (Segundo et al., 1963). These findings suggest that a train of spikes induced, for example, by whisking may contain information according to the temporal pattern of the sensory input. Our intention in this work is to determine whether spike responses in the BC are sensitive to small changes in the timing of the stimulation pattern (temporal jitter). We applied rhythmic whisker stimulation trains of three stimuli at a mean frequency of 4 Hz in which all intervals were equal (“regular” stimulation pattern, 250 ms between stimuli) or varied in a ramp: “accelerando” (intervals decreased, 375 + 125 ms) or “decelerando” (intervals increased, 125 + 375 ms). These stimulation patters try to mimic the sensory input when a whisker touches an object during whisking. Sensitivity to stimulation patterns has a general interest because it contributes to understand how the somatosensory system handles complex temporal information.
Materials and Methods
Animals
The experiments were performed on 24 adult C57Bl/6J mice (25–30 g weight) of either sex. Mice were group housed with a 12 h light/dark cycle and had ad libitum to food and water. In accordance with European Community Council Directive 2010/63/UE all animal procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee of the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (CEI72-1286-A156). Efforts were made to minimize animal suffering as well as to reduce the number of animals used.
Electrophysiological recordings
General anesthesia was induced by peritoneal injection of urethane (1.2 g/kg, i.p.). Depth of anesthesia was sufficient to eliminate pinch withdrawal, palpebral reflex and whisker movement and was assessed periodically throughout the surgical procedure and during electrophysiological recordings. Animals were placed in a Kopf small-animal stereotaxic device (David Kopf Instruments) in which surgical procedures and recordings were performed. The body temperature was maintained at 37°C. An incision was made exposing the skull and a small hole was drilled in the bone in the skull overlying the whisker barrel field of the somatosensory BC (A: 0–2 mm, L: 3–4 mm from bregma and V: 0.3–1.1 μm from the dura mater; Paxinos and Franklin, 2003). Longitudinal arrays of four Iridium oxide electrodes (15 μm electrode diameter; 200 μm separation between electrodes; 1–2 MΩ impedance; Q1x4-5mm-200-177-Q4) manufactured by NeuroNexus Technologies were used to obtain single-unit recordings in the BC. The array was carefully advanced perpendicular to the cortical surface, using a remote-controlled micromanipulator (David Kopf Instruments). Thus, the most superficial recording site was located at layer 2/3 (300 μm from the surface) and the deepest one was located at layer 6 (1100 μm from the surface; Fig. 1A).
Extracellular signals were preamplified through a home-made preamplifier. The signals were further amplified and bandpass filtered between 300 Hz and 3 kHz using an AC preamplifier (1902 isolated preamplifier; Cambridge Electronic Design), digitized at 10 kHz (Power 1401, CED), and stored for off-line analysis. The recording session lasted for 4–5 h after the beginning of the recording procedure.
Whisker stimulation
For each recording, the principal whisker was determined as the whisker that caused the strongest response to manual mechanical deflection. All selected neurons responded to passive whisker deflections using a hand-held probe or air-puff stimuli generated by a pneumatic pressure pump (Picospritzer). After identification of the receptive field with the handheld probe, air-puff stimulation was delivered by aiming a 1 mm inner diameter polyethylene tube 10–20 mm away from the receptive field so that whiskers were pushed backward by a 20 ms duration puff (20–30 psi).
The stimulation protocol consisted in a stimulation train of three air puffs in which the time interval between the first and the third air puff was fixed (500 ms) and the time interval between the first and the second air puff changed (Fig. 1B):
Regular stimulation pattern: the time interval between stimuli was 250 ms.
Accelerando stimulation pattern: the interval between the first and the second stimuli was 375 ms and thus, the second and the third stimuli were closer (125 ms).
Decelerando stimulation pattern: the interval between the first and the second stimuli was 125 ms and thus, the second and the third stimuli were 375 ms faraway.
In all cases the mean frequency of the stimulation train was 4 Hz and was repeated 100 times at 0.5 Hz (stimulation block). These stimulation protocols tried to compare a regular stimulation pattern at 4 Hz with a stimulation pattern containing temporal jitter in the second stimuli. This pattern of stimulation is reminiscent of that produced by an object that interferes with the rhythmic movement of whiskers during the whisking behavior.
Drugs
The following drugs were used: 2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (AP5; 50 μm), which is a selective NMDA (Sigma-Aldrich Quimica) receptor antagonist. AP5 was injected (0.1 μl) through a cannula connected to a Hamilton syringe located 300 μm deep in the BC to affect the entire cortical column. IGF-I (1 μg/g body weight; Peprotech) was injected intraperitoneally to increase cortical excitability.
Histology
Following the recording session, electrolytic lesions were performed using five pulses of 5 μA for 10 s at intervals of 10 s at the top and at the bottom of the electrode array for subsequent histologic reconstruction of the electrode tracks in coronal sections through the barrel cortex. At the end of the experiment the brain was processed for histology to corroborate the electrode track and electrode location. Animals were euthanized with an overdose of pentobarbital and perfused through the heart with 100 ml of 0.9% NaCl, followed by 100 ml 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer. Coronal sections (50 μm) were obtained using a Leica freezing microtome and stained with cresyl violet (Fig. 1A).
Statistical analysis
One to two cells were detected from each single electrode based on the spike amplitude and wave form. Selected neurons showed a response to the stimulation of a whisker located in the contralateral pad. Only neurons that met this criterion were used for further analysis. According to their firing rate of the recorded neurons we assumed that all recorded neurons were pyramidal (see Results section). Spikes were digitally stored for time series analysis. All subsequent analysis was performed with Spike 2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design).
Spontaneous spike firing was calculated from periods of 120 s without whisker stimulation. Peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were calculated for cortical neurons, where time 0 ms of the histogram corresponded to the onset for the first stimulus in the stimulation train. Spike responses were calculated in a 50 ms poststimulus time window following each stimulus in the stimulation train (PSTH; 1 ms bin-width; 100 stimulation trains; Fig. 1C). The response for the first stimulus of the stimulation train was considered 100%; the response for the second and the third stimuli was calculated with respect to this one. These response percentages were calculated in each neuron and the average and the SEM are indicated in the text and figures. Also, PSTHs were calculated for the first or for the last 25 stimulation trains of the 100 stimulation train block. Response duration was defined as the time elapsed from the onset to offset responses; response onset was defined as the first of three consecutive bins displaying significant activity (2× higher than the mean firing rate) after stimulus and response offset as the last bin of the last three consecutive bins displaying significant activity. Response duration was measured for each stimulus into the stimulation train for each of the different stimulation patterns.
Statistical analysis was performed using GraphPad Prism 7 software. Statistical analyses consisted, for the most part, of paired comparisons of responses evoked in the same neuron by different stimulation trains or drugs. Data were considered normally distributed, according to the Shapiro–Wilk normality test, and thus, we used parametric statistics. We used the t test (paired or independent) to compare data from two groups. The statistic test was applied on the values of response (spikes/stimulus; Table 1) in different conditions. Differences were considered statistically significant at the 95% level (p < 0.05). However, the normalized percentage respect to the first response in the stimulation train is shown in Figures and the statistical significant is indicated with asterisks. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.
Results
To study the effect of different stimulation patterns in BC responses we separated recorded cortical cells into three groups according to the cortical depth and observation of the histologic preparations (Fig. 1A): supragranular cells (up to 500 μm below the pia; layers 2–3); granular cells between 500 and 800 μm approximately representing layer 4 cells and infragranular cells below 800 μm (layers 5–6). Neurons recorded in the BC (n = 66) showed a low spontaneous firing rate as is characteristic of the urethane anesthetized mice (de Kock and Sakmann, 2009). Granular and supragranular neurons showed the lowest spontaneous firing rate 0.9 ± 0.22 spikes/s (n = 16) and 1.0 ± 0.22 spikes/s (n = 16), respectively, whereas infragranular neurons showed higher firing rate, 1.7 ± 0.18 spikes/s (n = 34). The low spontaneous firing rate strongly suggests that these recordings could be performed in pyramidal dendrites because they have firing rates larger than somatic spike rates (Moore et al., 2017). Results are based on the analysis of BC neurons which responded to whisker stimulation of the contralateral pad. There were no gender-related differences in the results; for this reason, they were pooled together. Responses consisted in a short-latency spike-burst of 1–3 action potentials. Supragranular neurons showed 1.4 ± 0.26 spikes/stimulus (n = 16), granular neurons 1.3 ± 0.16 spikes/stimulus (n = 16), and infragranular neurons 1.9 ± 0.29 spikes/stimulus (n = 34), measured in a 50 ms poststimulus time window following the stimulus onset.
The stimulation pattern altered response adaptation
Cortical neurons adapted strongly their response to regular stimulation trains at 4 Hz, as shown by the PSTHs. A representative example of a neuron located in layer 5 is shown in Figure 1C (left histogram) in which the spike response decreased during the stimulus train. The population analysis indicated that adaptation was lower in the granular layer than in supragranular or infragranular layers (Table 1, regular stimulation pattern; Fig. 2, blue circles in all figures), consistent with other previous studies (Higley and Contreras, 2003; Khatri et al., 2004; Martin-Cortecero and Nuñez, 2014). The response adaptation was reduced if the interval between the first and the second stimulus increased from 250 to 375 ms (accelerando stimulation pattern; orange circles in all figures) or decreased to 125 ms (decelerando stimulation pattern; gray circles in all figures). Examples of these differences are depicted in Figure 1C, where PSTHs were calculated from a unit located in layer 5 when different stimulation patterns were applied. The response to the second and third stimuli of the stimulation train varied according to the interval between stimuli. Figure 2 shows population data measuring the mean percentage of the response respect to the control (first stimulus of the stimulation train; 100%) for each cortical layer (A–C, supragranular, granular or infragranular layers, respectively); plots of responses to the 1–3 whisker stimuli are shown on the left. Differences between one and three stimulus responses during the application of the accelerando or decelerando stimulation pattern relative to the corresponding response during the application of a regular stimulation pattern are shown on the right. In all cases the response to the second and third stimuli was higher than during the application of the regular stimulation pattern, indicating that the adaptation observed in all layers during the application of the regular stimulation pattern was diminished when the interval between stimuli changed.
The response to the second and third stimuli in supragranular neurons (n = 14) was higher during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern comparing to the regular stimulation pattern (28 and 35%, respectively; p = 0.045 and p = 0.0022, respectively; paired t test; Fig. 2A; Table 1) as well as during the application of the decelerando stimulation pattern (34 and 22%, respectively; p = 0.0022 and p = 0.0014, respectively; paired t test). Equally, this effect was observed in granular neurons (n = 12). Responses were higher during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern (24 and 41%, respectively; p = 0.0192 and p = 0.0019; paired t test) or the decelerando stimulation pattern than it was expected during the application of the regular stimulation pattern (29 and 18%, respectively; p = 0.0029 and p = 0.019; paired t test; Fig. 2B; Table 1).
Neurons in infragranular layers also altered their response when a change of the interval between the first and the second stimuli occurred, especially during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern. Layer 5 neurons (n = 23) showed a higher response during the application of the accelerando stimulation patter than it was expected during the application of the regular stimulation pattern (21 and 35%, for the second and third stimuli of the train, respectively; p = 0.0103 and p = 0.0005, respectively; paired t test; Fig. 2C; Table 1). During the application of the decelerando stimulation pattern only the response to the second stimulus was higher than the response during the regular stimulation pattern (20%; p = 0.0181; paired t test; Fig. 2C; Table 1); the response to the third stimulus was not statistically different (10%; p > 0.05). In conclusion, the above results indicated that cortical cells are able to detect a change in the stimulation pattern, facilitating the detection of no-regular timing stimuli that appear into a rhythmic sequence of stimulation.
To establish whether this mechanism is proper of the cortex or appears in other subcortical relay stations of the somatosensory pathway we recorded multiunit activity in the ventro-posterior (VPM) or posterior-medial (POm) thalamic nuclei. VPM recordings showed that response adaptation was the same when VPM neurons were stimulated with the regular, accelerando or decelerando stimulation patterns (n = 12; Fig. 3A). The same occurred when POm thalamic neurons were recorded (n = 8; Fig. 3B).
It is expected that the detection of the stimulus may change over time during the application of successive trains of stimuli. Above findings in the BC show the mean response after the application of a block of 100 stimulation trains. It is logical to believe that these findings are not the same during the long-lasting stimulation train. Figure 4 compares the response of the first with the last 25 stimulation trains during the application of the 100 stimulation trains with regular, accelerando or decelerando stimulation patterns. In all layers, response adaptation during the application of the regular stimulation pattern was larger during the first stimulation trains (red points) in comparison with the last stimulation trains (Fig. 4A, green points). Equally, the facilitation observed when the interval between the first and the second stimuli was longer, during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern respect to the responses during the regular stimulation pattern, was also larger in granular and infragranular neurons (Fig. 4B). However, supragranular neurons showed facilitation respect to the responses observed during the application of the regular stimulation pattern between the second and the third stimuli for the first 25 stimulation trains which disappeared for the last stimulation trains. During the application of the decelerando stimulation pattern the response facilitation observed between the first and the second stimuli was larger in all layers in the first stimulation trains than in the last stimulation trains (Fig. 4C). Thus, facilitation processes evoked by a change in the interval between stimuli decayed during the stimulation period. The response adaptation was similar in the last stimulation trains during the application of the regular, accelerando or decelerando stimulation pattern for neurons of all layers.
NMDA receptors are involved in the detection of a change in the stimulation pattern
Above results indicated that response facilitation occurred when the interval between stimuli was reduced from 250 to 125 ms. Paired-pulse facilitation is because of the activation of NMDA-mediated responses in many cortical regions (Thomson, 2000; Dan and Poo, 2004; Citri and Malenka, 2008). We have investigated the role of this glutamatergic receptor in the detection of the stimulation pattern by the application of the specific NMDA receptor blocker AP5 in the BC (Fig. 5A). Application of AP5 through a cannula located next to the recording microelectrode in the BC (50 μm; 0.1 μl; recordings performed 10 min later) decreased tactile responses of supragranular neurons from 1.5 ± 0.19 to 0.7 ± 0.09 spikes/stimulus (p < 0.001; n = 8; paired t test) and from 1.8 ± 0.13 to 1.1 ± 0.09 spikes/stimulus (p = 0.005; n = 8; paired t test) in infragranular neurons. However, granular neurons were not significantly affected (from 1.5 ± 0.21 to 1.2 ± 0.10 spikes/stimulus; p > 0.05; n = 7; paired t test; Fig. 5B). A representative example is shown in the PSTHs of Figure 5A. AP5 reduced whisker responses, decreasing response duration (see Fig. 7C).
Supragranular (n = 8) and infragranular (n = 8) neurons showed response adaptation during the application of the regular stimulation pattern 10 min after AP5 application (Fig. 5C, blue circles). They also adapted in presence of AP5 when the accelerando or decelerando stimulation pattern was applied (Fig. 5C, orange or gray circles, respectively); facilitation of the second and the third response was not observed, indicating that response facilitation described above elicited by a change in the interval between stimuli was because of the activation of NMDA receptors. Granular neurons also showed response adaptation during the application of the regular and decelerando stimulation patterns after AP5 application. However, a significant reduction of adaptation was observed during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern in presence of AP5 (p = 0.0325; n = 7; paired t test; Fig. 5C, orange circles), suggesting that other mechanisms may be involved.
In a previous work we have demonstrated that an increase of NMDA-mediated response induces an increase in response duration of the BC neurons (Barros-Zulaica et al., 2014). Thus, we measured the response duration for each stimulus into the stimulation train during the application of different stimulation patterns (see Materials and Methods) to further study the participation of NMDA receptors in the detection of the stimulation pattern. The response duration of granular cells (27.0 ± 1.2 ms) was significantly shorter than responses in supragranular (31.8 ± 1.5 ms; p = 0.0211; t test) and infragranular neurons (31.1 ± 1.0 ms; p = 0.0151; t test) when the response duration was measured in the first stimulus of the stimulation train. Figure 6A shows the mean response duration of neurons located in supragranular (n = 11), granular (n = 11), and infragranular layers (n = 16) during the application of the regular, accelerando or decelerando stimulation patterns (Table 2). When the interval between stimuli was reduced to 125 ms (between the second and the third stimuli in the accelerando stimulation pattern or between the first and the second stimuli in the decelerando stimulation pattern) the duration increased in the second one, reaching statistical significance in supragranular and infragranular neurons. Granular neurons only showed significant differences during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern. These findings support that the response facilitation observed during the application of the accelerando or decelerando stimulation pattern was mainly because of activation of NMDA receptors. Accordingly, the application of AP5 into the BC (50 μm; 0.1 μl) reduced the duration of whisker responses in supragranular (from 30.5 ± 1.3 to 25.6 ± 0.7 ms; n = 8; p = 0.0012; paired t test) and in infragranular neurons (from 32.3 ± 1.3 to 25.3 ± 0.5 ms; n = 8; p < 0.001; paired t test). Granular neurons slightly decreased their response duration but did not reach statistical significance (from 26.5 ± 1.5 to 23.9 ± 0.7 ms; n = 7; p > 0.05; paired t test). Furthermore, response duration was not altered by the stimulation pattern in presence of AP5 (Fig. 6B). These findings matched above results obtained when spike responses were measured (Fig. 5), indicating that shortening the interval between stimuli in a stimulation train generated a response facilitation that was mediated by the activation of NMDA receptors.
IGF-I facilitates stimulus detection
Above results have been obtained under urethane anesthesia, a state characterized by the prevalence of synchronous slow oscillations (Steriade, 1993; Castro-Alamancos and Bezdudnaya, 2015). However, natural active whisking occurs in aroused animals exploring the environment; a state characterized by the absence of slow oscillations (cortical activation). It has been demonstrated that systemic application of IGF-I increases neuronal firing (Carro et al., 2000; Nuñez et al., 2003; Mysoet et al., 2015) and activates the electroencephalogram (Trueba-Sáiz et al., 2013). Thus, IGF-I was injected intraperitoneally to study whisker responses and the stimulation pattern effect during a state of increased cortical activation such as occurs during anesthesia. We performed simultaneous unit and field potential recordings in the BC of urethane anesthetized mice (n = 8) in control conditions (after injection of 0.1 ml of saline solution, i.p.) and 20 minutes after intraperitoneal injection of IGF-I (1 μg/g body weight dissolved in 0.1 ml of saline solution). Field potentials were recorded to determine that IGF-I induced cortical activation. In control conditions, field potentials showed slow waves evoked by the anesthetic (Fig. 7A, control). Intraperitoneal administration of IGF-I elicited an increase of the firing rate and a reduction of the slow wave amplitude in the cortex (Fig. 7A, IGF-I). This effect was accompanied with an increase in the response to whisker stimulation in neurons located in all cortical layers (Fig. 7B, closed bars). Supragranular neurons increased whisker responses from 1.4 ± 0.08 spikes/stimulus in control condition (n = 6) to 2.5 ± 0.35 spikes/stimulus 20 min after IGF-I intraperitoneal injection (p = 0.0128; n = 6; paired t test). Similarly, granular neurons increased whisker responses from 1.6 ± 0.16 spikes/stimulus in control condition to 2.2 ± 0.24 spikes/stimulus (p = 0.0293; n = 6; paired t test) and infragranular neurons increased whisker responses from 1.8 ± 0.22 spikes/stimulus in control condition to 2.7 ± 0.2 spikes/stimulus after IGF-I injection (p = 0.0049; n = 9; paired t test). The evoked facilitation by IGF-I of whisker responses was blocked if AP5 (50 μm; 0.1 μl) was injected in the BC 10 min before IGF-I injections (Fig. 6B, green bars). In this condition, supragranular neurons showed 1.0 ± 0.3 spikes/stimulus (p > 0.05, respect to control values; n = 4; paired t test), granular neurons showed 1.2 ± 0.29 spikes/stimulus (p > 0.05; n = 4; paired t test) and infragranular neurons showed 1.2 ± 0.38 spikes/stimulus after IGF-I injection (p > 0.05; n = 5; paired t test). Consequently, the increased whisker response evoked by IGF-I may be because of activation of NMDA receptors.
As it was expected, IGF-I injection also induced an increase of the response duration (Fig. 7C, closed bars). Supragranular neurons increased response duration from 29.2 ± 1.08 ms in control condition (n = 6) to 34.5 ± 1.3 ms 20 min after IGF-I intraperiotneal injection (p = 0.0128; n = 6; paired t test). Similarly, granular neurons increased whisker response from 26.5 ± 0.68 ms in control condition to 29.0 ± 1.21 ms (p = 0.0278; n = 6; paired t test) and infragranular neurons increased whisker response from 29.5 ± 2.02 to 35.2 ± 2.15 ms after IGF-I injection (p < 0.001; n = 9; paired t test). Similarly, the increase in the response duration evoked by IGF-I was blocked if AP5 (50 μm; 0.1 μl) was injected in the BC 10 min before IGF-I injections (Fig. 7C, green bars). In this condition, supragranular neurons showed a response duration after IGF-I injection of 25.3 ± 1.44 ms (p > 0.05, respect to control values; n = 4; paired t test), granular neurons showed 26.5 ± 0.58 ms (p > 0.05; n = 4; paired t test) and infragranular neurons showed 24.6 ± 2.22 ms after IGF-I injection (p > 0.05; n = 5; paired t test).
In addition, response adaptation to the regular stimulation pattern was reduced and the facilitation of responses during the application of the accelerando or decelerando stimulation patterns was enhanced in all cortical layers after IGF-I administration (Fig. 7D), suggesting that response adaptation and detection of different timings were facilitated during a cortical activation state induced, in this case, by IGF-I.
Discussion
During exploration, rodents move their whiskers rhythmically across objects or surfaces (Carvell and Simons, 1990; Berg and Kleinfeld, 2003; Grant et al., 2009). This rhythmic behavior induces spike responses in BC neurons that displayed a progressive adaptation with time (Derdikman et al., 2006; Katz et al., 2006). The rhythmic behavior to explore the environment is not exclusive of the somatosensory system or the whisker system, also appears in the visual or auditory systems and in different species, suggesting that it may facilitate the detection of stimuli (Szwed et al., 2003; Kleinfeld et al., 2006; Diamond and Arabzadeh, 2013). The effect of anesthesia on the adaptation, following the results reported for an oddball paradigm in anesthetized and freely-moving rats (Eriksson and Villa, 2005), suggests that response adaptation in un-anesthetized animals is likely to be much larger than any observed modification during anesthesia. Our results show that response adaptation was reduced in BC layers when a modification of the stimulation interval was introduced in the stimulation sequence (shifted stimulus), decreasing (decelerando) or increasing (accelerando) the interval between the first and the second stimuli. The response to the shifted stimulus was greater than estimated during the 4 Hz regular stimulation. This facilitation was mediated by activation of NMDA receptors since the effect was blocked by AP5. Facilitation of the shifted stimulus increased during periods of EEG activation induced by IGF-I application, probably by activation of NMDA receptors because the increment of whisker responses induced by IGF-I was also blocked by AP5. This process may contribute to improve the detection of “unexpected” stimuli that disturbed the rhythmic behavior of exploration.
Adaptation, the reduction of the response to ongoing or repeated stimulation, occurs across species and sensory modalities (Ahissar et al., 2000; Maravall et al., 2007; Kheradpezhouh et al., 2017). The functional role of the response adaptation process may be to alter the sensitivity of neurons to encode sensory stimuli more efficiently (Sharpee et al., 2006; Maravall et al., 2007, 2013; Ganmor et al., 2010) or to improve the detectability of rare stimuli by decreasing responses to frequent stimuli (Dragoi et al., 2002; Ulanovsky et al., 2003). In the somatosensory system, most studies observed response depression in different layers of BC to whisker stimulation (Ahissar et al., 2000, 2001; Martin-Cortecero and Nuñez, 2014; Kheradpezhouh et al., 2017). We also observed a response depression when the regular stimulation pattern was applied. Our findings also showed that the introduction of a temporal jitter increased the response to the shifted stimuli in all cortical layers. An increased response to irregular stimulation has been previously described in the BC, suggesting that may contribute to the detection of relevant stimuli (Lak et al., 2008; Kheradpezhouh et al., 2017).
Our findings showed that adaptation changed during the stimulation time. If we compared the response of the first stimulation trains with the last stimulation trains, we observed a reduction of adaptation with time, mainly in supragranular and infragranular layers (Fig. 4A). In addition, the response facilitation of the shifted stimulus was also reduced along the stimulation time (Fig. 4B,C), indicating that it may contribute to the detection of unexpected and non-repetitive stimuli.
During quiescent states the sensory adaptation observed in the neocortex is high and may be a consequence of depression at thalamocortical pathways; during activated states animals are alert exploring the environment; in this case sensory adaptation is reduced (Castro-Alamancos, 2004). Results present here also showed a reduction of sensory adaptation during activated states (in this case induced by IGF-I injection; compare Figs. 2 and 7 during the application of the regular stimulation pattern). The reduction was more evident in supragranular and infragranular cells as was indicated early (Ahissar et al., 2001). Changes in sensitivity during adaptation may allow the neuronal circuit to respond optimally to a larger range of stimuli despite the limited dynamic range of neuronal firing (Derdikman et al., 2006; Katz et al., 2006). Synaptic mechanisms, such as enhancement of inhibition (Dealy and Tolhurst, 1974) or depression of excitatory synapses (Adorján et al., 1999; Ahissar et al., 2000; Sanchez-Vives and Mccormick, 2000; Chung et al., 2002; Castro-Alamancos, 2004; Higley and Contreras, 2006; Díaz-Quesada and Maravall, 2008) have been proposed as mechanisms for adaptation. In vivo experiments have ruled out enhanced inhibition since iontophoretic application of GABAA receptor antagonists or agonists did not affect adaptation (Martin-Cortecero and Nuñez, 2014; Ayala and Malmierca, 2018). Derdikman et al. (2006) demonstrated that responses to frequent stimuli can depress, remain unchanged, or facilitate, depending on the information conveyed by the stimulus and the cortical layer. Here, a change in the interval between stimuli is enough to facilitate the response to the shifted stimulus. Thus, these results support the notion that cortical adaptation is not an automatic mechanism during repetitive stimulation but rather a process that depends and changes according to the sensory input pattern.
Recordings of thalamic cells showed a small adaptation during the application of a regular stimulation train, consistent with other previous studies conducted on VPM or POm neurons (Hellweg et al., 1977; Ahissar et al., 2000; Ahissar and Zacksenhouse, 2001; Sosnik et al., 2001; Hartings et al., 2003; Khatri et al., 2004; Martin-Cortecero and Nuñez, 2014). Moreover, either VPM or POm thalamic neurons were not sensitive to a change in the interval between stimuli, suggesting that this property is generated, at least in part, in the BC. Other brain structures may also contribute to the detection of relevant stimuli. For example, acetylcholine could enhance sensory detection, processing, and plasticity with the intervention of complex synaptic interactions (Ego-Stengel et al., 2001; Castro-Alamancos, 2009; Rahman and Berger, 2011; Nuñez et al., 2012; Barros-Zulaica et al., 2014).
A likely mechanism for facilitation when the interval was shorter (125 ms) may be a paired-pulse facilitation mediated by the activation of NMDA receptors (Thomson, 2000; Dan and Poo, 2004; Citri and Malenka, 2008). Many long-term modifications in synaptic efficacy in the cortex have been proposed to be mediated by the activation of NMDA receptors and to be the cellular basis of the learning machinery and the detection of relevant stimuli (Nuñez et al., 2012; Nabavi et al., 2014; Gruart et al., 2015; Díez-García et al., 2017). For example, antagonists of NMDA receptors block cortical plasticity after electrical thalamic stimulation (Heynen and Bear, 2001) or whisker repetitive stimulation (Barros-Zulaica et al., 2014). Indeed, the facilitation observed during the accelerando or decelerando stimulation pattern of the shifted stimulus was blocked after cortical application of the NMDA receptor antagonist AP5 in supragranular and infragranular layers. However, this facilitation was not affected by AP5 in granular neurons during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern, probably because of the different functional roles of each layer (Feldmeyer et al., 2013), in which different NMDA receptors subtypes are involved (Kaczmarek et al., 1997; Scheppach, 2016). Thus, the detection of an object that is interposed during the rhythmic whisker exploration may be facilitated by a mechanism that implies activation of NMDA receptors, mainly in supragranular and infragranular neurons. Granular cells express less this effect may be because they receive an important synaptic input from the thalamus, that is not sensitive to a change in the interval between stimuli, and faithfully follow the stimulation pattern, regardless of its temporal pattern.
A reduction of response adaptation to the second stimulus was also observed when the accelerando stimulation pattern was applied in comparison with the regular stimulation pattern. In this case, the interval between the first and the second stimuli was increased to 375 ms in which a facilitation mediated by NMDA receptors would be difficult to admit. However, this facilitation was also blocked by AP5 except in the case of granular cells. The increase of the interval between stimuli from 250 to 375 ms can increase the synaptic current available that can be activated by the second stimulus. In fact, previous studies have shown that at low stimulation frequencies BC neurons evoked greater responses than high stimulation frequencies (Ahissar et al., 2001; Ahissar and Zacksenhouse, 2001). In addition, membrane potential recordings of neurons in the somatosensory cortex showed that a sensory stimulus may induce depolarizations that lasted hundreds of milliseconds (Yamashita and Petersen, 2016). Thus, AP5 could reduce part of this synaptic current, blocking the facilitation observed during the application of the accelerando stimulation pattern in control conditions. Additional experiments are required to investigate the synaptic and network mechanisms responsible for this response facilitation.
It is known that systemic application of IGF-I increases neuronal firing (Carro et al., 2000; Nuñez et al., 2003; Mysoet et al., 2015) and induces EEG activation (Nishijima et al., 2010; Trueba-Sáiz et al., 2013). IGF-I is well known to be expressed throughout the brain, including the somatosensory cortex (Rotwein et al., 1988; Yamaguchi et al., 1990). Here, we show that IGF-I induced an increase in whisker responses by the activation of NMDA receptors because it was blocked by previous injection of AP5 in the BC. During the activation state evoked by IGF-I, BC neurons detected the change in the interval between stimuli, suggesting that the mechanism of detection of shifted stimulus may play a role in sensory processing during anesthesia, and also during activate states.
It is known that alterations of the rhythmic stimulation pattern induce important changes in the response (Perkel et al., 1963; Segundo et al., 1963; Villa et al., 2007), indicating that the discharge pattern of a train of spikes carries much more information than the simple mean discharge. Our results show that such higher-order information processing is likely to be primarily the outcome of an intrinsic cortical processing, built up on ascending thalamic input unaffected by stimulus dynamics. Consequently, the rhythmic exploration behavior observed in many sensory systems (Szwed et al., 2003; Kleinfeld et al., 2006; Diamond and Arabzadeh, 2013), instead of diminishing sensibility by the induction of response adaptation, may facilitate the detection of stimuli that appear surprisingly during the exploration by the activation of cortical properties.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by Grants from the Spanish Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad Grant (SAF2016-76462 AEI/FEDER, UE) and the Neuroheuristic Research Group. We thank Dr. Washington Buño for critical revision of this paper, Jose Manuel Ibarz for electronics support, and Marta Callejo for technical help.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.