Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleNew Research, Disorders of the Nervous System

Chronic Alcohol Drinking Slows Brain Development in Adolescent and Young Adult Nonhuman Primates

Tatiana A. Shnitko, Zheng Liu, Xiaojie Wang, Kathleen A. Grant and Christopher D. Kroenke
eNeuro 1 April 2019, 6 (2) ENEURO.0044-19.2019; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0044-19.2019
Tatiana A. Shnitko
1Division of Neuroscience, Oregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, Oregon 97006
2Advanced Imaging Research Center, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon 97239
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Tatiana A. Shnitko
Zheng Liu
1Division of Neuroscience, Oregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, Oregon 97006
2Advanced Imaging Research Center, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon 97239
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Xiaojie Wang
1Division of Neuroscience, Oregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, Oregon 97006
2Advanced Imaging Research Center, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon 97239
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Xiaojie Wang
Kathleen A. Grant
1Division of Neuroscience, Oregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, Oregon 97006
3Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon 97239
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kathleen A. Grant
Christopher D. Kroenke
1Division of Neuroscience, Oregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, Oregon 97006
2Advanced Imaging Research Center, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon 97239
3Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon 97239
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Christopher D. Kroenke
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The transition from adolescence to adulthood is associated with brain remodeling in the final stages of developmental growth. It is also a period when a large proportion of this age group engages in binge alcohol drinking (occasional consumption of four to five drinks leading to intoxication) and heavy alcohol drinking (binge drinking on ≥5 d in a month). Here we report on magnetic resonance imaging of developmental changes in the brain occurring during late adolescence and early adulthood (3.5–7.5 years of age) in a rhesus macaque model of alcohol self-administration. Monkeys were imaged prior to alcohol exposure, and following ∼6 and ∼12 months of daily (22 h/d) access to ethanol and water. The results revealed that the brain volume increases by 1 ml/1.87 years throughout the late adolescence and early adulthood in controls. Heavy alcohol drinking reduced the rate of brain growth by 0.25 ml/year per 1 g/kg daily ethanol. Cortical volume increased throughout this period with no significant effect of alcohol drinking on the cortical growth rate. In subcortical regions, age-dependent increases in the volumes of globus pallidus, thalamus, brainstem, and cerebellum were observed. Heavy drinking attenuated the growth rate of the thalamus. Thus, developmental brain volume changes in the span of late adolescence to young adulthood in macaques is altered by excessive alcohol, an insult that may be linked to the continuation of heavy drinking throughout later adult life.

  • underage drinking
  • brain growth
  • magnetic resonance imaging
  • thalamus
  • white matter
  • ethanol
  • self administration

Significance Statement

Alcohol abuse during late adolescence and early adulthood is a risk factor for the development of alcohol dependence. This longitudinal study used a macaque model of alcohol self-administration and in vivo MRI to quantify the impact of chronic alcohol use on developmental changes occurring within the brain during this period. Chronic alcohol self-intoxication reduced the growth rate of brain, cerebral white matter, and subcortical thalamus. Thus, daily alcohol drinking during the critical transition to adulthood significantly impacts critical areas of sensory motor integration, concomitant with a decrease in cortical white matter, documented in the primate brain neural circuitry implicated in the propagation of alcohol use disorder.

Introduction

Adolescence is associated with experiencing alcohol binge drinking to extreme intoxication (Patrick and Terry-McElrath, 2017) coincident with the brain maturation processes. In vivo magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies in humans and other species have confirmed that the brain volume (VB) continues to increase throughout childhood and adolescence (Pfefferbaum et al., 1994; Mengler et al., 2014; Scott et al., 2016). The volumetric changes are attributed to synaptic pruning, leading to a reduction of gray matter in the cerebral cortex and an increase in white matter (WM) volume, concurrent in some brain regions, with protracted myelination of late-developing fiber systems (Giedd et al., 1999; Lebel et al., 2008; Sullivan et al., 2011; Yeatman et al., 2014; Levman et al., 2017; Narvacan et al., 2017). In addition, specific age-related growth trajectories have been demonstrated for the medial frontal cortex, thalamus, amygdala, hippocampus, and cerebellum (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2014; Squeglia et al., 2015). Heavy alcohol use in human subjects ranging in age from 9 to 23 years attenuates white matter growth, increases age-related decline in cortical volume, and reduces cortical thickness (Luciana et al., 2013; Squeglia et al., 2015; Pfefferbaum et al., 2016). These studies correspond to imaging experiments in rodents demonstrating that vapor alcohol exposure during adolescence affects cerebral cortical thickness (Vetreno et al., 2017). Chronic alcohol self-administration also attenuated brain growth in a selected line of alcohol-preferring rats (Pfefferbaum et al., 2006). To our knowledge, there are only a couple of reports that have found effects of alcohol on volumetric characteristics of subcortical nuclei. One study found reduced brainstem and caudate volumes in adolescents who engage in heavy alcohol drinking (Squeglia et al., 2014), and another study found a reduction in the volume of the cerebellum, hypothalamus, and hippocampus in alcohol-exposed rodents (Vetreno et al., 2017).

The majority of in vivo MRI brain-developmental experiments in humans are cross-sectional due to the resources and time required for a longitudinal design. An exception is the multisite National Consortium on Alcohol and Neurodevelopment (N-CANDA), which is designed to quantify the longitudinal effects of low and heavy alcohol use in children throughout adolescence on the development of cortical regions and the white matter. Importantly, this study includes scanning subjects before any alcohol use (∼9 years of age; Pfefferbaum et al., 2018). The N-CANDA studies included alcohol use assessments, with the caveat that the quantity and frequency of alcohol intake are estimates in human subject studies where high rates of inaccurate self-reported alcohol use might significantly impact the results (Clark et al., 2016; Bertol et al., 2017). The nonhuman primate (NHP) model of alcohol self-administration can control a variety of variables contributing to brain-imaging outcomes, most notably the precise measurements of alcohol intake, diet, daily schedules, and health care (Grant and Bennett, 2003). Therefore, translational MRI research using this model for tracking developmental brain changes might be especially valuable (Zahr and Pfefferbaum, 2017). However, there are relatively few MRI studies that report on overall anatomic change in the maturing NHP brain, especially during late adolescence and early adulthood (Knickmeyer et al., 2010; Scott et al., 2016; Uematsu et al., 2017), much less the effect of alcohol on brain growth parameters. Thus, although reduced cortical gray matter volume was demonstrated in heavy-drinking rhesus macaques, the study was underpowered to specifically address changes in brain growth when chronic ethanol drinking begins in late adolescence (Kroenke et al., 2014).

This gap in our knowledge was addressed in the present study, which measured volumetric trajectories occurring in the rhesus macaque brain during the period of late adolescence to early adulthood and how these changes are affected by chronic alcohol self-administration. We tested the hypothesis that chronic alcohol self-administration dose-dependently decreases whole brain, cortical, and white matter volumes in the macaque. We also predicted that subcortical structures undergoing growth during this age range would also be vulnerable to alcohol-dependent effects on volume in the heavy drinker (HDs).

Materials and Methods

Animals

Male (n = 58) and female (n = 13) rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta; N = 71; cohorts 4, 5, 6a, 6b, 7a, 7b, 10, and 14; for details, see www.matrr.com) were obtained from the Oregon National Primate Research Center (ONPRC) breeding colony. All monkeys except cohort 4 were enrolled in the study at 3.5–5.5 years of age and 3–6 kg in body weight. All animals were housed indoors in rooms with controlled temperature (20–22°C), humidity (65%), and an 11 h light cycle with lights on at 7:00 A.M. Each subject was housed in a metal cage (0.8 × 0.8 × 0.9 m) and fed a diet of nutritionally complete 1 g of banana-flavored pellets (TestDiet) and fresh fruit. Food and fluid availability was dependent on experimental phase, as described below. All monkeys were weighed weekly, and ethanol intake based on body weight (in grams per kilogram) was calculated from the contemporary weights. All procedures were conducted according to the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Research Council, 2011) and approved by the Oregon National Primate Research Center Animal Care and Use Committee.

Alcohol self-administration

Equipment

The housing cages were equipped with operant panels as previously described (Vivian et al., 2001; Shnitko et al., 2017). Each panel incorporated a centrally located dowel and two drinking spouts, with a food receptacle located below one of the spouts. The receptacle was connected to a 1 g pellet dispenser (Med Associates). Below the receptacle, there was a recessed well that could detect the insertion of a finger via the breaking of an infrared beam. Each drinking spout was connected via tubing to a plastic bottle placed on a digital scale (Adventurer, Ohaus) located outside the cage. The bottle contained either filtered tap water or 4% ethanol (w/v, diluted in water), refilled with fresh solutions daily. All programming used a National Instruments interface and LabView software (LabView 2011, Service Pack 1, National Instruments). Thus, the operant panels were used for the ethanol and water self-administration and food delivery. To initiate food or fluid delivery, the dowel had to be pulled (closing an electrical circuit) and held (for details, see Grant et al., 2008).

Procedure to induce alcohol self-administration

Alcohol self-administration began after a baseline MRI assessment was completed. As previously described, a schedule-induced polydipsia procedure was used to induce monkeys to drink 4% (w/v) ethanol (Vivian et al., 2001; Grant et al., 2008). The induction sessions were given daily over a period of ∼4 months (∼120 sessions, 7 d/week). During each session, 1 g of banana-flavored pellets were delivered at a fixed time interval of 300 s until a required volume of either water or 4% (w/v) ethanol was consumed. Initially, monkeys were induced to drink water for 30 sessions (1 session/d), and then water was replaced with 4% ethanol (for monkeys assigned to ethanol experimental group). Monkeys were required to consume the ethanol solution at a volume sufficient to obtain 0.5 g/kg/d (30 d), 1.0 g/kg/d (30 d), and 1.5 g/kg/d (30 d) ethanol. Control (CTR) animals were required to drink water at the volumes corresponding to the ethanol doses. When an animal consumed the required volume, the ethanol spout became inoperative, but water was available through the other spout, and the remaining daily ration of food became available 2 h later (Grant et al., 2008).

Open access to alcohol self-administration

When induction to ethanol drinking was completed, all animals were allowed daily “open access” to water and ethanol (4% w/v, or water for control subjects) for 22 h/d, 7 d/week (Grant et al., 2008). During the open access period, the daily food ration (banana-flavored pellets) was divided equally into three meals; the first meal was available at the beginning of each daily session, and subsequent meals were available at 2 h intervals. The pellets were available under a fixed ratio 1 schedule. Initially, animals were allowed ∼6 months of concurrent access to water and 4% (w/v) ethanol, for a total of 201 ± 19 sessions (N = 61, including only adolescent cohorts; i.e., cohort 4 excluded). Subsequently, an additional 171 ± 16 self-administration sessions were allowed (∼12 months of open access). The study timeline for each cohort is shown in Figure 1A.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A–C, Schematic representations of experimental timeline (A) and MRI data processing (B, C). A, The timelines of the experiment are shown for each of eight cohorts of subjects admitted to and completed the study at different periods from 2008 to 2017. As indicated in the legend, the black squares represent an MRI sessions in the timeline of the experiment, and the numbers within them indicate an MRI protocol used at the time. B, The MR image processing is described in detail in the text (see Materials and Methods). C, Representation of the brain map of the monkey used for segmentation of ROIs in individual subjects (top, coronal; middle, axial; bottom, 3D image showing cortical surface, the cerebellum, and brainstem). The ROIs were labeled in INIA19 template brain.

Blood ethanol concentration

Before MRI and ethanol self-administration sessions, all animals were trained to comply with veinipuncture procedure without the use of anesthesia. Initially, subjects were trained to present their leg through an opening in the front wall of the housing cage. They then were trained to comply with blood sampling to be reinforced with raisins or trail mix. Blood samples were collected from the saphenous vein 30, 60, and 90 min after the start of the induction sessions and 7 h after the start of the 22 h drinking sessions in 4–5 d intervals. The blood samples were collected throughout the 3 months of ethanol induction and period of open access to ethanol. Ethanol content was assessed by gas chromatography (5890 Series II, Hewlett-Packard).

MRI data acquisition and processing

Before an imaging session, each animal was anesthetized with ketamine (15 g/kg, i.m.) in the housing environment. The animal was transferred to the ONPRC MRI Core Facility, intubated and maintained anesthetized with 1–1.5% isoflurane throughout imaging procedures (≤2 h). The images were acquired using a 3 T Siemens Magnetom MRI system. For all subjects in this study, the MRI protocol included an acquisition of T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MP-RAGE) with one of the four imaging protocols described in Table 1. The imaging data were acquired at baseline (before alcohol self-administration, MRI1), after 6 months of self-administration (MRI2), and after 12 months of self-administration (MRI3). Within each cohort, image acquisition settings were kept constant, except for cohorts 6a, 7a, and 7b, in which the MRI system was upgraded from a Trio to a Tim Trio between the MRI1 and MRI2 time points, and different MRI protocols were used for data acquisition in the cohorts. Between cohorts, experiments took advantage of upgrades in radiofrequency coil or scanner capabilities as they became available.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1:

Protocols of MR image acquisition

MRI data processing

Figure 1B shows a schematic representation of the processing for volumetric analysis within a NHP brain. At the beginning, all T1-weighted images collected at a given time point were averaged within a subject after motion correction and intensity bias correction. The motion correction was implemented by the rigid-body registration with “antsRegistrationSyN.sh,” which is an Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTS) standard function (Avants et al., 2008). The intensity bias field of each T1-weighted image was corrected using a B-spline approximation routine and a hierarchical optimization scheme implemented by “N4BiasFieldCorrection” in ANTS (Tustison et al., 2010). Next, a registration-based skull-stripping procedure was applied to the averaged MP-RAGE images successively from MRI1 to MRI3. At the beginning, the INIA19 template, which includes a T1-weighted head image, brain mask, and NeuroMap labels, was used a reference (Rohlfing et al., 2012). All corrected and averaged T1-weighted images in MRI1 were nonlinearly registered to the INIA19 head image using “antsRegistrationSyN.sh.” With the resulting transformation parameters, the INIA19 brain mask was then reversely aligned to each subject image to generate the brain mask using a nearest neighbor interpolation method. For MRI2 and MRI3, skull stripping was performed with the same method but updated references (MRI2 with MRI1 as reference, and MRI3 with MRI2 as reference). Based on the registration results, the label map of the INIA19 template was transformed to the space of each MRI1 image. With the same method, the label maps of brain images in MRI2 and MRI3 were generated based on the resulting transformation parameters. Subsequent to the analysis of brain volume changes, a secondary analysis was performed to determine whether individual brain regions are differentially susceptible to age or ethanol exposure. Accordingly, we explored effect of ethanol on age-related changes on a finer scale by parcellating the brain volume into the following 10 well resolved regions of interest (ROIs): WM, cortex, putamen, caudate nuclei, globus pallidus, thalamus, amygdala, hippocampus, brainstem, and cerebellum, illustrated in Figure 1C. The ROI boundaries were defined before the statistical analysis of the determined volumes.

Statistical data analysis

Imaging data for all subjects collected longitudinally across three time points were analyzed using a linear mixed model (LMM). A series of LMMs were used in this study to estimate the effects of age and chronic ethanol drinking on volumetric characteristics of the primate brain where Yi,j is VB, white matter volume (VWM) or volume of an ROI, as follows:

LMM 1: Yi,j ∼ β0 + β1agei,j + bjsubject + εi,j

LMM 2: Yi,j ∼ β0 + β1agei,j + β2intakei,j + β3agei,j × intakei,j + bjsubject + εi,j

LMM 3.1: Yi,j ∼ β0 + β1agei,j + β2groupi,j + β3agei,j × groupi,j + bjsubject + εi,j

LMM 3.2 Yi,j ∼ β0 + β1agei,j + β2groupi,j + β3sexi,j + β4agei,j × groupi,j + β5agei,j × sexi,j + β6groupi,j × sexi,j + β7agei,j × groupi,j × sexi,j + bjsubject + εi,j

First, we estimated age-related change in the VB of CTR animals using LMM 1, which incorporates a fixed effect of age (β1) and a random intercept (bj). Next, we explored whether the age effect on Yi,j depends on ethanol intake using LMM 2 with fixed effects of age and average ethanol intake (β2), as well as an age-by-intake interaction (β3), and a random intercept.

Subjects were categorized as low, binge, heavy, and very heavy drinkers based on the ethanol intake parameters of each individual during the open access period, as described in previous studies (Baker et al., 2014). Briefly, an animal was considered to be a low drinker if its average ethanol intake per day was <2 g/kg; an animal was considered a binge drinker if similar ethanol intake per day (<2 g/kg) resulted in at least a single blood ethanol concentration (BEC) value >80 mg%. The demarcation for heavy and very heavy drinkers was an average ethanol intake >3 g/kg during 30% of total given 22 h sessions, and >4 g/kg during 20% of total given 22 h sessions, respectively. The low and binge drinkers were collapsed into a group of non-HDs (NHDs; n = 19), and heavy and very heavy drinkers were grouped as HDs (n = 26). This parallels previous work in humans (Squeglia et al., 2015) and in nonhuman primates (Cervera-Juanes et al., 2017; Shnitko et al., 2019). The third linear mixed model (LMM 3.1) used the categorical variable (group) to determine how heavy and nonheavy alcohol drinking affects age-related changes in volume of the brain, white matter, and gray matter. If a significant effect of group or an age-by-group interaction was found, then the age-dependent changes were compared with the CTR group as a reference. Finally, we used LMM 3.2 to explore whether sex has a significant effect on age-related changes in the volume of the brain and interacts with effects of heavy and nonheavy alcohol drinking. All statistical analysis was performed in SPSS Statistics, version 24 (IBM). All significant effects of main factors and interactions were confirmed with an F test within each model indicated above. For the analysis of age-dependent change in the total brain volume, a significance threshold of p < 0.05 was considered to be significant. For the analysis of age-dependent change in the volume of the 10 ROIs, a significance threshold of p < 0.005 (adjusting for 10 ROIs) was considered significant. The significant fixed effects and interactions found in linear mixed model analyses were followed by post hoc t tests in which p values were adjusted for the number of comparisons, and differences of p < 0.05 were considered to be significant.

Results

Brain growth in late adolescent/young adult rhesus monkeys

Subjects were initially studied (MRI1) at ages ranging from 3.9 to 5.9 years and reached MRI3 at ages ranging from 5.6 to 7.5 years (Fig. 1A). First, we wanted to establish whether brain continues to grow in the NHPs over the age range of 3.9–7.5 years (Fig. 2A). The average brain volume across all CTR animals was 104.6 ± 9.4 ml at the MRI1, 105.9 ± 11.4 ml at the MRI2, and 107.1 ± 8.5 ml at the MRI3. Analysis of VB using LMM 1 revealed a significant effect of age on VB [β = 1.87, SE = 0.17, p < 0.00001; 95% confidence interval (CI), 1.5–2.1]; the VB of CTR animals increases by 1 ml/1.87 years.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Age-dependent brain growth in the nonhuman primates. A–C, Plots of individual brain volume changes across age in CTR (n = 16), NHD (n = 19), and HD (n = 26) NHPs. The VB of each subject was measured three times, as depicted by empty circles. An individual regression line is drawn across the three VB values for each subject. D, Average VB-by-age linear regression estimated for CTRs, NHDs, and HDs with 95% confidence interval depicted by the shaded area around the line. E, Correlation between age-dependent brain growth (βi) and individual daily ethanol intake averaged across 12 months of drinking. The βi value is based on the slope of the individual regression line, as shown in A–C. The ethanol intake is estimated based on daily consumption of 4% (w/v) ethanol solution averaged across ∼372 drinking sessions with the exception of the induction period (rs = 0-0.41, p < 0.01).

Voluntary ethanol intake during open access

After the induction period, 55 of the 71 animals were allowed to voluntarily self-administer ethanol during a 12 month period (Fig. 1A, open access). The average number of open access drinking days was 179 ± 13 before MRI2, and 164 ± 11 between MRI2 and MRI3. The average ethanol intake of NHD animals was 1.8 ± 0.5 and 2.1 ± 0.4 g/kg prior to MRI2 and between MRI2 and MRI3, respectively (Table 2). Low ethanol intake was reflected in the low levels of BEC collected 7 h after the initiation of drinking sessions. The group average BECs of NHD animals was 30 ± 7.6 mg% (individual averages ranged from 3 to 73 mg%), and 37 ± 7.2 mg% (individual averages ranged from 3 to 100 mg%) before and after MRI2, respectively. The average ethanol intake of HD animals was 3.0 ± 0.3 and 3.4 ± 0.4 g/kg during the same periods. Higher ethanol intake in the HD group resulted in relatively high group average BECs of 75 ± 19 mg% (individual averages ranged from 21 to 159 mg%) averaged across drinking sessions before MRI2 and 105 ± 24.2 mg% (individual averages ranged from 6 to 196 mg%) averaged across sessions between MRI2 and MRI3.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2:

Data sample characteristics

Heavy drinking attenuates growth in the late adolescent/young adult rhesus brain in an age-dependent manner

To explore whether chronic ethanol self-administration affects brain growth in nonhuman primates, we used LMM2 with age at MRI sessions 1-3, average ethanol intake, and the potential interaction between them as predictors of VB. This analysis identified that age (β = 1.9, SE = 0.2, p < 0.00001; 95% CI, 1.5–2.3) and ethanol intake (β = 1.2, SE = 0.5, p < 0.05; 95% CI, 0.24–2.2) significantly predicted VB, and these effects were interdependent, as demonstrated by a statistically significant age-by-intake interaction (β = −0.25, SE = 0.08, p < 0.01; 95% CI, −0.4 to −0.09). Thus, brain growth occurred in all NHP subjects studied over this range; however, it was attenuated by a factor of 0.25 ml/year per daily gram per kilogram ethanol. Figure 2E shows a correlation between individual brain growth (βi) and average ethanol intake of the subject across 12 months of open access (Spearman correlation, rs = −0.41, p < 0.01).

An additional analysis was performed in which ethanol-drinking monkeys were categorized as NHDs and HDs. Drinking group (CTR animals, NHDs, and HDs) was used as a fixed factor in LMM 3.1 to compare age-related brain growth between subjects with different drinking status. Figure 2, B and C, shows brain growth trajectories obtained based on data collected longitudinally during the three MRI sessions in NHD and HD monkeys. The LMM 3.1. analysis identified that age significantly predicts VB (F(1,123) = 141.9, p < 0.0001); however, the age-related VB was dependent on drinking group (age × group interaction: F(2,123) = 5.5, p < 0.01). Multiple comparisons between groups (Fig. 2D) show that the age-dependent increase of VB in HDs was significantly attenuated when compared with CTR animals (β´ = −0.9, SE = 0.3, adjusted p = 0.004; 95% CI, −1.5 to −0.3), while a similar rate of increase of VB was observed in CTRs and NHDs (β´ = −0.3, SE = 0.64, p = 0.32; 95% CI, −0.9 to −0.3). Importantly, the age-dependent changes in the VB significantly differ between two groups of NHD animals (late adolescent and adult) as depicted in Figure 2D. A linear mixed-model analysis revealed a significant effect of the groups (F(1,71) = 8.6, p < 0.01) on the age-related changes in VB. The brain continues to grow in younger NHD animals throughout the period from 4 to 7.5 years of age. In adult subjects, the growth stopped and VB declined by 0.27 ml/year. This differed from the adolescent NHD by 1.8 ml/year (β´ = 1.8, SE = 0.4, p = 0.0001; 95% CI, −0.9 to 2.6). Importantly, as indicated in Table 1, multiple hardware configurations were used as a result of MRI system upgrades over the course of this study. An analysis of the potential effect of MRI protocol on the results reported here was conducted using a mixed-model analysis. This revealed no significant effect of data acquisition parameters (effect of MRI protocol: F(3,73) = 1.4, p = 0.24; effect of age: F(1,123) = 68.7, p < 0.0001; effect of group: F(2,100) = 1.4, p = 0.25; age × group interaction: F(1,123) = 5.5, p < 0.01).

Last, we used the small subset of female subjects in this study (n = 13) to examine the possibility that the effects of alcohol exposure on brain growth was sex dependent. The analysis using LMM 3.2 revealed a significant effect of sex on VB (F(1,122) = 91.4, p < 0.0001), but no significant interactions between sex and age (F(1,122) = 0.4, p = 0.5) and between sex and group (F(2,120) = 1, p = 0.35), or a sex × age × group interaction (F(2,122) = 1.7, p = 0.2). These findings correspond to previous reports demonstrating sex-dependent differences in the brain volume of humans and nonhuman primates (Leonard et al., 2008).

The white matter and the thalamus are the most susceptible to chronic drinking during late adolescence

Previous studies have demonstrated varying age-dependent changes in the volume of different brain structures (Sowell et al., 2004; Sullivan et al., 2011; Dennison et al., 2013; Barnea-Goraly et al., 2014; Yeatman et al., 2014; Bernard et al., 2015) and have also shown that heavy alcohol drinking specifically attenuates white matter growth and decreases the volume of other brain structures such as the cortex, brainstem, caudate, hippocampus, and cerebellum (Luciana et al., 2013; Kroenke et al., 2014; Vetreno et al., 2017). To determine whether ethanol differentially affects the development of different brain regions, the brain was parcellated into 10 ROIs, and ethanol effects were examined within each of them separately, using LMM 3.1. The results of tests of fixed effects are presented in Table 3. According to our criteria, there were no significant age-related changes in the volume of caudate nucleus, hippocampus, putamen, cortex, and amygdala, as indicated by a lack of significant effects of age and group × age interaction (p < 0.005). However, age-dependent changes in volumes were identified for the cerebellum, brainstem, globus pallidus, thalamus, and white matter. The cerebellum, brainstem, and globus pallidus exhibit a statistically significant effect of age, but no age × group interaction. As shown in Table 4, the rates of growth in these structures were estimated to be 0.14, 0.012, and 0.16 ml/year in the cerebellum, globus pallidus, and brainstem, respectively. Thus, the brainstem volume undergoes the highest change (5% per year from the baseline level) during the age period from 3.9 to 7.9 years compared with two other structures: the globus pallidus (3.2%/year) and the cerebellum (1.6%/year).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3:

Tests of fixed effects in LMM 3.1

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4:

Estimates of fixed effects in LMM 1

As shown in the Table 3, the white matter and thalamus also exhibit significant age-related changes in their volumes; however, the volumetric changes were ethanol intake dependent. Table 5 demonstrates the estimated effects of age on volume for these ROIs within CTR, NHD, and HD groups. The analysis with LMM 3.1. revealed a striking effect of heavy drinking on the age-related growth in the white matter and the thalamus. The whole-brain white matter continued to increase during the period from 3.9 to 7.9 years in all subjects (Fig. 3B).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 5:

Estimates of fixed effects in LMM 3

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Heavy ethanol intake reduces the rate of the white matter growth in NHP brain. A, 3D representation of the cortical white matter in the brain. B, The estimated rate of white matter growth in the control, non-heavy-drinking, and heavy-drinking NHPs. The shadows above and below the regression lines depict the 95% confidence interval. C, The effect of chronic ethanol use on the average change in white matter volume occurred in three groups of subjects from baseline until the end of ethanol/water self-administration. The dots represent change in the volume measured in individual monkeys. Asterisks show the results of Bonferroni post hoc test, where p values adjusted for the multiple comparisons were **p < 0.01 and *p < 0.05.

In the CTR group, the estimated rate of VWM growth was 0.6 ml/year, and it was slightly lower in NHDs by 0.07 ml/year compared with CTR animals. However, the rate of growth was robustly attenuated in HDs compared with CTR animals by 0.25 ml/year. To confirm that there were no group differences in the VWM at baseline (MRI1) before ethanol self-administration, we performed ANOVA. The analysis revealed no significant differences among three groups of monkeys (F(2,58) = 1.1, p = 0.35), where the average VWM at MRI1 was 19.9 ± 2 ml in CTR animals, 20.4 ± 1.7 ml in NHDs, and 20.9 ± 2.2 ml in HDs. Figure 3C shows the change in the volume of WM that occurred between baseline (MRI1) and the end of alcohol or water (control) self-administration (an ∼12 month period). In the CTR monkeys, VWM increased by 4.7 ± 0.4%. Similar to CTR animals, the 4.0 ± 0.5% increase in the VWM was observed in NHDs. In HDs, the increase in the VWM was significantly smaller (2.6 ± 0.3%) compared with CTR. One-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of group (F(2,58) = 7.8, p < 0.001; Fig. 3C, results of post hoc analysis).

Thalamic growth also occurred in the NHP brain over this period (Fig. 4B). In the CTR group, the estimated rate of growth was 0.06 ml/year. It was significantly decreased in both types of alcohol drinkers, NHDs (by 0.02 ml/year) and HDs (0.03 ml/year). We confirmed that there were no group differences in the thalamic volume (VT) at baseline (MRI1) before ethanol self-administration using ANOVA. The analysis revealed no significant differences among the three groups of monkeys (F(2,58) = 0.3, p = 0.73), where the average VT at MRI1 was 1.7 ± 0.2 ml in CTR animals, 1.7 ± 0.15 ml in NHDs, and 1.7 ± 0.13 ml in HDs. The VT increased by 5 ± 0.7% between baseline (MRI1) and the end of self-administration in CTR animals and by 2.8 ± 0.6% in NHDs. In HDs, the 1.8 ± 0.6% increase in the VT was significantly smaller, as revealed as a significant effect of group in one-way ANOVA (F(2,58) = 5.5, p < 0.01; Fig. 4C, results of post hoc analysis).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Ethanol drinking attenuates thalamic growth in adolescent/early adult NHP. A, 3D representation of the thalamus in the brain. B, The estimated rate of the thalamic growth in the control, non-heavy-drinking, and heavy-drinking NHPs. The shadows above and below the regression lines depict 95% confidence interval. C, The effect of chronic ethanol use on the average change of thalamic volume occurred in three groups of subjects from baseline until the end of ethanol/water self-administration. The dots represent change in the volume measured in individual monkeys. Asterisks show the results of a Bonferroni post hoc test, where p values adjusted for the multiple comparisons were **p < 0.01.

Discussion

Rhesus macaques have been widely used as a translational model for investigating the neural substrates of human behavior and, particularly, alcohol use and abuse (Grant and Bennett, 2003; Wright and Taffe, 2014; Chandler et al., 2017; Shnitko et al., 2019; Thomas and Czoty, 2019). The present longitudinal study was focused on the measurement of brain changes that occur during late adolescence and early adulthood in rhesus monkeys (3.5–7.5 years of age) because this stage of development confers maximum risk for heavy drinking in humans and macaques (Helms et al., 2014; Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2017). We quantified changes in the volume of brain structures in macaques over this period of life and characterized the effects of chronic alcohol use on these changes. First, we demonstrated that the brain of the macaque continues to grow well into young adulthood (at least until 7.5 years of age), as previous studies have not measured brain volume increases beyond adolescence in the macaque (until 5 years of age; Malkova et al., 2006; Bakken et al., 2016; Scott et al., 2016). Second, the reduced rate of brain growth due to heavy ethanol drinking could be quantified with this analysis, and was found to be 0.25 ml/year per gram per kilogram daily ethanol intake. Finally, these results extend the chronic effects of heavy alcohol intake from brain volume reductions in adult rhesus macaques to significant impact on brain growth at an age range associated with a high risk of establishing a pattern of unhealthy alcohol consumption (Kroenke et al., 2014).

Although the rate of brain volumetric growth decreases during development (Bakken et al., 2016), the present data clearly show that brain continues to grow throughout late adolescence and early adulthood (Fig. 2A). A diverse set of neurodevelopmental processes contribute to this volume change. For example, the cortical volumes of gray and white matter undergo nonlinear changes across the human life span, with gray matter volume decreasing during late adolescence and early adulthood, while white matter volume continues to increase beyond adolescence and early adulthood (Sowell et al., 2003, 2004). A regressive neuronal process contributing to the gray matter volumetric changes is the synaptic pruning that begins in childhood and continues in adulthood (Sowell et al., 2001; Chung et al., 2017). In rhesus macaques, the initiation of synaptic pruning occurs between 1 and 3 years of age and accelerates during puberty (Eckenhoff and Rakic, 1991; Zecevic and Rakic, 1991). At the same time, myelination of many white matter fascicles is quite protracted over adolescent development in humans and NHPs (Sowell et al., 2001; Miller et al., 2012), which contributes to the increase in white matter volume throughout adolescence. As stated above, the present data are the first to document normal developmental changes in the brain volume beyond 5 years of age in rhesus macaques.

These neurodevelopmental underpinnings are thought to form the biological bases of robust behavioral and cognitive changes that occur concurrently and are accompanied by susceptibility to maladaptive behaviors in adolescents. Importantly, the age at onset of alcohol drinking is considered as one of the significant predictors of heavy alcohol drinking in adulthood (Poikolainen et al., 2001; Englund et al., 2008; Morean et al., 2012; but see Maimaris and McCambridge, 2014). Furthermore, recent N-CANDA experiments found robust effects of chronic alcohol abuse on brain volumes in human adolescents (Squeglia et al., 2015; Pfefferbaum et al., 2016, 2018). Similar to human subjects, macaque individuals that begin drinking to intoxication as late adolescents/young adults have a greater risk for future heavy drinking during mature adulthood (Helms et al., 2014; Baker et al., 2017). Further, the present study extended this finding to an estimate of the dose dependence of the ethanol effect on brain growth in rhesus macaques (Fig. 2).

There are numerous factors that could contribute to the observed impact of chronic alcohol drinking on brain growth. For example, cortical volume, which occupies the greatest volume fraction of the total brain in rhesus macaques (Rakic, 1995; Toro et al., 2008), could account for the observed impact of alcohol on the brain growth. In human cross-sectional data, heavy alcohol drinking was associated with smaller volumes of the lateral frontal and temporal cortices (Squeglia et al., 2015). However, here we established that cortical volume did not increase from 3.9 to 7.5 years in rhesus monkeys (Table 3) and that the global measure of cortical volume was not sensitive to the effects of chronic alcohol drinking. Another significant contributor to the total brain volume is the white matter volume. For example, morphometric studies using MRI have demonstrated that the volume of white matter increases faster with the total brain growth then gray matter volume (Rilling and Insel, 1999). An impact of chronic ethanol on the development of white matter subsequently contributes to the attenuated brain growth. In this study, daily alcohol drinking for over 12 months reduced white matter growth in the NHP brain (Fig. 3), and these results correspond to attenuated growth of white matter in heavy-drinking human subjects (Squeglia et al., 2015). Overall, the unique analyses of growth rate, rather than a single measure of volume, appears to be a key factor in documenting the adverse effects of alcohol on relatively late brain growth studied here.

The volume of subcortical structures also showed age-dependent growth in four of eight subcortical gray matter regions of interest included in the study (specifically, globus pallidus, thalamus, brainstem, and cerebellum). These findings partially correspond to a human cross-sectional pediatric study where the volumes of thalamus, brainstem, and cerebellum increase in a period from 4 to 18 years (Brain Development Cooperative Group, 2012). The data presented here help to establish that subcortical nuclei during adolescence are susceptible to voluntary intake of alcohol. The effect of chronic alcohol drinking on the volumes of subcortical gray matter regions in the human adult brain largely resulted in reduced volumes. Specifically, daily drinking of both heavy and nonheavy amounts of alcohol dramatically reduces the rate of growth in the thalamus, a finding that parallels the effect observed in the adult human brain with reductions in caudate, putamen, thalamus, cerebellum, and hippocampus (Sullivan et al., 2000; Chanraud et al., 2007; Yang et al., 2016). In adolescents, smaller hippocampal, thalamic, and putamen volumes are reported in male alcohol users (Nagel et al., 2005); however, larger thalamic and putamen volumes have been reported in adolescent female drinkers (Fein et al., 2013). In the present study, chronic heavy alcohol drinking attenuated age-related growth of the thalamus in the macaques (Table 5). Overall, the changes in the growth rate of the thalamus and white matter due to chronic alcohol drinking in the macaques have some key similarities to the human data, but the macaque also offers quantitative information related to ethanol dose. Further, the observation that a subset of structures shows an increased rate of volumetric growth suggests that additional studies are needed to understand the allopathic balance between responses of subcortical structures to chronic alcohol use in the late adolescent brain.

In summary, the NHP model of alcohol self-administration, in combination with longitudinal measures by MRI used in this study, highlights the rate of volume changes within the developing primate brain to isolate dose-dependent effects of chronic voluntary alcohol drinking in rhesus macaques. These effects are concentrated within cerebral white matter, and the thalamus is involved in critical control over sensory and limbic integration with behavioral choice and output. Thus, future research is needed to determine whether these volumetric changes might lead to altered functioning within the neural circuitry underlying excessive alcohol drinking.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This research was supported by Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health, and National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grants U01-AA-013510, P60-AA-010760, R24-AA-019431, P51-OD-011092, and U24-AA-025473.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, Gee JC (2008) Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med Image Anal 12:26–41. doi:10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Baker EJ, Farro J, Gonzales S, Helms C, Grant KA (2014) Chronic alcohol self-administration in monkeys shows long-term quantity/frequency categorical stability. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 38:2835–2843. doi:10.1111/acer.12547
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Baker EJ, Walter NA, Salo A, Rivas Perea P, Moore S, Gonzales S, Grant KA (2017) Identifying future drinkers: behavioral analysis of monkeys initiating drinking to intoxication is predictive of future drinking classification. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 41:626–636. doi:10.1111/acer.13327
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Bakken TE, Miller JA, Ding SL, Sunkin SM, Smith KA, Ng L, Szafer A, Dalley RA, Royall JJ, Lemon T, Shapouri S, Aiona K, Arnold J, Bennett JL, Bertagnolli D, Bickley K, Boe A, Brouner K, Butler S, Byrnes E, et al. (2016) A comprehensive transcriptional map of primate brain development. Nature 535:367–375. doi:10.1038/nature18637
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Barnea-Goraly N, Frazier TW, Piacenza L, Minshew NJ, Keshavan MS, Reiss AL, Hardan AY (2014) A preliminary longitudinal volumetric MRI study of amygdala and hippocampal volumes in autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 48:124–128. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.09.010
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    Bernard JA, Leopold DR, Calhoun VD, Mittal VA (2015) Regional cerebellar volume and cognitive function from adolescence to late middle age. Hum Brain Mapp 36:1102–1120. doi:10.1002/hbm.22690 pmid:25395058
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Bertol E, Vaiano F, Boscolo-Berto R, Fioravanti A, Palumbo D, Catalani V, Mari F, Patussi V, Serpelloni G (2017) Alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine consumption in adolescents: hair analysis versus self-report. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 43:341–349. doi:10.1080/00952990.2016.1216556
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    Brain Development Cooperative Group (2012) Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based normative sample from 4 to 18 years: the NIH MRI study of normal brain development. Cereb Cortex 22:1–12.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Cervera-Juanes R, Wilhelm LJ, Park B, Grant KA, Ferguson B (2017) Alcohol-dose-dependent DNA methylation and expression in the nucleus accumbens identifies coordinated regulation of synaptic genes. Transl Psychiatry 7:e994. doi:10.1038/tp.2016.266
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    Chandler CM, Follett ME, Porter NJ, Liang KY, Vallender EJ, Miller GM, Rowlett JK, Platt DM (2017) Persistent negative effects of alcohol drinking on aspects of novelty-directed behavior in male rhesus macaques. Alcohol 63:19–26. doi:10.1016/j.alcohol.2017.03.002
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    Chanraud S, Martelli C, Delain F, Kostogianni N, Douaud G, Aubin HJ, Reynaud M, Martinot JL (2007) Brain morphometry and cognitive performance in detoxified alcohol-dependents with preserved psychosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacology 32:429–438. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301219
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Chung DW, Wills ZP, Fish KN, Lewis DA (2017) Developmental pruning of excitatory synaptic inputs to parvalbumin interneurons in monkey prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:E629–E637. doi:10.1073/pnas.1610077114
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    Clark CB, Zyambo CM, Li Y, Cropsey KL (2016) The impact of non-concordant self-report of substance use in clinical trials research. Addict Behav 58:74–79. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.02.023 pmid:26921721
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Dennison M, Whittle S, Yücel M, Vijayakumar N, Kline A, Simmons J, Allen NB (2013) Mapping subcortical brain maturation during adolescence: evidence of hemisphere- and sex-specific longitudinal changes. Dev Sci 16:772–791. doi:10.1111/desc.12057
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Eckenhoff MF, Rakic P (1991) A quantitative analysis of synaptogenesis in the molecular layer of the dentate gyrus in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 64:129–135. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(91)90216-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Englund MM, Egeland B, Oliva EM, Collins WA (2008) Childhood and adolescent predictors of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in early adulthood: a longitudinal developmental analysis. Addiction 103 [Suppl. 1]:23–35. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02174.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Fein G, Greenstein D, Cardenas VA, Cuzen NL, Fouche JP, Ferrett H, Thomas K, Stein DJ (2013) Cortical and subcortical volumes in adolescents with alcohol dependence but without substance or psychiatric comorbidities. Psychiatry Res 214:1–8. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.06.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL (1999) Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci 2:861–863. doi:10.1038/13158
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Grant KA, Bennett AJ (2003) Advances in nonhuman primate alcohol abuse and alcoholism research. Pharmacol Ther 100:235–255. doi:10.1016/j.pharmthera.2003.08.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Grant KA, Leng X, Green HL, Szeliga KT, Rogers LS, Gonzales SW (2008) Drinking typography established by scheduled induction predicts chronic heavy drinking in a monkey model of ethanol self-administration. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 32:1824–1838. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00765.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Helms CM, Rau A, Shaw J, Stull C, Gonzales SW, Grant KA (2014) The effects of age at the onset of drinking to intoxication and chronic ethanol self-administration in male rhesus macaques. Psychopharmacology 231:1853–1861. doi:10.1007/s00213-013-3417-x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    Knickmeyer RC, Styner M, Short SJ, Lubach GR, Kang C, Hamer R, Coe CL, Gilmore JH (2010) Maturational trajectories of cortical brain development through the pubertal transition: unique species and sex differences in the monkey revealed through structural magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 20:1053–1063. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp166
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Kroenke CD, Rohlfing T, Park B, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A, Grant KA (2014) Monkeys that voluntarily and chronically drink alcohol damage their brains: a longitudinal MRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 39:823–830. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.259
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    Lebel C, Walker L, Leemans A, Phillips L, Beaulieu C (2008) Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood. Neuroimage 40:1044–1055. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.053
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Leonard CM, Towler S, Welcome S, Halderman LK, Otto R, Eckert MA, Chiarello C (2008) Size matters: cerebral volume influences sex differences in neuroanatomy. Cereb Cortex 18:2920–2931. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn052
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Levman J, MacDonald P, Lim AR, Forgeron C, Takahashi E (2017) A pediatric structural MRI analysis of healthy brain development from newborns to young adults. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5931–5942. doi:10.1002/hbm.23799
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  27. ↵
    Luciana M, Collins PF, Muetzel RL, Lim KO (2013) Effects of alcohol use initiation on brain structure in typically developing adolescents. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 39:345–355. doi:10.3109/00952990.2013.837057
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Maimaris W, McCambridge J (2014) Age of first drinking and adult alcohol problems: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 68:268–274. doi:10.1136/jech-2013-203402
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    Malkova L, Heuer E, Saunders RC (2006) Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of rhesus monkey brain development. Eur J Neurosci 24:3204–3212. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05175.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Mengler L, Khmelinskii A, Diedenhofen M, Po C, Staring M, Lelieveldt BP, Hoehn M (2014) Brain maturation of the adolescent rat cortex and striatum: changes in volume and myelination. Neuroimage 84:35–44. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.034
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Miller DJ, Duka T, Stimpson CD, Schapiro SJ, Baze WB, McArthur MJ, Fobbs AJ, Sousa AM, Sestan N, Wildman DE, Lipovich L, Kuzawa CW, Hof PR, Sherwood CC (2012) Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:16480–16485. doi:10.1073/pnas.1117943109
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    Morean ME, Corbin WR, Fromme K (2012) Age of first use and delay to first intoxication in relation to trajectories of heavy drinking and alcohol-related problems during emerging adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 36:1991–1999. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01812.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Nagel BJ, Schweinsburg AD, Phan V, Tapert SF (2005) Reduced hippocampal volume among adolescents with alcohol use disorders without psychiatric comorbidity. Psychiatry Res 139:181–190. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.05.008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Narvacan K, Treit S, Camicioli R, Martin W, Beaulieu C (2017) Evolution of deep gray matter volume across the human lifespan. Hum Brain Mapp 38:3771–3790. doi:10.1002/hbm.23604 pmid:28548250
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    National Research Council (2011) Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals, Ed 8. Washington, DC: National Academies.
  36. ↵
    Patrick ME, Terry-McElrath YM (2017) High-intensity drinking by underage young adults in the United States. Addiction 112:82–93. doi:10.1111/add.13556
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Rawles JM, Zipursky RB, Lim KO (1994) A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol 51:874–887. doi:10.1001/archneur.1994.00540210046012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Pfefferbaum A, Adalsteinsson E, Sood R, Mayer D, Bell R, McBride W, Li TK, Sullivan EV (2006) Longitudinal brain magnetic resonance imaging study of the alcohol-preferring rat. Part II: effects of voluntary chronic alcohol consumption. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 30:1248–1261. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00146.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Pfefferbaum A, Rohlfing T, Pohl KM, Lane B, Chu W, Kwon D, Nolan Nichols B, Brown SA, Tapert SF, Cummins K, Thompson WK, Brumback T, Meloy MJ, Jernigan TL, Dale A, Colrain IM, Baker FC, Prouty D, De Bellis MD, Voyvodic JT, et al. (2016) Adolescent development of cortical and white matter structure in the NCANDA sample: role of sex, ethnicity, puberty, and alcohol drinking. Cereb Cortex 26:4101–4121. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv205
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Pfefferbaum A, Kwon D, Brumback T, Thompson WK, Cummins K, Tapert SF, Brown SA, Colrain IM, Baker FC, Prouty D, De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Nagel BJ, Chu W, Park SH, Pohl KM, Sullivan EV (2018) Altered brain developmental trajectories in adolescents after initiating drinking. Am J Psychiatry 175:370–380. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17040469
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Poikolainen K, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Aalto-Setälä T, Marttunen M, Lönnqvist J (2001) Predictors of alcohol intake and heavy drinking in early adulthood: a 5-year follow-up of 15-19-year-old Finnish adolescents. Alcohol Alcohol 36:85–88. doi:10.1093/alcalc/36.1.85
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Rakic P (1995) A small step for the cell, a giant leap for mankind: a hypothesis of neocortical expansion during evolution. Trends Neurosci 18:383–388. pmid:7482803
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Rilling JK, Insel TR (1999) The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging. J Hum Evol 37:191–223. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0313
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Rohlfing T, Kroenke CD, Sullivan EV, Dubach MF, Bowden DM, Grant KA, Pfefferbaum A (2012) The INIA19 template and NeuroMaps atlas for primate brain image parcellation and spatial normalization. Front Neuroinform 6:27. doi:10.3389/fninf.2012.00027 pmid:PMC3515865/
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Scott JA, Grayson D, Fletcher E, Lee A, Bauman MD, Schumann CM, Buonocore MH, Amaral DG (2016) Longitudinal analysis of the developing rhesus monkey brain using magnetic resonance imaging: birth to adulthood. Brain Struct Funct 221:2847–2871. doi:10.1007/s00429-015-1076-x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    Shnitko TA, Allen DC, Gonzales SW, Walter NA, Grant KA (2017) Ranking cognitive flexibility in a group setting of rhesus monkeys with a set-shifting procedure. Front Behav Neurosci 11:55.
    OpenUrl
  47. ↵
    Shnitko TA, Gonzales SW, Grant KA (2019) Low cognitive flexibility as a risk for heavy alcohol drinking in non-human primates. Alcohol 74:95–104.
    OpenUrl
  48. ↵
    Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Tessner KD, Toga AW (2001) Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. J Neurosci 21:8819–8829. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-22-08819.2001
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW (2003) Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci 6:309–315. doi:10.1038/nn1008 pmid:12548289
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Toga AW (2004) Mapping changes in the human cortex throughout the span of life. Neuroscientist 10:372–392. doi:10.1177/1073858404263960 pmid:15271264
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Squeglia LM, Rinker DA, Bartsch H, Castro N, Chung Y, Dale AM, Jernigan TL, Tapert SF (2014) Brain volume reductions in adolescent heavy drinkers. Dev Cogn Neurosci 9:117–125. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2014.02.005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Squeglia LM, Tapert SF, Sullivan EV, Jacobus J, Meloy MJ, Rohlfing T, Pfefferbaum A (2015) Brain development in heavy-drinking adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 172:531–542. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101249 pmid:25982660
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2017) Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: results from the 2016 national survey on drug use and health. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.
  54. ↵
    Sullivan EV, Deshmukh A, Desmond JE, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A (2000) Cerebellar volume decline in normal aging, alcoholism, and Korsakoff's syndrome: relation to ataxia. Neuropsychology 14:341–352. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.14.3.341
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A, Rohlfing T, Baker FC, Padilla ML, Colrain IM (2011) Developmental change in regional brain structure over 7 months in early adolescence: comparison of approaches for longitudinal atlas-based parcellation. Neuroimage 57:214–224. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Thomas SG, Czoty PW (2019) Effect of menstrual cycle on ethanol drinking in rhesus monkeys. Drug Alcohol Depend 194:205–209. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.11.001 pmid:30453105
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Toro R, Perron M, Pike B, Richer L, Veillette S, Pausova Z, Paus T (2008) Brain size and folding of the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 18:2352–2357. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm261
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Tustison NJ, Avants BB, Cook PA, Zheng Y, Egan A, Yushkevich PA, Gee JC (2010) N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 29:1310–1320. doi:10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908 pmid:20378467
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Uematsu A, Hata J, Komaki Y, Seki F, Yamada C, Okahara N, Kurotaki Y, Sasaki E, Okano H (2017) Mapping orbitofrontal-limbic maturation in non-human primates: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 163:55–67. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.028
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  60. ↵
    Vetreno RP, Yaxley R, Paniagua B, Johnson GA, Crews FT (2017) Adult rat cortical thickness changes across age and following adolescent intermittent ethanol treatment. Addict Biol 22:712–723. doi:10.1111/adb.12364
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    Vivian JA, Green HL, Young JE, Majerksy LS, Thomas BW, Shively CA, Tobin JR, Nader MA, Grant KA (2001) Induction and maintenance of ethanol self-administration in cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): long-term characterization of sex and individual differences. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25:1087–1097. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2001.tb02321.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Wright MJ Jr., Taffe MA (2014) Chronic periadolescent alcohol consumption produces persistent cognitive deficits in rhesus macaques. Neuropharmacology 86:78–87. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.07.003
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  63. ↵
    Yang X, Tian F, Zhang H, Zeng J, Chen T, Wang S, Jia Z, Gong Q (2016) Cortical and subcortical gray matter shrinkage in alcohol-use disorders: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 66:92–103. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.034 pmid:27108216
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Yeatman JD, Wandell BA, Mezer AA (2014) Lifespan maturation and degeneration of human brain white matter. Nat Commun 5:4932. doi:10.1038/ncomms5932 pmid:25230200
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Zahr NM, Pfefferbaum A (2017) Alcohol's effects on the brain: neuroimaging results in humans and animal models. Alcohol Res 38:183–206.
    OpenUrl
  66. ↵
    Zecevic N, Rakic P (1991) Synaptogenesis in monkey somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex 1:510–523. pmid:1822755
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Philippe Tobler, University of Zurich

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. Note: If this manuscript was transferred from JNeurosci and a decision was made to accept the manuscript without peer review, a brief statement to this effect will instead be what is listed below.

Dear Dr Shnitko, dear Dr. Kroenke,

Thank you for the revision of the JNS version of the article. While the revision was responsive, I noticed a couple of points that should be addressed before going ahead.

1) Please report whether the findings remain the same if you include a regressor of no interest for scanner upgrade

2) ROI-analysis: Please describe the rationale for including the 10 regions as ROI (around line 191) and describe how you accounted for the multiple comparison issue (i.e., correcting for the number of ROIs)

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 6 (2)
eNeuro
Vol. 6, Issue 2
March/April 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Chronic Alcohol Drinking Slows Brain Development in Adolescent and Young Adult Nonhuman Primates
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Chronic Alcohol Drinking Slows Brain Development in Adolescent and Young Adult Nonhuman Primates
Tatiana A. Shnitko, Zheng Liu, Xiaojie Wang, Kathleen A. Grant, Christopher D. Kroenke
eNeuro 1 April 2019, 6 (2) ENEURO.0044-19.2019; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0044-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Chronic Alcohol Drinking Slows Brain Development in Adolescent and Young Adult Nonhuman Primates
Tatiana A. Shnitko, Zheng Liu, Xiaojie Wang, Kathleen A. Grant, Christopher D. Kroenke
eNeuro 1 April 2019, 6 (2) ENEURO.0044-19.2019; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0044-19.2019
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • underage drinking
  • brain growth
  • magnetic resonance imaging
  • thalamus
  • white matter
  • ethanol
  • self administration

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

New Research

  • A Very Fast Time Scale of Human Motor Adaptation: Within Movement Adjustments of Internal Representations during Reaching
  • TrkB Signaling Influences Gene Expression in Cortistatin-Expressing Interneurons
  • Optogenetic Activation of β-Endorphin Terminals in the Medial Preoptic Nucleus Regulates Female Sexual Receptivity
Show more New Research

Disorders of the Nervous System

  • Gene variants related to primary familial brain calcification: perspectives from bibliometrics and meta-analysis
  • Expression of HDAC3-Y298H Point Mutant in Medial Habenula Cholinergic Neurons Has No Effect on Cocaine-Induced Behaviors
  • Parallel Gene Expression Changes in Ventral Midbrain Dopamine and GABA Neurons during Normal Aging
Show more Disorders of the Nervous System

Subjects

  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.