Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Commentary, Sensory and Motor Systems

Beyond Motor Noise: Considering Other Causes of Impaired Reinforcement Learning in Cerebellar Patients

Pierre Vassiliadis, Gerard Derosiere and Julie Duque
eNeuro 13 February 2019, 6 (1) ENEURO.0458-18.2019; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0458-18.2019
Pierre Vassiliadis
Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium 1200
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Pierre Vassiliadis
Gerard Derosiere
Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium 1200
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Gerard Derosiere
Julie Duque
Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium 1200
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Julie Duque
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading
  • basal ganglia
  • cerebellum
  • motor learning
  • motor noise
  • reinforcement learning

Significance Statement

Motor and reinforcement learning have been classically linked to functionally independent brain networks centered on the cerebellum and the basal ganglia, respectively. In a recent study published in eNeuro, Therrien et al. (2018) showed that increasing motor noise in healthy subjects disrupts reinforcement learning. However, this impairment remained well below that detected in cerebellar patients even when motor noise in healthy subjects was adjusted to match that observed in the patients. This suggests that impaired reinforcement learning following cerebellar damage cannot be solely accounted for by altered motor noise in these patients. Based on recent anatomic and functional evidence, we argue that the cerebellum may directly contribute to reinforcement learning, consistent with its tight connections with the basal ganglia.

The ability to adapt to changes occurring in the environment is a fundamental feature of human behavior, which relies on both sensory and reward feedbacks. On the one hand, the role of sensory feedback has been largely considered by studying how motor commands adapt to visual perturbations (e.g., a visuomotor rotation), a process called error-based learning (Shadmehr et al., 2010; Wolpert et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2018; Roemmich and Bastian, 2018). This type of motor learning involves the computation of sensory prediction errors (SPEs), namely, the difference between predicted and actual sensory outcome (Tseng et al., 2007; Schlerf and Ivry, 2012; Shadmehr, 2017, 2018). On the other hand, the role of reward feedback has been mostly investigated in tasks that require learning what action to select or not, by updating reward predictions based on previous experience, a process named reinforcement learning (Lee et al., 2012; Derosiere et al., 2017a,b; Gershman and Daw, 2017; O’Doherty et al., 2017). A central aspect here is the computation of reward prediction errors (RPEs), namely, the difference between predicted and actual rewards (Schultz, 2015).

For a long time, motor learning and reinforcement learning have been studied apart and have been linked to functionally independent brain networks (Doya, 2000), mostly centered on either the cerebellum (Tseng et al., 2007; Schlerf and Ivry, 2012; Taylor and Ivry, 2014; Herzfeld et al., 2018) or on dopaminergic-basal ganglia circuits (Lee et al., 2012; O’Doherty et al., 2017), respectively. However, this view has changed in the past few years, with recent works indicating that rewards can strongly impact motor learning (Abe et al., 2011; Izawa and Shadmehr, 2011; Dayan et al., 2014; Galea et al., 2015; Nikooyan et al., 2015; Quattrocchi et al., 2017; Song and Smiley-Oyen, 2017). Hence, efforts are now made to understand how motor and reinforcement learning may interact at the neural level (Wilkinson et al., 2015; Mawase et al., 2017; Uehara et al., 2017). Consistently, Therrien et al. (2016) recently reported data pointing toward an implication of the cerebellum in reinforcement-based motor learning. As such, cerebellar patients exhibited a reduced ability to learn from reinforcement in a visuomotor adaptation task, compared to healthy subjects (Therrien et al., 2016). However, because the patients also exhibited increased motor noise (i.e., defined as an uncontrollable source of motor variability), this deficit could have occurred indirectly, due to an impaired ability to precisely relate an action to the received reward. The work reported by the same authors in eNeuro aimed at tackling this issue by increasing motor noise artificially in healthy individuals (Therrien et al., 2018).

Therrien et al. (2018) used the same visuomotor adaptation task as in their previous 2016 paper. Specifically, the healthy subjects were required to make reaching movements toward a visual target with no visual information on the position of their hand and received binary reward feedback. To get a reward, subjects had to learn to alter their reach angle to counteract a visuomotor rotation between the location of the visual target and their hand position: the reward feedback was based on this modified reach angle. Critically, the authors added motor noise by deviating the subjects’ reach in a way that was proportional to baseline motor variability. The experimental design involved both a low and a high noise condition, with the latter set to approximate the level of motor noise observed in cerebellar patients (Therrien et al., 2016). Hence, such approach allowed comparing reinforcement learning abilities between patients and healthy controls with comparable motor noise. The authors report an impaired reinforcement learning in healthy individuals in the high-noise compared to the control condition (i.e., when no noise was added). Yet, one critical result is that this impairment remained well below that observed in cerebellar patients. This finding indicates that motor noise does not entirely account for the reinforcement learning deficits observed following cerebellar damage.

A main line of argumentation in the paper focuses on the reduced proprioceptive acuity of cerebellar patients. As such, even with added motor noise, healthy subjects can still relate the rewards they receive to their reach angle, based on proprioception, while this ability is known to be altered in cerebellar patients (Miall and King, 2008; Bhanpuri et al., 2013; Weeks et al., 2017a,b). Hence, the reduction in proprioceptive precision might have indirectly altered reinforcement learning in the patients. Note though that the clinical tests run by Therrien et al. (2016) failed to reveal any reduction in proprioceptive precision in the patients. Hence, even if a discrete reduction in proprioceptive precision could have gone unnoticed based on clinical tests (Rinderknecht et al., 2018), we would like to propose that alterations in proprioceptive precision may not completely explain reinforcement learning deficits observed in the patients. Rather, cerebellar damage may directly alter reinforcement learning, as already suggested by others (Swain et al., 2011; McDougle et al., 2016; Miall and Galea, 2016).

Our viewpoint is supported by recent anatomic studies showing bidirectional connections between the cerebellum and dopaminergic-basal ganglia routes (Bostan et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2014; Bostan and Strick, 2018). Specifically, the dentate nucleus of the cerebellum sends disynaptic projections to the striatum (Hoshi et al., 2005), and to midbrain dopaminergic structures (Watabe-Uchida et al., 2012). Conversely, the cerebellar cortex receives disynaptic projections from the subthalamic nucleus (Bostan et al., 2010). Functionally, recent works in rodents provide evidence that reward expectation modulates the firing rate of cerebellar cells (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; Wagner et al., 2017; Heffley et al., 2018). In the same vein, neuroimaging studies in humans have reported activity related to RPEs in the cerebellum (O’Doherty, 2004; Ramnani et al., 2004; Seymour et al., 2004; Tanaka et al., 2004; Tobler et al., 2006; Garrison et al., 2013), suggesting that this structure is functionally involved in processing reward feedback. These works are in agreement with the result of a previous study showing that cerebellar patients exhibit altered reinforcement learning in a decision-making task requiring very simple movements (Thoma et al., 2008). In line with these considerations, structural and functional alterations of the cerebellum were found in individuals suffering from an addiction, such as alcohol or cocaine dependence, a condition characterized by abnormal reward processing (Moulton et al., 2014; Miquel et al., 2016; Moreno-Rius and Miquel, 2017). Furthermore, an important feature of reinforcement-based compared to error-based adaptation is that the former increases trial-to-trial movement variability, reflecting an exploration process of the environment (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2011; Taylor and Ivry, 2014; Dhawale and Smith, 2017). Following this idea, modeling work in the present study showed that healthy subjects increased motor exploration following unrewarded compared to rewarded trials. This effect was absent in the patients reflecting an inability to modulate behavior optimally according to reward feedback. In this view, a recent study showed that poor performance in a visuomotor adaptation task in cerebellar patients is not only due to impaired error-based learning but also to a difficulty in using feedback information to develop and maintain an explicit aiming strategy (Butcher et al., 2017). Hence, it seems that cerebellar dysfunction could have impaired the ability to learn both from error and reward feedbacks.

Nevertheless, an important point that needs to be raised here is the age difference between the healthy subjects tested in the commented paper (Therrien et al., 2018) and the cerebellar patients to which they are compared but that were originally tested in Therrien et al. (2016). As such, the healthy subjects (25.0 ± 4.8 years old) were much younger than the patients (61.5 ± 10.0 years old), and in fact, when the groups were matched for age in Therrien et al. (2016), the healthy (older) controls also exhibited impaired motor exploration, to a comparable extent as the patients. This suggests that aging could also have contributed to the reduced reinforcement learning abilities of the patients (Chowdhury et al., 2013). Further studies are therefore required to examine the respective contribution of aging and cerebellar dysfunction to reinforcement learning.

In conclusion, the work by Therrien and colleagues provides new insights into the influence of motor noise on reinforcement learning in healthy subjects and in patients suffering from cerebellar impairment. Moreover, the data are also consistent with the view that the cerebellum may be directly involved in reinforcement learning and more precisely in reinforcement-based motor learning. Future studies could directly test this hypothesis by relating the reinforcement learning impairment of patients to their score at the International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale, reflecting the severity of the cerebellar impairment. This line of research opens very interesting perspectives to design innovative multi-approach neurorehabilitation strategies.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • P.V. was a PhD student supported by the Fonds Spéciaux de Recherche (FSR) of the Université Catholique de Louvain. G.D. was a post-doctoral fellow supported by the Belgian National Funds for Scientific Research (FNRS). J.D. was supported by grants from the FSR of the Université Catholique de Louvain, and the Belgian FNRS (MIS F.4512.14).

  • Commentary on “Increasing Motor Noise Impairs Reinforcement Learning in Healthy Individuals” by Therrien, Wolpert, and Bastian. eNeuro. 2018 Aug 13; 5(3).

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Abe M, Schambra H, Wassermann EM, Luckenbaugh D, Schweighofer N, Cohen LG (2011) Reward improves long-term retention of a motor memory through induction of offline memory gains. Curr Biol 21:557–562. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.02.030 pmid:21419628
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Bhanpuri NH, Okamura AM, Bastian AJ (2013) Predictive modeling by the cerebellum improves proprioception. J Neurosci 33:14301–14306. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0784-13.2013 pmid:24005283
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Bostan AC, Dum RP, Strick PL (2010) The basal ganglia communicate with the cerebellum. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:8452–8456. doi:10.1073/pnas.1000496107 pmid:20404184
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Bostan AC, Strick PL (2018) The basal ganglia and the cerebellum: nodes in an integrated network. Nat Rev Neurosci 19:338–350. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0002-7 pmid:29643480
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Butcher PA, Ivry RB, Kuo S, Rydz D, Krakauer JW, Taylor JA (2017) The cerebellum does more than sensory prediction error-based learning in sensorimotor adaptation tasks. J Neurophysiol 118:1622–1636. doi:10.1152/jn.00451.2017 pmid:28637818
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Chen CH, Fremont R, Arteaga-Bracho EE, Khodakhah K (2014) Short latency cerebellar modulation of the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci 17:1767–1775. doi:10.1038/nn.3868 pmid:25402853
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Chowdhury R, Guitart-Masip M, Lambert C, Dayan P, Huys Q, Düzel E, Dolan RJ (2013) Dopamine restores reward prediction errors in old age. Nat Neurosci 16:648–653. doi:10.1038/nn.3364 pmid:23525044
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Dayan E, Averbeck BB, Richmond BJ, Cohen LG (2014) Stochastic reinforcement benefits skill acquisition. Learn Mem 21:140–142. doi:10.1101/lm.032417.113 pmid:24532838
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Derosiere G, Vassiliadis P, Demaret S, Zénon A, Duque J (2017a) Learning stage-dependent effect of M1 disruption on value-based motor decisions. Neuroimage 162:173–185. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.075
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Derosiere G, Zénon A, Alamia A, Duque J (2017b) Primary motor cortex contributes to the implementation of implicit value-based rules during motor decisions. Neuroimage 146:1115–1127. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.010 pmid:27742597
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Dhawale AK, Smith MA (2017) The role of variability in motor learning. Annu Rev Neurosci 40:479–498. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031548 pmid:28489490
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Doya K (2000) Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 10:732–739. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00153-7 pmid:11240282
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Galea JM, Mallia E, Rothwell J, Diedrichsen J (2015) The dissociable effects of punishment and reward on motor learning. Nat Neurosci 18:597–602. doi:10.1038/nn.3956 pmid:25706473
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Garrison J, Erdeniz B, Done J (2013) Prediction error in reinforcement learning: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37:1297–1310. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.023 pmid:23567522
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Gershman SJ, Daw ND (2017) Reinforcement learning and episodic memory in humans and animals: an integrative framework. Annu Rev Psychol 68:101–128. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033625 pmid:27618944
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Heffley W, Song EY, Xu Z, Taylor BN, Hughes MA, McKinney A, Joshua M, Hull C (2018) Coordinated cerebellar climbing fiber activity signals learned sensorimotor predictions. Nat Neurosci 21:1431–1441. doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0228-8 pmid:30224805
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Herzfeld DJ, Kojima Y, Soetedjo R, Shadmehr R (2018) Encoding of error and learning to correct that error by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci 21:736–743. doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0136-y pmid:29662213
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Hoshi E, Tremblay L, Féger J, Carras PL, Strick PL (2005) The cerebellum communicates with the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci 8:1491–1493. doi:10.1038/nn1544 pmid:16205719
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Izawa J, Shadmehr R (2011) Learning from sensory and reward prediction errors during motor adaptation. PLoS Comput Biol 7:e1002012. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002012 pmid:21423711
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Kim HE, Morehead JR, Parvin DE, Moazzezi R, Ivry RB (2018) Invariant errors reveal limitations in motor correction rather than constraints on error sensitivity. Commun Biol 1:19. doi:10.1101/189597 pmid:30271906
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    Lee D, Seo H, Jung M (2012) Neural basis of reinforcement learning and decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:287–308. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150512 pmid:22462543
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Mawase F, Uehara S, Bastian AJ, Celnik P (2017) Motor learning enhances use-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 37:2673–2685. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3303-16.2017 pmid:8143961
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    McDougle SD, Boggess MJ, Crossley MJ, Parvin D, Ivry RB, Taylor JA (2016) Credit assignment in movement-dependent reinforcement learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:6797–6802. doi:10.1073/pnas.1523669113 pmid:27247404
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    Miall RC, Galea J (2016) Cerebellar damage limits reinforcement learning. Brain 139:4–7. doi:10.1093/brain/awv343 pmid:26747853
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Miall RC, King D (2008) State estimation in the cerebellum. Cerebellum 7:572–576. doi:10.1007/s12311-008-0072-6 pmid:18855092
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Miquel M, Vazquez-Sanroman D, Carbo-Gas M, Gil-Miravet I, Sanchis-Segura C, Carulli D, Manzo J, Coria-Avila GA (2016) Have we been ignoring the elephant in the room? Seven arguments for considering the cerebellum as part of addiction circuitry. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 60:1–11. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.005 pmid:26602022
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Moreno-Rius J, Miquel M (2017) The cerebellum in drug craving. Drug Alcohol Depend 173:151–158. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.12.028 pmid:28259088
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Moulton EA, Elman I, Becerra LR, Goldstein RZ, Borsook D (2014) The cerebellum and addiction: insights gained from neuroimaging research. Addict Biol 19:317–331. doi:10.1111/adb.12101 pmid:24851284
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Nikooyan AA, Ahmed AA, Nikooyan AA, Ahmed AA (2015) Reward feedback accelerates motor learning Reward feedback accelerates motor learning. J Neurophysiol 113:633–646. doi:10.1152/jn.00032.2014 pmid:25355957
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    O’Doherty JP (2004) Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:769–776. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.016 pmid:15582382
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    O’Doherty JP, Cockburn J, Pauli WM (2017) Learning, reward, and decision making. Annu Rev Psychol 68:73–100. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044216 pmid:27687119
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Ohmae S, Medina JF (2015) Climbing fibers encode a temporal-difference prediction error during cerebellar learning in mice. Nat Neurosci 18:1798–1803. doi:10.1038/nn.4167 pmid:26551541
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Quattrocchi G, Greenwood R, Rothwell JC, Galea JM, Bestmann S (2017) Reward and punishment enhance motor adaptation in stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 88:730–736. doi:10.1136/jnnp-2016-314728 pmid:28377451
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Ramnani N, Elliott R, Athwal BS, Passingham RE (2004) Prediction error for free monetary reward in the human prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 23:777–786. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.028 pmid:15528079
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Rinderknecht MD, Lambercy O, Raible V, Büsching I, Sehle A, Liepert J, Gassert R (2018) Reliability, validity, and clinical feasibility of a rapid and objective assessment of post-stroke deficits in hand proprioception. J Neuroeng Rehabil 15:47. doi:10.1186/s12984-018-0387-6 pmid:29880003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Roemmich RT, Bastian AJ (2018) Closing the loop: from motor neuroscience to neurorehabilitation. Annu Rev Neurosci 41:415–429. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-062245 pmid:29709206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Schlerf J, Ivry RB (2012) Encoding of sensory prediction errors in the human cerebellum. J Neurosci 32:4913–4922. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4504-11.2012 pmid:22492047
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    Schultz W (2015) Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data. Physiol Rev 95:853–951. doi:10.1152/physrev.00023.2014 pmid:26109341
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Seymour B, O’Doherty JP, Dayan P, Koltzenburg M, Jones AK, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS (2004) Temporal difference models describe higher-order learning in humans. Nature 429:664–667. doi:10.1038/nature02581 pmid:15190354
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Shadmehr R (2017) Learning to predict and control the physics of our movements. J Neurosci 37:1663–1671. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1675-16.2016 pmid:28202784
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Shadmehr R (2018) Motor learning: a cortical system for adaptive motor control. Curr Biol 28:R793–R795. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.071 pmid:30040941
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW (2010) Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:89–108. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153135 pmid:20367317
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Song Y, Smiley-Oyen AL (2017) Probability differently modulating the effects of reward and punishment on visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 235:3605–3618. doi:10.1007/s00221-017-5082-5 pmid:28887626
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Swain RA, Kerr AL, Thompson RF (2011) The cerebellum: a neural system for the study of reinforcement learning. Front Behav Neurosci 5:8. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00008 pmid:21427778
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Tanaka SC, Doya K, Okada G, Ueda K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S (2004) Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nat Neurosci 7:887–893. doi:10.1038/nn1279 pmid:15235607
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Taylor JA, Ivry RB (2014) Cerebellar and prefrontal cortex contributions to adaptation, strategies, and reinforcement learning. Prog Brain Res 210:217–253. doi:10.1016/b978-0-444-63356-9.00009-1 pmid:24916295
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Therrien AS, Wolpert DM, Bastian AJ (2016) Effective reinforcement learning following cerebellar damage requires a balance between exploration and motor noise. Brain 139:101–114. doi:10.1093/brain/awv329 pmid:26626368
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Therrien AS, Wolpert DM, Bastian AJ (2018) Increasing motor noise impairs reinforcement learning in healthy individuals. eNeuro 5: ENEURO.0050-18.2018. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0050-18.2018 pmid:30105298
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    Thoma P, Bellebaum C, Koch B, Schwarz M, Daum I (2008) The cerebellum is involved in reward-based reversal learning. Cerebellum 7:433–443. doi:10.1007/s12311-008-0046-8 pmid:18592331
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Tobler PN, Doherty JPO, Dolan RJ, Schultz W, Philippe N, Doherty JPO, Dolan RJ (2006) Human neural learning depends on reward prediction errors in the blocking paradigm. J Neurophysiol 95:301–310. doi:10.1152/jn.00762.2005 pmid:16192329
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Tseng Y, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ (2007) Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J Neurophysiol 98:54–62. doi:10.1152/jn.00266.2007 pmid:17507504
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Uehara S, Mawase F, Celnik P (2017) Learning similar actions by reinforcement or sensory-prediction errors rely on distinct physiological mechanisms. Cereb Cortex 28:3478–3490. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx214 pmid:28968827
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Wagner MJ, Kim TH, Savall J, Schnitzer MJ, Luo L (2017) Cerebellar granule cells encode the expectation of reward. Nature 544:96–100. doi:10.1038/nature21726 pmid:28321129
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Watabe-Uchida M, Zhu L, Ogawa SK, Vamanrao A, Uchida N (2012) Whole-brain mapping of direct inputs to midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuron 74:858–873. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.017 pmid:22681690
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Weeks HM, Therrien AS, Bastian AJ (2017a) Proprioceptive localization deficits in people with cerebellar damage. Cerebellum 16:427–437. doi:10.1007/s12311-016-0819-4 pmid:27538404
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Weeks HM, Therrien AS, Bastian AJ (2017b) The cerebellum contributes to proprioception during motion. J Neurophysiol 118:693–702. doi:10.1152/jn.00417.2016 pmid:28404825
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Wilkinson L, Steel A, Mooshagian E, Zimmermann T, Keisler A, Lewis JD, Wassermann EM (2015) Online feedback enhances early consolidation of motor sequence learning and reverses recall deficit from transcranial stimulation of motor cortex. Cortex 71:134–147. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.012 pmid:26204232
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011) Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751. doi:10.1038/nrn3112 pmid:22033537
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Christian Hansel, University of Chicago

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: NONE.

In the current manuscript commenting the paper by Therrien et al., on eNeuro (2018 Aug 13; 5(3)) the authors argue that the cerebellum may directly contribute to reinforcement learning. This is an interesting comment that adds value to the paper by Therrien et al. and connects it to anatomical studies showing connections of the cerebellum with areas relevant for reinforcement learning, and functional data on cerebellar firing rate modulation in response to reward expectation.

In this Commentary article on the recently accepted manuscript by Therrien et al., on eNeuro (2018 Aug 13; 5(3)) the authors argue that the cerebellum may directly contribute to reinforcement learning. The current and previous work by Therrien et al. showed that cerebellar patients exhibit a reduced reinforcement learning in a visuomotor adaptation task in association with increased motor noise but, artificially increased motor noise at high-levels in healthy subjects (similar to the level observed in cerebellar patients), caused an impairment in reinforcement learning that remained well below that observed in cerebellar patients. The authors of the manuscript, therefore, argue that motor noise does not entirely account for the deficits in reinforcement learning observed in cerebellar patients suggesting (as already suggested by others) that cerebellar damage may directly alter reinforcement learning. This is supported by previous anatomical studies showing connections of the cerebellum with areas relevant for reinforcement learning, and functional data on cerebellar firing rate modulation in response to reward expectation.

The commentary is well-written and sound in its argumentation. No major revisions are suggested.
It might be relevant to briefly comment on the possible implications for addiction or on possible studies relating alterations in addiction behavior and cerebellar function.

Some additional critical notes on possible weaknesses on the commented paper by Therrien et al. may also be beneficial. For instance, the authors might consider if it is relevant to mention that groups of healthy subjects were apparently not aged-matched with cerebellar patients or that inter-subject variability of ataxia ICARS scores was not considered as a possible factor to be related to the different levels of impairment in reinforcement learning.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 6 (1)
eNeuro
Vol. 6, Issue 1
January/February 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Beyond Motor Noise: Considering Other Causes of Impaired Reinforcement Learning in Cerebellar Patients
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Beyond Motor Noise: Considering Other Causes of Impaired Reinforcement Learning in Cerebellar Patients
Pierre Vassiliadis, Gerard Derosiere, Julie Duque
eNeuro 13 February 2019, 6 (1) ENEURO.0458-18.2019; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0458-18.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Beyond Motor Noise: Considering Other Causes of Impaired Reinforcement Learning in Cerebellar Patients
Pierre Vassiliadis, Gerard Derosiere, Julie Duque
eNeuro 13 February 2019, 6 (1) ENEURO.0458-18.2019; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0458-18.2019
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Significance Statement
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • basal ganglia
  • cerebellum
  • motor learning
  • motor noise
  • reinforcement learning

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Commentary

  • Some Tips for Writing Science
  • COVID-19 Deterred Career Path of Our Undergraduate Neuroscience Students: Educators’ Perspective
  • Remembering Hirsh Cohen and His Role in Developing Computational Neuroscience
Show more Commentary

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • The nasal solitary chemosensory cell signaling pathway triggers mouse avoidance behavior to inhaled nebulized irritants
  • Different control strategies drive interlimb differences in performance and adaptation during reaching movements in novel dynamics
  • Taste-odor association learning alters the dynamics of intra-oral odor responses in the posterior piriform cortex of awake rats
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Commentaries

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.