Abstract
In awake mice, sniffing behavior is subject to complex contextual modulation. It has been hypothesized that variance in inhalation dynamics alters odor concentration profiles in the naris despite a constant environmental concentration. Using whole-cell recordings in the olfactory bulb of awake mice, we directly demonstrate that rapid sniffing mimics the effect of odor concentration increase at the level of both mitral and tufted cell (MTC) firing rate responses and temporal responses. Paradoxically, we find that mice are capable of discriminating fine concentration differences within short timescales despite highly variable sniffing behavior. One way that the olfactory system could differentiate between a change in sniffing and a change in concentration would be to receive information about the inhalation parameters in parallel with information about the odor. We find that the sniff-driven activity of MTCs without odor input is informative of the kind of inhalation that just occurred, allowing rapid detection of a change in inhalation. Thus, a possible reason for sniff modulation of the early olfactory system may be to directly inform downstream centers of nasal flow dynamics, so that an inference can be made about environmental concentration independent of sniff variance.
Significance Statement
One of the fundamental tasks of the brain is to represent the features of the environment in a stable way. In the olfactory system, it has been hypothesized that changing the way you sniff will alter the concentration of odor coming into the nasal passage, even when the environmental concentration has not changed. Here we show that indeed, the effect of faster sniffing on olfactory bulb responses is very similar to increasing odor concentration. Despite this, mice can easily tell the difference between a change in sniffing and a change in concentration in an olfactory task. To resolve this apparent discrepancy, we suggest and give evidence for ways in which olfactory bulb information about sniffing parameters may be utilized.
Introduction
For optimal perception, an organism must be able to distinguish between the sensory consequences of its own actions and externally generated stimuli in the environment (Crapse and Sommer, 2008). An example of this comes from controlled eye movements, such as saccades: these act to shift the visual scene on the retina. Such a pattern of motion across the retina could just as easily come from the world moving relative to the eye, and yet we maintain perception of a stable world (von Helmholtz, 1867). An olfactory problem of this nature is the stable encoding of odor intensity—the perceptual correlate of odor concentration (Wojcik and Sirotin, 2014). Increasing concentration is known to affect neural activity in many ways (Mainland et al., 2014). At the level of glomerular input from olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs), increasing concentration enhances the activity of already responsive glomeruli and incorporates new glomeruli into the activity profile, overall resulting in a broadening of the spatial map of activity (Rubin and Katz, 1999; Spors and Grinvald, 2002). Changes in spike rate are also seen at the level of the olfactory bulb (OB) output cells, mitral and tufted cells (MTCs), though this can be a more complex mixture of inhibitory and excitatory effects (Meredith, 1986; Bathellier et al., 2008; Cury and Uchida, 2010; Fukunaga et al., 2012) and is thought to be constrained via inhibitory circuits (Kato et al., 2013; Miyamichi et al., 2013; Fukunaga et al., 2014; Roland et al., 2016). The perhaps more ubiquitous correlates of concentration increase, however, are temporal response changes, notably with early excitation undergoing a latency reduction in OSNs (Rospars et al., 2000; Ghatpande and Reisert, 2011) and MTCs (Cang and Isaacson, 2003; Fukunaga et al., 2012; Sirotin et al., 2015), as well as in the piriform cortex (Bolding and Franks, 2017). This is thought to arise since OSNs will depolarize to threshold more quickly when the concentration profile in the naris is steeper.
In awake mice, sniffing behavior is in continual flux (Welker, 1964; Youngentob et al., 1987; Kepecs et al., 2007; Wesson et al., 2008a, 2009). This might present a problem for concentration coding: changing nasal flow will affect the number of odor molecules entering the nasal passage, altering the concentration profile in the naris despite a stable environmental concentration (Teghtsoonian et al., 1978; Mainland and Sobel, 2006; Shusterman et al., 2018). In other words, altering sniffing may cause self-generated changes in naris odor concentration. Indeed, previous work has shown that faster sniffing can alter firing rates and temporal features of an odor response (Wesson et al., 2009; Carey and Wachowiak, 2011; Shusterman et al., 2011; Cenier et al., 2013; Díaz-Quesada et al., 2018; Jordan et al., 2018). Despite this, previous work suggests that humans can perceive odor intensity independent of the inhalation flow rate (Teghtsoonian et al., 1978), and whether response changes during faster sniffing are similar for a given cell to those evoked by increased concentration is unknown.
Our aims were two-fold: (1) to test the hypothesis that response changes evoked by faster sniffing are the same as those caused by increasing concentration, and (2) to test whether sniff variance would have a negative impact on performance of mice in a fine concentration discrimination task. Using whole-cell patch recordings in awake mice, we show that faster sniffs can indeed evoke both firing rate and temporal response changes identical to those caused by increasing concentration. Surprisingly, however, we show that variance in sniffing has very little impact on the performance of mice during fine concentration discrimination. These results are highly congruent with an accompanying paper using different experimental techniques (Shusterman et al., 2018). Finally, we discuss how the olfactory system could make an inference about whether a response change was caused by concentration change or sniff change, showing that the olfactory bulb encodes sniff dynamics to allow rapid detection of a change in sniffing.
Materials and Methods
All animal experiments were approved by the local ethics panel of the [Francis Crick Institute]. All mice used were C57BL/6 Jax males aged between 5 and 12 weeks and were obtained by in-house breeding. All chemicals were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich.
Olfactometry
Odorants were delivered to the animal using a custom-made olfactometer. This consisted of eight different odor channels connecting two manifolds, a clean air channel, and a final dilution channel also carrying clean air. Air was pressure controlled at 1 bar with a pressure regulator (IR 1000, SMC Pneumatics). Flow was computer controlled via mass flow controllers to each manifold such that the olfactometer output provided a constant flow of 2 l/min at all times, meaning that no tactile stimulus accompanied odor pulses. Odor pulses were calibrated to square pulses of different concentrations using a mini photo-ionization detector (miniPID, Aurora Scientific): briefly, pure odor was presented to the PID from an open bottle, and the maximum recorded voltage (Vmax) was assumed to represent 100% saturated vapor pressure. The pulse amplitudes were then calibrated according to this value, such that a given concentration C (% saturated vapor pressure) could be specified by attaining a square pulse of amplitude equal to C · Vmax/100. Valves and flow controllers were controlled using custom-written LabView software. Odors applied to animals included 2 different odor mixtures (for recordings, either mixture A: methyl salicylate, eugenol, cinnamaldehyde, creosol, and 1-nonanol; or mixture B: guaiacol, valeric acid (+)-carvone, 2-phenyl ethanol, and 4-allylanisol). The components of each mixture were of similar vapor pressure, and proportions were adjusted according to relative vapor pressure values as in a previous study (Jordan et al., 2018). For behavior, either mixture A or pure vanillin odor was applied at various concentrations (Figs. 3 and 4).
Surgery
Sterile surgical technique was applied during all surgeries. For implantation of the head-plate, mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane in 95% oxygen (5% for induction, 1.5%–3% for maintenance). Local (mepivacaine, 0.5% s.c.) and general analgesics (carprofen 5 mg/kg s.c.) were applied immediately at the onset of surgery. An incision was made dorsally above the cranium overlying the cortex and cerebellum, and periosteal tissue was removed. The surface of the bone was drilled away across the implantation surface using a dental drill, and cyanoacrylate was applied to the sutures between the cranial bones to reduce movement. A stainless steel custom head-plate was then glued to the bone surface with cyanoacrylate, and dental cement was used to reinforce the bond. For mice going on to whole-cell recording, an additional recording chamber was constructed on the bone overlying the right olfactory bulb using dental cement. After surgery, the mice were allowed to recover for 48 h with access to wet diet.
Whole-cell recordings
On the day of recording, mice were again anaesthetized with isoflurane as above, and carprofen analgesic was injected (5 mg/kg s.c.). A 1-mm-diameter craniotomy was made overlying the right olfactory bulb, and the dura was removed. A layer of 4% low-melting-point agar was then applied to the surface of the bulb, ∼0.5–1 mm thick, to reduce brain movement. Cortex buffer (125 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 10 mM HEPES, 2 mM MgSO4, 2 mM CaCl2, 10 mM glucose) was used to fill the recording chamber. The animal would then be transferred to the recording rig, head-fixed above a treadmill, and allowed to wake from anesthesia for 20 min. Whole-cell recordings were then made blindly by descending a 5–7-MΩ borosilicate glass micropipette (Hilgenberg, pulled on a DMZ Universal puller, Zeitz Instruments) filled with intracellular solution (130 mM KMeSO4, 10 mM HEPES, 7 mM KCl, 2 mM ATP-Na, 2 mM ATP-Mg, 0.5 mM GTP, 0.05 mM EGTA, and in some cases 10 mM biocytin; pH adjusted to 7.4 with KOH, osmolarity = 280 mOsm) through the agar and 180 µm into the olfactory bulb with high pressure. Here pressure was reduced, and the micropipette advanced in steps of 2 µm until a substantial and sudden increase in resistance was observed, indicating proximity to a cell. Pressure was then dropped to zero or below, and a gigaohm seal was attained. Whole-cell configuration was then achieved, and the membrane voltage recording was made in current clamp mode. Identification of mitral and tufted cells was achieved using electrophysiological parameters: an input resistance <150 MΩ, a resting membrane potential between –60 and –40 mV, and an afterhyperpolarization (AHP) waveform conforming to MTC phenotype in an independent component analysis performed as detailed in previous studies (Kollo et al., 2014; Jordan et al., 2018).
Altogether, 14 cells were recorded in passive mice and presented with 2 different odor concentrations, as well as puff stimuli to evoke fast sniffing (Figs. 1 and 2). Some cells were presented with two different odor stimuli (two different mixtures), resulting in 20 cell-odor pairs in total. Concentrations were presented in a pseudorandom order, and puff stimuli occurred on a random subset of trials only for the low concentration. Puff stimuli were applied simultaneously with the odor stimuli with a gentle clean air stream to the flank. For some analyses, such as Figs. 2A,B, 2-1, 5, 5-1, and 5-2, data were supplemented with previously recorded cells from the passive mouse presented with the same odor mixtures at 1% vapor pressure (n = 6 and n = 38, respectively).
Extended Figure 1-1
Effect of puff stimulus on sniff behaviour. (A) Example nasal flow traces from one animal during a control trial (no puff stimulus accompanying 2 s odor stimulus) and a trial with a puff stimulus. Shaded area shows odor stimulus. Black ticks indicate inhalation onsets. (B) Plots to show average change in sniff frequency, mean inhalation duration, and first inhalation duration between five control (black) and five puff (orange) trials for all 20 cell-odor pairs. Download Extended Figure 1-1, TIF file.
Extended Figure 1-2
Relationships between different sniffing parameters. (A) Example nasal flow trace during an inter-trial interval (no odor), with sniffs colored according to their inhalation duration (blue to red = long to short duration). Black ticks show time of inhalation onset, orange plot shows peak inhalation slope for each inhalation aligned to the inhalation onset, and green plot shows inhalation duration for each inhalation. (B) Example correlation between inhalation duration and peak inhalation slope for 1988 sniffs in 1 animal (top), and histogram of correlation R values between inhalation duration and peak inhalation slope across 50 animals. Black bars indicate significant correlations. (C) As for B, but for correlation between sniff duration and inhalation duration. As expected from constraints on duty cycle during respiration, we found a biphasic relationship between sniff duration and inhalation duration, with a linear correlation for sniffs < 250 ms duration (magenta) and a plateau for sniffs > 250 ms duration (blue). This resulted in high R values for sniffs < 250 ms duration and low R values for sniffs > 250 ms duration. (D) As for B, but for the correlation between the previous sniff duration and the current inhalation duration. Download Extended Figure 1-2, TIF file.
Extended Figure 1-3
Changes in subthreshold response are more inhibitory for concentration increase than for fast sniffing. (A) Example average subthreshold response traces for low concentration, slow sniffing (black), high concentration, slow sniffing (green) and low concentration, fast sniffing (magenta), for two different cells, cell c (top) and cell d (bottom). Each trace is the average of 5 spike-subtracted trials. (B) Scatter plot to show average change in membrane potential response for the first 1 s of the odor stimulus for concentration increase (high conc.-low conc.) and sniff frequency change (fast sniffing-slow sniffing). (C) Cumulative histograms of membrane potential response change for concentration increase (green) and sniff frequency increase (magenta). P = 0.03, paired t-test. Download Extended Figure 1-3, TIF file.
Extended Figure 2-1
Additional data for sniff-induced temporal shifts in odor response. (A) Heatmaps of mean spike count for 13 cell-odor pairs showing early excitation in response to the odor presented, for both slow inhalation (top) and fast inhalation (middle). White dashed line indicates odor onset aligned to the first inhalation onset. Cell-odor pairs are sorted from short to long response onset latency (during slow inhalation). Bottom heatmap shows the difference between the two above (fast-slow). White solid and dotted line indicates onset latency of each cell-odor pair for slow inhalation. Blue line indicates onset latency for fast inhalation. (B) Histogram of onset latency changes (fast-slow) for all 13 cell-odor pairs. Errorbar shows mean and SD. (C) Scatter plot to show relationship between onset latency for slow inhalation, and the onset change between fast and slow inhalation (ΔOnset). (D) Correlation between response onset latency and peak spike count (analysed within 10 ms time bins) for early excitatory odor responses evoked by a slow sniff. Blue data comes from pTCs and red data comes from pMCs. Boxplots compare the two parameters for pTCs and pMCs. (E) Comparison of response onset latency change (fast-slow sniff) for pMCs and pTCs. (F) Above and below plots are for two different example cells. Left: plot to show first inhalation duration during odor stimulation sorted from shortest to longest for all trials for one cell. Right: heatmap of spike count for the cell during odor stimulation for trials sorted by first inhalation duration as in left plot. White dotted line indicates where odor is on (aligned to first inhalation onset). Download Extended Figure 2-1, TIF file.
Behavioral task and training
On day 0 (48 h after surgery), mice with head-plates implanted would begin water restriction. On day 1, mice were habituated to the experimenter and hand-fed 0.5 ml of highly diluted sweetened condensed milk with a Pasteur pipette. On day 2, mice were habituated to head-fixation: mice were head-fixed above a treadmill and allowed access to free reward on licking the reward port (licks were detected using an IR beam). On day 3, successfully habituated mice underwent operant conditioning with repeated presentations of CS+ concentration of the odor mixture until the mouse learned to lick in the 1 s after odor offset to receive the reward. On day 5, the CS– concentration was also presented alongside the CS+ concentration in a pseudorandom order, until the mice learned to refrain from licking to the CS–. Licking to the CS– would evoke an addition of 6 s to the intertrial interval. Five mice were trained with high-concentration stimuli as the CS+ (“high go”), and three mice were trained on the reverse contingency (“low go”). On days 6–8, mice would be presented with five different concentrations (three additional concentrations spanning the range between the previously two learned concentrations), and contingencies as depicted in Fig. 4A. On day 9, five mice went on to a final session: after observing criterion performance on the binary odor concentration task with the mixture as learned previously, the odor would switch to vanillin with the same contingency between concentrations.
Mice were carefully monitored to maintain their body weights >80% of their prerestriction weight and were ensured a minimum of 1 ml water per day regardless of performance. Any mouse exceeding this weight loss or showing signs of distress was immediately returned to water access.
Sniff measurement
Nasal flow was recorded by placing a flow sensor (FBAM200DU, Sensortechnics) externally in close proximity to the nostril contralateral to the side of whole-cell recording and sampled at 1 kHz. The position of the sensor was manually optimized at the start of each session such that all sniff cycles were captured with a high signal-to-noise ratio.
Data analysis
In all cases, 5%–95% confidence intervals were used to determine significance unless otherwise stated. In all figures, a single asterisk denotes p < 0.05, a double asterisk denotes p < 0.01, and a triple asterisk denotes p < 0.001. Means and error bars showing a single standard deviation either side are used in all cases where comparing normally distributed data of equal variance. Lilliefors tests were used to determine if a dataset was normally distributed. In the case of normal distributions, two-sided Student’s t tests were used for comparison of means and Bartlett tests used to compare variances, unless otherwise stated. If data were not normally distributed, or where two datasets were not of equal variance, ranksum tests were used to compare the medians, and Browne–Forsythe tests used to compare variance, unless otherwise stated. Boxplots are used to represent such data (data comparisons of unequal variance, or non–normally distributed data), where median is plotted as a line within a box formed from 25th (q1) and 75th (q3) percentile. Points are drawn as outliers if they are larger than q3 + 1.5 × (q3 – q1) or smaller than q1 – 1.5 × (q3 – q1). Superscript letters listed with p-values correspond to the statistical tests shown in Table 1.
Sniff parameters
Using the recording of nasal flow, different sniff parameters could be extracted. First, inhalation peaks were detected using Spike2 algorithms that mark each peak above a certain threshold voltage manually defined by the user, such that all inhalations were included and no false positives were present. Inhalation onset was defined as the nearest time point before inhalation peak at which the flow trace reached zero. Inhalation offset was similarly calculated as the first time point after inhalation peak where the flow trace reached zero. Inhalation duration was defined as the difference in time between inhalation onset and offset. Peak inhalation slopes were calculated by detecting the peak value of the differentiated flow waveform 50–0 ms before inhalation peak. Sniff duration was calculated as the time between subsequent inhalation onsets. Sniff frequency was calculated by taking the inverse of the mean sniff duration within the odor time period.
Spike rate responses and onsets
Note that when comparing response changes due to concentration and response changes due to sniff change, the same number of trials was used in both conditions.
Long timescale (Fig. 1): For each cell, mean spike count was calculated in 250-ms time bins for the full 2-s odor stimulus. These were then averaged across trials to generate PSTHs for low concentration and fast sniffing (five trials of lowest mean inhalation duration), low concentration and slow sniffing (five trials with highest mean inhalation duration), and high concentration and slow sniffing (five trials of highest mean inhalation duration). Values were quadrupled to estimate fining rate (FR) in Hz.
Short timescale (Fig. 2): For each cell, spike counts were calculated in 10-ms time bins for only the first 250 ms from odor onset (aligned to first inhalation after odor onset). These spike counts were then averaged across trials for low concentration and fast inhalation (>70th percentile peak inhalation slopes), low concentration and slow inhalation (<30th percentile inhalation slopes), and high concentration and slow inhalation (<30th percentil peak inhalation slopes). Onset for excitatory responses was defined at the point at which the mean spike count exceeded the mean +2 standard deviations (SDs) of the baseline spike rate in the 250 ms before odor onset, and remained there for at least 2 consecutive points.
Vm responses
To analyze subthreshold responses in absence of spiking activity, spikes and their AHPs were subtracted from the trace. This was done by first using the “wavemark” tool in Spike2 to detect spikes by thresholding and matching them to a generated spike waveform template. The length of this spike waveform template was manually adjusted for each cell according to its AHP length, but was usually around –4 ms to 20–30 ms relative to spike peak. A trace was then generated containing all detected spike waveforms connected by zero values, and this was subtracted from the original voltage trace.
Correlations between response changes due to sniffing and concentration change
For both long- and short-timescale mean FR responses, changes in FR response were calculated for sniff change (ΔS, fast minus slow sniffing, low-concentration odor) and concentration change (ΔC, high minus low concentration, slow sniffing). For all cell-odor pairs across the sample, a single regression was made between FR changes for sniff change and FR changes for concentration change in the corresponding time bins, generating an actual R 2 and p value (Figs. 1F and 2E). For shuffle controls, low-concentration trials were shuffled with respect to the sniff behavior on each trial, and the same analysis was repeated 100 times. To compare how strong the correlations were in a relevant way, the high-concentration trials were randomly separated into two halves, and a linear regression was made between the changes in FR for each half (relative to low-concentration trials) as above. This allowed us to compare R 2 values for correlations between FR changes due to concentration increase versus concentration increase (ΔC1 versus ΔC2, different trial subsets) and FR changes due to concentration increase versus faster sniffing (ΔC versus ΔS).
Euclidean distance analysis of concentration discriminability
In reference to Fig. 2G, Euclidean distance was taken across the population between mean spike counts for high concentration and low concentration (slow inhalation). This generated a measure of discriminability between concentrations when the inhalation was slow for both concentrations. To test how much of the discriminability was due to latency shift of excitation between low and high concentrations, responses that underwent a detectable latency shift had their spike count response to low concentration manually shifted earlier according to the latency shift occurring between high and low concentration. Euclidean distance was then recalculated between spike counts for high concentration and the latency-shifted spike counts at low concentration. Finally, Euclidean distances were calculated between spike counts for high concentration (slow inhalation) and low concentration (fast inhalation). Time for discrimination was calculated, if possible, as the point at which the Euclidean distance exceeded the mean + 2 SDs of the baseline Euclidean distance (250 ms before odor onset) for at least 2 consecutive 10-ms time bins.
Baseline activity correlations with inhalation duration
For each cell (n = 45), 1000–2000 sniffs were analyzed in absence of odor. Sniffs were categorized according to their inhalation duration, 35–45, 45–55, 55–65 ms and so forth. For each individual sniff, different parameters were calculated from the corresponding neural activity. Mean membrane potential was calculated from the subthreshold membrane potential occurring from 0 to 250 ms from inhalation onset. Peak membrane potential was designated as the maximum membrane potential within 30–250 ms after inhalation onset, and time of the peak membrane potential was determined as the time of this maximum membrane potential relative to inhalation onset. Spike counts were calculated by summing all action potentials occurring within the same time frame. To calculate the correlations for each parameter, each was averaged across all sniffs within the category, and regression analysis was used to generate an R and p value between the resulting average parameters and the corresponding inhalation duration (minimum of the category). For each cell, inhalation duration categories were excluded from the correlation if they contained <25 sniffs, and cells that had <5 valid categories were additionally excluded. For shuffle controls, inhalation duration was shuffled throughout the data, and the regression analysis was repeated 10 times per cell.
Euclidean distance analysis of detectability of sniff change
For this analysis, only cells with >50 sniffs during baseline in each category, 55–65, 75–85, and 95–105 ms inhalation duration, were included. A random subset of 25 sniffs in each group were selected, and spike activity within these samples were used to construct PSTHs. Each PSTH was normalized such that the first 30 ms started at zero Hz on average. PSTHs were put in sequence, either 3 consecutive 95-ms inhalation duration sniffs (control sequence), or the same sequence but with the final sniff of a different inhalation duration, either 75 or 55 ms. Euclidean distance across the population of these sequences were then calculated between the control sequence and sequences ending in 55- or 75-ms inhalation duration sniffs. Detection time for the change in inhalation duration was calculated where the Euclidean distance in the last sniff exceeded the mean + 2 SDs of the baseline Euclidean distance from the first 2 sniffs.
Phase preference and putative mitral and tufted cell boundaries
The sniff-Vm modulation properties of each cell were calculated from the intertrial intervals (i.e., in absence of odor) as in previous studies (Fukunaga et al., 2012; Jordan et al., 2018). Due to the high variability of sniff behavior in awake mice, analysis was restricted to sniff cycles between 0.25 and 0.3 s in duration, where the preceding sniff cycle was also within this range. Mean Vm from the spike-subtracted Vm trace was taken as a function of sniff cycle phase for at least 150 sniffs, and this was plotted as Cartesian coordinates. The angle of the mean vector calculated by averaging these Cartesian coordinates was taken as the phase preference of the cell. To determine putative mitral cell (MC) or tufted cell (TC) type based on phase preference, we used the phase boundaries determined previously (Jordan et al., 2018): pMCs were defined as cells with phase preferences within the phase boundaries 0.39–4.11 radians (inhalation), and pTCs were defined as those with phase preferences within the remaining boundaries (exhalation).
Prediction of inhalation duration with peak spike rates with simple linear model
Sniff cycles occurring in absence of odor (during the intertrial intervals) from 25 whole-cell recorded neurons (19 pMC, 6 pTC) were divided into 10-ms bins according to inhalation duration (e.g., 30–40, 40–50 ms, and so forth) and used to construct pseudo-population activity for individual sniff cycles of a given inhalation duration. Only cells with at least 20 sniff cycles for each inhalation duration bin were included. From each cell, the peak spike rate (smallest interspike interval) within 400 ms of sniff onset was calculated for each sniff cycle. The peak spike rates across the pseudo-population for 13 random sniff cycles within each inhalation duration category were used to generate a simple linear model to predict the inhalation duration. The resulting model was then tested on the remaining 7 sniff cycles, and the relationship between predicted and true inhalation duration was compared (Fig. 5E).
Modulation of sniff-activity relationships across phase preference
In reference to extended data Fig. 5-1, to determine if the sign of relationship between inhalation duration and the various activity parameters was related to the sniff phase preference of the cell, R values for the various correlations were plotted as a function of phase preference. Only correlations with a significant p value (<0.05) and an R 2 > 0.6 were included. A sliding window of 2 radians was then used to calculate the mean R value for all cells with phase preference within the window, resulting in a mean R value as a function of phase preference. The modulation strength of mean R value as a function of phase was then calculated: the plot of mean R value was normalized to the minimum value across all phases, and the result was plotted as Cartesian coordinates. The length of the mean vector calculated by averaging these Cartesian coordinates was taken as the modulation strength of the R value across phase space. To determine the significance of this modulation, R values were shuffled with respect to phase preference 10,000 times, and the resulting distribution of shuffled modulation strength was compared to the value for the unshuffled data.
Learning time and reaction time
For the generation of learning curves as in Fig. 3, a moving window was used across five consecutive CS+ and five consecutive CS– trials and advanced by one trial on each step, and a percentage correct was calculated. The trial at which this reached at least 80% correct for five consecutive points was deemed the learning time.
Extended Figure 3-1
Additional behavioral data. (A) Mean flow change recorded 1 mm from olfactometer output for high concentration stimulus (red) and low concentration stimulus (blue). Average of 10 trials; shaded area shows standard deviation. Y scale bar is compared to that of nasal flow traces recorded in the same manner to demonstrate the negligible nature of flow change from the olfactometer. (B) Diagram to show training sequence for mice (described in methods). (C) Comparison of changes in the first inhalation duration between physiological and behavioural experiments. Black ('phys.') shows distribution of mean change used for analysis of odor responses for 20 cell-odor pairs recorded in passive mice (as in Figure 2). Purple ('F vs S') shows mean difference between red and cyan sections of the inhalation distribution as in Figure 4F for all mice and concentrations (n = 7 mice x 5 concentrations). Orange 'puff' shows average changes in mean first inhalation duration for puff vs control trials during behaviour (n = 7 mice x 5 concentrations, as in Figure 4J). (D) Average difference in lick-histograms between CS+ and CS- (concentration 4, 2.6%, vs concentration 2, 1.4%) averaged across all 7 mice for slow sniff trials (cyan data) and fast sniff trials (red data) partitioned as in Figure 4F. Dotted line indicates onset of odor stimulus (aligned to the first sniff onset). Right plot shows difference in reaction times as measured by licking for fast and slow sniff trials for all 7 mice. Mean difference in RT (fast-slow) = -92 ± 235 ms, p = 0.34 paired t-test. (E) As for panel D, but now comparing lick distributions and reaction times between puff trials (orange) and control trials (black), as in Figure 4L. Mean difference in RT (puff-control) = -36 ± 125 ms, p = 0.61, paired t-test. Download Extended Figure 3-1, TIF file.
Reaction time calculations were based on 10 or more trials of at least 80% correct performance. From lick behavior, for each trial, lick probability was calculated in a moving time window of 100 ms, aligned to the first inhalation onset after final valve opening. The difference between the probability of licking for CS+ and CS– stimuli for each time window was calculated, and the leading edge of the first window at which this calculated difference significantly deviated (>2 SD) from the values calculated in the 2-s window before odor onset was considered the reaction time. From sniff behavior, inhalation and exhalation duration values were calculated for each trial as a function of sniff number from odor onset. These values were compared between those calculated for CS+ and CS– using a t test, and the reaction time was calculated based on the first inhalation or exhalation within the series to show a significant difference (p < 0.05).
Results
Changes in sniffing can mimic the effect of increased concentration on firing rate response
We first wanted to determine if the effect of sniff changes on MTC odor response could qualitatively mimic concentration changes at the level of FR change. To do this, we used whole-cell recordings from identified MTCs in awake passive mice, as this allows unbiased sampling from the MTC population in terms of baseline FR, and reliable identification of cell type based on electrophysiology (Margrie et al., 2002; Kollo et al., 2014). On each trial, mice were presented randomly with 2-s-long odor stimuli calibrated to either 1% (low concentration) or 2.5% (high concentration) square pulses. On a small percentage of low-concentration trials, mice also received a gentle air puff to the flank, evoking fast sniffing behavior characterized by high-frequency sniffs and short inhalation durations (Figs. 1A and 1-1). For all analyses in the manuscript, “odor onset” (t = 0) is defined as the first inhalation onset during the odor stimulus. Note that several parameters of sniffing covary with inhalation duration, including the sniff duration, the previous sniff duration, and the slope of the inhalation (Fig. 1-2). Thus wherever we refer to fast or slow sniffing, this will necessarily refer to differences in these multiple parameters.
During recordings, it was apparent that some cells displayed overt changes in FR with the increase in concentration, and the most salient of these were increases in excitatory FR response, which could come even from cells that did not respond to the lower concentration (Fig. 1B, cell a and cell b). To compare changes in response for higher concentration to those for faster sniffing, we took trials from each stimulus concentration based on inhalation duration: slow sniffing trials (for both high and low concentrations) were taken as the five trials with highest mean inhalation duration (MID), and fast sniffing trials were the five trials with the lowest MID. When comparing changes in FR evoked by concentration increase to those taking place as a result of increased sniff frequency, it was apparent that very similar changes took place (Fig. 1B). Altogether we recorded from 20 mitral/tufted cell-odor pairs in such a manner, with a range of FR responses to the low concentration odorant (Fig. 1D). When taking a broad measure of the change in firing rate across the first second of the stimulus (normalized by baseline SD), changes in FR were significantly correlated between those resulting from concentration increase and those resulting from faster sniffing (R 2 = 0.70, p = 5 × 10−6, n = 20; Fig. 1C). Furthermore, comparing heat maps of the changes in FR due to increased concentration and due to increased sniff frequency revealed a very similar set of changes that were significantly correlated compared to shuffle controls (R = 0.71, p = 5 × 10−27; n = 160 time bins; Fig. 1E,F; see Methods). This level of correlation was very similar to that for the FR changes due to concentration increase when compared between two random halves of high-concentration trials (R = 0.73, p = 6 × 10−28). Overall, this indicates that the pattern of FR changes across time bins was highly similar for concentration increase and for fast sniffing.
While in the output of MTCs the effects of sniffing and concentration increase were very similar, differences were seen in the subthreshold response changes, suggesting that changes in input in the two cases were (perhaps unsurprisingly) not identical: increases in inhibition were generally larger for the concentration increase than for faster sniffing (Fig. 1-3). We suggest this could be the result of inhibitory networks that act to normalize olfactory bulb output (within limits) in the face of increased global input (Kato et al., 2013; Miyamichi et al., 2013; Roland et al., 2016).
Thus, while increased concentration causes greater increases in subthreshold inhibition than increased sniff frequency, the latter results in changes in olfactory bulb output that apparently mimic those resulting from increases in concentration.
Faster inhalation mimics effect of concentration increase on latency response in the timescale of a single sniff
It has been reported that increased concentration causes changes in response on finer temporal timescales, in particular the temporal advance of excitatory bursts (Cang and Isaacson, 2003; Fukunaga et al., 2012; Schaefer and Margrie, 2012; Sirotin et al., 2015). MCs undergo robust reductions in latency of excitation for concentration increase, while TCs—which already respond earlier—do not (Fukunaga et al., 2012). We wanted to know whether faster sniffing could cause the same temporal effects as concentration increase on a cell-by-cell basis.
To determine this, we first analyzed 13 cell-odor pairs with early excitatory responses (within 250 ms of odor onset) recorded in passive awake mice where only a single concentration stimulus (1% saturated vapor pressure) was presented to the animal across trials. Comparing the FR response over the first 250 ms for fast sniff trials (>70th percentile peak inhalation slopes) and slow sniff trials (<30th percentile), it was apparent that faster inhalation could cause a latency advance of the excitatory burst (Figs. 2A and 2-1F). Consistent with previous results (Carey and Wachowiak, 2011; Shusterman et al., 2011), faster inhalation caused a significant latency reduction in mean response onset across the dataset (latency change, fast-slow = –16 ± 14 ms, p = 0.002,a n = 13, paired t test between onsets for slow and fast inhalations; Fig. 2-1A,B). Onset latencies displayed a significant relationship with the peak firing rate during the response (Fig. 2-1D), suggesting that the most strongly activated cells respond earlier. The extent of the latency reduction correlated with the onset time during slow inhalation: if the response was of longer latency during slow sniffing, the latency reduction was greater (Fig. 2-1C), indicating that cell-odor pairs showing a stable latency are likely already activated at the earliest possible timescale. We next used sniff cycle phase preference (calculated from Vm during baseline breathing in air) to determine putative MC and TC (pMC and pTC) phenotype using subthreshold activity as previously described (Fukunaga et al., 2012; Jordan et al., 2018). Examples could be found where both pMCs and pTCs underwent reductions in latency of excitation when the sniff was fast (Fig. 2A); however, in general, reductions for pMCs were greater than reductions for pTCs (pMCs: latency change = –30 ± 7 ms, p = 7 × 10−4, paired t test,b n = 5 cell-odor pairs; pTCs: latency change = –8 ± 10 ms, p = 0.08,c paired t test, n = 8 cell-odor pairs; pMCs versus pTCs: p = 0.001,d unpaired t test; Figs. 2B and 2-1E), and this was potentially because pTCs already tended to respond with shorter latency during slow sniffs than pMCs (Fig. 2B). Thus, the effect of fast sniffing, including cell-type specificity, is similar to that previously reported for increasing concentration (Fukunaga et al., 2012).
We next asked whether the effect of sniffing on latency directly mimics the effect of concentration change within a single cell. When comparing high- and low-concentration stimuli over the first 250 ms in MTC recordings from passive mice (dataset as in Fig. 1), the only salient changes in response to increased concentration were latency advances of excitatory burst stimuli (Fig. 2C,D). When correlating the pattern of changes in spike count as before (Fig. 1F) between those occurring for sniff change and those occurring for concentration change, there was a significant positive correlation between the two (R = 0.71, p = 4 × 10−72, n = 525 time bins; Fig. 2E). This level of correlation was only marginally smaller than that when correlating spike count changes due to concentration increase between two random halves of high-concentration trials (R = 0.78, p = 4 × 10−111). Latency reductions for concentration increase were similar in magnitude to those seen due to sniff change (Fig. 2F, mean onset advance = –18 ± 10 ms, p = 0.04,e n = 4; paired t test between onsets for low and high concentration), and similar to those previously reported (Sirotin et al., 2015). To determine the effects of sniffing on ability to distinguish the two concentrations from our dataset, we calculated the Euclidean distance between FR responses to the two different stimuli. This revealed that latency changes contributed to the entirety of the difference between the two different concentrations on this timescale, with the Euclidean distance between the two dropping to baseline if the excitatory bursts were manually shifted forward for the low concentration (Fig. 2G, slow sniff vs slow sniff adv.; see Methods). Faster inhalations during low-concentration trials mimicked the latency shifts caused by concentration increase, also causing the Euclidean distance between high and low concentration stimuli to drop to baseline (Fig. 2G, slow sniff vs fast sniff).
Thus, even on short timescales, a more rapid inhalation mimics concentration increases at the level of the single-cell output from the OB, making it very difficult to distinguish the effect of increased concentration at this timescale.
Variance in sniffing has no overt impact on performance in a fine concentration discrimination task
Rodents have previously demonstrated the ability to discriminate odor concentrations (Slotnick and Ptak, 1977; Abraham et al., 2004; Parthasarathy and Bhalla, 2013; Wojcik and Sirotin, 2014); however, it is not known how sniff variance affects this ability. Given the physiology (Figs. 1 and 2; accompanying paper, Shusterman et al., 2018), we next sought to determine the capabilities of mice when distinguishing odor concentrations in a simple head-fixed go/no-go paradigm (Fig. 3A–C), despite variance in sniffing.
First, mice were trained to distinguish high-concentration (3%) versus low-concentration (1%) stimuli. Three mice were trained with the low-concentration stimulus as the CS+ (Low go), and five mice were trained with high concentration as the CS+ (High go). To ensure mice could not use flow changes to perform the task, our olfactometer design kept flow from odor outlet constant (Fig. 3-1A). After pretraining (Fig. 3-1B), all mice learned this task within a single training session (Fig. 3D) and could make rapid decisions within the timescale of a single sniff cycle (160–200 ms; Fig. 3E). To test whether mice were using trigeminal rather than olfactory input, after the task was learned, the odor would subsequently be switched to vanillin (a chemical that is thought not to stimulate trigeminal afferents; Frasnelli et al., 2011), presented at the same two concentrations. Mice learned to perform this discrimination within a significantly shorter time frame than the original odor mixture, and with the same short reaction times (Fig. 3D,E). This suggests the mice may have learned the task rule for odor concentration and applied it rapidly to the new, non–trigeminal-activating odorant. Learning in the task was likely the result of acquiring the response to the stimulus rather than learning how to perceive the difference in concentrations, since on the very first presentation of the CS– concentration after pretraining on the CS+ concentration, mice typically displayed a rapid sniffing response (Fig. 3F) classically associated with stimulus novelty (Verhagen et al., 2007; Wesson et al., 2008b; Roland et al., 2016). Thus, in this task mice can very rapidly make decisions based on relatively modest concentration differences within the timescale of a single sniff, comparing very well to their abilities in odor identity tasks (Uchida and Mainen, 2003; Wesson et al., 2009; Resulaj and Rinberg, 2015).
To determine if sniff variation impacted the concentration decisions of mice, seven trained mice were advanced on to a five-concentration task. Here, three new intermediate concentrations between the two previously learned concentrations were also presented (Fig. 4A). The concentration most similar to the learned CS+ was rewarded as a CS+, while the other two concentrations, including one exactly halfway between the previously learned concentrations, were treated as CS– (Fig. 4A). Two to three sessions of 200 trials were performed on this task, over which mice generally performed at a relatively high level of accuracy (Fig. 4B,C, mean percentage correct across session = 75 ± 6%, n = 7 mice).
Was there any evidence that mice were learning a stable sniffing strategy to perform the task? This seems unlikely, as variance in inhalation duration of the first sniff did not decrease across the session (if anything, a mild increase in variance was observed: R 2 = 0.5, p = 0.0001, regression between block number and mean variance, n = 20 blocks; Fig. 4D,E), and variance was significantly larger across blocks compared to passively exposed mice (concentration go/no-go: standard deviation of 1st inhalation duration = 19 ± 4 ms across 7 mice and 20 blocks; passive: SD = 13 ± 1 ms across 23 mice and 6 blocks; p = 0.004,f F = 12.5, df = 1; two-way ANOVA; Fig. 4D,E). Thus, while mice are capable of more stable sniffing as seen during passive exposure, they do not use this as a strategy in the concentration task.
Mice displayed a graded percentage of go trials across concentrations, indicating that the discrimination task was not trivial (Fig. 4B). Thus, if sniff changes cause shifts in perceived concentration, this should be overtly seen in the performance curves, and importantly, these shifts should have opposite polarity depending on which contingency the mouse was trained on (i.e., fast sniffing for Low go trained mice should decrease go responses, while it should increase go responses in High go trained mice). To test this, we first separated trials according to whether the first sniff was fast (<30th percentile inhalation duration) or slow (>70th percentile; Fig. 4F). This resulted in a comparison of trials between which the difference in the inhalation duration exceeded that used in the whole-cell recordings when comparing fast and slow sniff trials (Fig. 3-1C). Calculating performance curves separately for fast and slow sniff trials for each mouse revealed that there was a significant tendency for higher go rates in fast sniff trials for both Low go and High go trained animals across concentrations (Low go: p = 0.04,g F = 4.8; High go: p = 0.001,h F = 14.1; three-way ANOVA). However, this tendency did not differ across concentrations or training contingency (two-way ANOVA performed on difference in go rate between fast and low sniff trials across mice: contingency versus go rate difference: p = 0.41i; n = 7 mice × 5 concentrations; Fig. 4G). This makes it more likely that a fast sniff indicates higher motivation to do the task (consistent with previous findings; Wesson et al., 2009; Jordan et al., 2018), resulting in higher go rates across the board.
Thus, we wanted to more directly probe the effect of sniff variance on performance. On a small selection of trials for five of the mice, the puff stimulus (as used during the physiologic recordings) was used to evoke fast sniffs, including the first inhalation (Fig. 4J). The mean changes in first inhalation duration evoked by this puff were again highly comparable to that used for analysis of fast and slow sniffs in the physiologic data (Fig. 3-1C). The puff was associated with an increased error rate likely owing to distraction, but this did not reach significance (percentage correct: control trials = 83 ± 8%, probe trials = 77 ± 9%, p = 0.16,j paired t test, n = 5 mice). There was a small and insignificant tendency for increased go rates during the puff stimulus relative to control trials for mice trained on either contingency (Low go: p = 0.17,k F = 2.0, df = 1; High go: p = 0.14,l F = 2.3, df = 1; three-way ANOVA performed on go rate; Fig. 4K), and this tendency was not significantly different between mice trained on the two contingencies (p = 0.84,m F = 0.04, df = 1; two-way ANOVA performed on difference in go rate between fast and low sniff trials across mice).
Could mice be compensating for ambiguity by taking more inhalations to make the correct response? If so, this would be reflected in longer reaction times for fast compared to slow first sniff trials. On the contrary, comparing trials with fast and slow inhalations as above (Fig. 4F), reaction times (calculated between the highest and lowest concentration) were slightly though significantly shorter for fast sniff trials (Δreaction time, fast-slow = –35 ± 38 ms, p = 0.048,n paired t test, n = 7, Fig. 4H), again consistent with the idea that faster sniffing indicates a higher motivation level (Wesson et al., 2009; Jordan et al., 2018). Reaction times were unaffected by the puff stimulus compared to control trials (Δreaction time, probe-control = –5 ± 45 ms, p = 0.80,o paired t test, n = 5, Fig. 4L). This was also the case for finer concentration discrimination (Fig. 3-1D,E).
Reductions of inhalation duration of 10–20 ms rendered 1% and 2.5% concentrations hard to distinguish within our sample of MTC cells (Fig. 2G). Here we are comparing similar and even larger reductions in inhalation duration, yet behaviorally the ability to discriminate concentration on an even finer scale shows no overt differences, congruent with findings in rats for a different task in an accompanying paper (Shusterman et al., 2018). Thus, mice can easily discriminate fine concentration differences even in the face of large changes in sniffing.
Mitral and tufted cells respond to inhalation changes in absence of applied odor
We have so far shown that it is difficult to distinguish the effect of a change in inhalation or a change in concentration via their effects on MTC responses (Figs. 1 and 2), yet mice are perfectly capable of fine concentration discrimination in the face of fluctuating inhalations (Fig. 4). One explanation for this apparent conundrum could be that the olfactory system obtains information about the kind of inhalation that just occurred to infer whether concentration or sniffing evoked the response change. Congruent with the latter idea, OSNs have been demonstrated to respond to pressure changes (Grosmaitre et al., 2007), giving rise to sniff coupling in the olfactory bulb (Adrian, 1950; Macrides and Chorover, 1972; Cang and Isaacson, 2003; Margrie and Schaefer, 2003; Fukunaga et al., 2012), which disappears with naris occlusion (Margrie and Schaefer, 2003), and bouts of rapid sniffing are known to cause activity changes in MTCs in absence of applied odor (Jordan et al., 2018; Kato et al., 2013). We wanted to determine if the olfactory bulb reports graded changes in inhalation parameters on the timescale of a single sniff.
We took baseline activity in absence of odor as a proxy for the large portion of mitral and tufted cells that will not be responsive to an odor, whose activity could instead be used to directly determine the kind of sniff that took place. To do this, we analyzed the cellular activity of 45 MTCs recorded in passive mice across over 1000–2000 sniffs occurring in absence of the odor. Sniffs were categorized according to inhalation duration, and for each category peristimulus time histograms and average membrane potential waveforms were calculated over 250 ms triggered by inhalation onset (Fig. 5A–C). We found that individual MTCs would show linear transformations in their activity according to the duration of the inhalation just occurring. For example, some cells showed increased spike count (Fig. 5A1,B1) and depolarizing membrane potential (Fig. 5C1) as inhalations became faster, while others showed decreasing spike count (Fig. 5A2,B2) and more hyperpolarizing membrane potential (Fig. 5C2). 24% of cells showed significant relationships between spike count and inhalation duration (p < 0.01, linear regression; Fig. 5D) compared to only 3% in shuffle controls (odds ratio = 7.8, p = 1 × 10−5,p Fisher’s exact test). Similarly, 22% showed significant correlations with mean membrane potential compared to 2% of shuffle controls (odds ratio = 9, p = 3 × 10−5,q Fisher’s exact test; Fig. 5D). Timing of activity was also often linearly correlated with inhalation duration, generally with the peak of the membrane potential shifting to earlier times as inhalation duration reduced (significant R values in 32% of cells versus 2% in shuffle controls, odds ratio = 14, p = 1 × 10−8,r Fisher’s exact test; Fig. 5D). Altogether 51% of cells showed a significant relationship between inhalation duration and at least one or more of these activity parameters (Fig. 5D). Interestingly, the directionality of the relationships (i.e., whether a cell hyperpolarizes or depolarizes with a faster sniff), could be attributed to putative mitral or tufted cell type, as defined by sniff phase preferences (Fig. 5-1).
Extended Figure 5-1
Cell type specificity of effect of inhalation as defined by sniff-phase preference. In absence of applied odor, putative mitral cells (pMCs) respond to faster sniffs with increases in inhibition, and putative tufted cells (pTCs) with increases in excitation. (A1) Reconstructed morphology of a tufted cell recorded in awake mouse. 'Bb' refers to brain border, 'EPL' refers to external plexiform layer and 'MCL' refers to mitral cell layer (these morphologies have been previously published in Jordan et al. 2018 for different purposes). (B1) Example nasal flow and Vm trace during a rapid sniff bout (blue to purple represents longer to shorter inhalation duration on flow trace. Spikes have been cropped for display. (C1) Mean membrane potential waveform for different bands of inhalation duration: blue = long inhalation duration, purple = short. (A2)-(C2) as for A1-C1, but for a filled mitral cell recorded in an awake mouse. (D) R values for correlations between inhalation duration and mean Vm as a function of phase preference. Only strong correlations have been included (p<0.05 and R2>0.6). Grey line shows mean R value for all cells within a 2 radian moving window (centred), to give an idea of the phase modulation strength of the data. There was a significant organisation according to phase (p<0.01, bootstrapping, see methods). Boxplots to the right compare all values within the putative MC (red) and putative TC (blue) phase boundaries (mean Vm: pMC: median = 0.93, IQR = 0.84 to 0.95, n = 6; pTC: median = -0.83, IQR = -0.96 to -0.82, n = 10; p = 0.002, Ranksum). These phase boundaries are based upon those used in previous studies (Jordan et al., 2018; Fukunaga et al., 2012). (E) As for panel D, but for mean spike count per sniff. Again, there was a significant organisation according to phase preference (p<0.001; bootstrapping, see methods), and R values were significantly different between pMC and pTC boundaries (spike count: pMC: median = 0.84, IQR = -0.88 to 0.96, n = 22; pTC: median = -0.92, IQR = -0.94 to -0.89, n = 12; p = 0.008, Ranksum). Download Extended Figure 5-1, TIF file.
Extended Figure 5-2
Detection of inhalation duration change and cell 'weights' in linear model (A) Top: diagram to show construction of sniff sequences of different inhalation duration: either three of 95 ms inhalation duration (blue), or two of 95 ms with the final sniff of 55 ms duration (purple). Below shows two example PSTH sequences (from two different cells) averaged from random subsets of 25 sniffs that show the particular inhalation duration. Blue plot shows the PSTH sequence for 4 sniffs of 95 ms, and purple plot shows sequence in which the last inhalation is of 55 ms. Bottom trace shows mean Euclidean distance calculated between population vectors containing all cells constructed from the two sniff sequences as in panel A. Plot shows the average of 5 different subsets of data (made by averaging different sniff subsets for each cell), and shaded area shows standard deviation. Dashed red line indicates time of significant detection of change. (B) As for panel A, this time comparing PSTHs for a smaller inhalation duration change (95 ms, blue to 75 ms, purple). (C) Regression coefficients (weights) for individual MTCs in the linear model used to predict inhalation duration based on peak spike rates (related to Figure 5E; see methods). Download Extended Figure 5-2, TIF file.
Extended Figure 5-3
Diagram of a potential relative timing code for concentration. (A) Highly simplified diagram of the olfactory bulb, depicting only glomeruli and MTCs. Green glomeruli/MTCs show those receiving odor input (odor responsive), while grey glomeruli/MTCs show those with absence of such input (unresponsive population). While odor inputs are sparse, mechanical input (response to the pressure change associated with each sniff) is widespread. (B) Diagram to show how a relative time code may work. Each instance shows one sniff prior to and during odor stimulation. 'Nasal flow' shows these two sniffs, with grey showing slow sniffs (>95 ms inhalation duration) and red showing a fast sniff (e.g. 55 ms inhalation duration). 'Odor concentration' trace shows a step increase in concentration from zero just prior to the second sniff. Grey traces show low concentration and red shows high concentration (2 x low concentration). 'Unresponsive population' shows a hypothetical population average FR for all cells without odor inputs during the odor stimulus. Dotted traces in second and third column for the second sniff cycle show the trace for the first column, for sake of comparison. 'Odor responsive' shows the average population FR for cells receiving direct odor input. Dotted traces in second and third column for the second sniff cycle show the trace for the first column, for sake of comparison. Δt shows the difference in peak population activity for unresponsive and odor-responsive cells during the stimulus. Note that Δt remains stable unless the concentration changes, and not when the sniff alone changes. A change in Δt allows perception of a different concentration. Download Extended Figure 503, TIF file.
We next sought to determine if we could detect changes in inhalation from the spiking activity of cells in absence of odor. For all cells with enough sniff variation (>50 sniffs in each inhalation duration category: 95–105, 55–65, and 75–85 ms), we calculated sequences of spike histograms for sniffs of different inhalation durations using random subsets of sniffs within each category. We constructed either a sequence with PSTHs calculated from three consecutive sniffs of 95-ms inhalation duration or a sequence with PSTHs calculated from 2 consecutive sniffs of 95 ms, with the last PSTH instead constructed from 55-ms inhalation duration sniffs (Fig. 5-2A). Using these, it was possible to determine a change in inhalation duration (95–55-ms inhalation duration) within only 70 ± 12 ms by calculating Euclidean distances between constructed population vectors of the two different sequences (Fig. 5-2A). Smaller changes in inhalation duration (95–75 ms) could also be detected on similarly rapid timescales (Fig. 5-2B).
We next wanted to assess the overall predictive power of MTC firing activity for inhalation duration. Using 25 whole-cell MTC recordings, we generated a simple linear model to classify inhalation durations within 10-ms bins using the peak spike rates within each sniff cycle. The linear model was generated using constructed “population” activity of the 25 cells across 13 sniff cycles from each inhalation duration category and was tested subsequently on 7 sniff cycles from each category. Considering the limited number of cells and trials used, this classifier performed very well (Pearson’s r = 0.79; Fig. 5E). Comparing the model’s weights (regression coefficients) for different cells, we found that the large majority of cells were involved in the classification, but weights for pMCs tended to be stronger than for pTCs and showed significantly larger variance (p = 0.01, Bartlett test; Fig. 5-2C).
Thus MTC activity—in the absence of applied odor input—is informative of the inhalation that just occurred, such that the large population of non–odor-responsive cells could be utilized by the olfactory system to distinguish sniff changes versus concentration changes.
Discussion
For stable perception, sensory systems must find ways of encoding of stimulus features independent of fluctuating sampling behaviors, such as eye movements or sniffing. Here we show that faster sniffs can evoke response changes in the olfactory bulb that appear indistinguishable from those caused by increasing concentration (Figs. 1 and 2), yet mice are highly capable of perceiving concentration on fast timescales, regardless of sniffing parameters (Fig. 4). We reason that a way the olfactory system could distinguish these two occurrences directly is via information about the kind of sniff that just occurred. While this could conceivably happen downstream via efference copy of sniff motor commands, we find that MTC activity already allows inference about the kind of sniff that just occurred on a rapid timescale (Fig. 5). Thus, the olfactory bulb itself does not appear to be the site where the sniff-invariant percept of intensity is generated, but does appear to already contain information that could be used to generate the percept elsewhere.
Given the timescale of decision-making for concentration (Figs. 3 and 4), it seems likely that the information used by the mouse is the fast-timescale temporal shifts in excitation that have been previously described (Cang and Isaacson, 2003; Fukunaga et al., 2012; Sirotin et al., 2015). Congruently, this temporal information contributes to the entirety of the difference in response to the two concentration stimuli in our dataset (Fig. 2G). It has been suggested that high baseline firing rates of MTCs could obscure such a latency code for concentration being used (Mainland et al., 2014); however, this was based on a overestimation of baseline FRs from unit recordings. The whole-cell recordings we employ here are thought to be unbiased in terms of baseline FRs (Margrie et al., 2002; Shoham et al., 2006; Kollo et al., 2014), and discriminability of MTC responses based on latency shifts is overt (Fig. 2G). Congruently it is known that mice can perceive the latency difference in optogenetic glomerular activation on the order of tens of milliseconds (Smear et al., 2013; Rebello et al., 2014).
Sniff changes have been hypothesized to alter odor concentration profiles within the nasal cavity (Teghtsoonian et al., 1978; Shusterman et al., 2018). Here we show for the first time directly that sniff changes can indeed mimic the effect of concentration change at the level of both firing rates (Fig. 1) and temporal shifts in spike activity (Fig. 2). This is not to say that OSN input is perfectly matched when we compare faster sniff rates and higher concentration. In fact, since subthreshold inhibition is greater for the higher concentration (Fig. 1-3), it would appear that the input strength is higher for the case of increased concentration as compared to faster sniffing. Despite this, overt changes in the spiking output are very similar for increased sniff frequency compared to increased concentration. Potentially, inhibitory circuits are normalizing the spiking output across large changes in input (within a dynamic range), such that while we see differences in subthreshold inhibition, the excitatory spike outputs look very similar. Such a role has been suggested for the various external plexiform layer and juxtaglomerular cells (Tavakoli et al., 2018), including periglomerular (Aungst et al., 2003; Roland et al., 2016), dopaminergic (Banerjee et al., 2015), and parvalbumin-positive (Kato et al., 2013; Miyamichi et al., 2013) neurons.
Here we chose relatively high concentrations (1%–3% saturated vapor pressure) to ensure a good rate of response in whole-cell recordings, and relatively modest concentration differences (up to 0.5 logfold change), since we expected sniff-related differences in representation to have the most pronounced effect on performance in these fine discriminations. The question then arises, at what concentration range will sniff variation affect concentration estimation (at the level of OB activity)? This is difficult to answer without direct measurement of naris odor concentration, but we can make some tentative hypotheses. Fluid dynamic models predict that a sniff with a higher flow rate will cause the temporal profile of concentration in the naris to become steeper—i.e., more odor molecules are drawn in per unit time (Shusterman et al., 2018). Thus we would expect the concentration change that a sniff change can mimic to be proportional to both the environmental concentration and the range over which nasal flow can change (previous measurements show that this is at least twofold; e.g., Youngentob et al., 1987).
It has been known for some time that the olfactory bulb is highly modulated by the sniff cycle (Adrian, 1950; Macrides and Chorover, 1972; Cang and Isaacson, 2003; Margrie and Schaefer, 2003; Wachowiak, 2011; Fukunaga et al., 2012). Since sniff modulation is more overt in anesthetized mice and is seemingly reduced at higher sniff frequencies (Kay and Laurent, 1999; Bathellier et al., 2008; Carey and Wachowiak, 2011), the importance of sniff modulation in the awake animal may come into question. Here we find that sniff patterning of activity in awake mice gives rise to linear transformations of baseline activity as inhalation parameters are changed, a feature that is widespread throughout MTCs (Fig. 5). We thus reason that a key function of sniff modulation could be to inform the olfactory system about what kind of inhalation took place, such that a change in concentration and a change in sniffing are readily distinguishable without explicit information from breathing control centers. Congruently, we find that inhalation parameters can indeed be readily and rapidly inferred from the spiking activity of MTCs (Figs. 5E and 5-2).
Sensory encoding of sniff parameters has been hypothesized previously when psychophysics showed that humans could categorize concentrations well despite large changes in inhalation flow rate (Teghtsoonian et al., 1978). Previous work has shown that sniff modulation of the olfactory bulb is generated predominantly peripherally rather than centrally, since blocking the naris abolishes sniff modulation in the olfactory bulb (Sobel and Tank, 1993; Margrie and Schaefer, 2003; Schaefer et al., 2006; Iwata et al., 2017). One possibility is that the olfactory system uses reafference (the sensory effect of the sniff) to infer the kind of flow rate evoked by the sniff, and thus determine real changes in concentration from those caused by flow changes. This could be an explanation for the observation that olfactory receptors respond to pressure changes as well as olfactory stimuli (Connelly et al., 2015; Grosmaitre et al., 2007), and indeed may comprise a feature rather than a bug in the olfactory system. Consistently, concentration perception in humans can be affected when the nostril flow rate was changed via experimenter-induced changes in airway resistance instead of volitional changes in sniff pressure (Teghtsoonian and Teghtsoonian, 1984)—i.e., only when flow rate is altered but pressure stays constant. Moreover, imaging of the olfactory cortex in humans identified a region that primarily responds to the sensory effect of sniffing in absence of odor (Sobel et al., 1998). However, it is possible that the system employs predictive coding (Wolpert et al., 1995), in which an internal model of the respiratory motor system predicts the effect on nasal odor concentration based on the sniff command, and accounts for this somewhere in the olfactory pathway. Since airway resistance is subject to continual changes and even differs between the two nostrils (Principato and Ozenberger, 1970; Sobel et al., 1999), the internal model would require constant updating individually for each nostril, and mechanical sensory reafference from mitral and tufted cells could be used to do this on a sniff-by-sniff basis. However, since there is currently no known projection from the sniffing motor system to the olfactory system, and given that mitral and tufted cells can detect a sniff change on rapid, behaviorally relevant timescales (Fig. 5-2), a purely feedforward solution could be an efficient way to encode sniff effort.
An accompanying study intuitively suggests that the advance of odor-driven excitation as sniff frequency increases is the result of fluid dynamics in the nasal cavity (Shusterman et al., 2018). While we do not investigate the coding scheme used for invariant concentration coding, the accompanying study examines various models in detail. A large fraction of our cells show an advance of their baseline activity peak as the inhalation becomes faster (Fig. 5D). We could thus hypothesize that non–odor-responsive MTCs within a region of the bulb can provide information about the timing of inspired air reaching the epithelium. If the inhalation becomes faster, both responsive and the much larger population of unresponsive cells show a latency reduction in their peak activity, while if concentration has increased, only the sparse odor-responsive population will show this latency shift. Thus, a relative timing code could be used as a sniff-invariant representation of concentration (Fig. 5-3). Previous imaging work congruently suggests that subtracting the population response of MTCs throughout the entire bulb can act to provide more consistent trial-to-trial odor responses and remove variation associated with sniffing (Blauvelt et al., 2013). Exactly where and how the two kinds of information could be integrated to form a sniff-invariant representation of concentration should be the objective of future investigations, though recent evidence from the piriform cortex of awake mice already suggests that cortical interneurons sharpen the latency shifts evoked by concentration change and encode concentration via the synchronicity of ensemble firing (Bolding and Franks, 2017). It is possible that in a mouse performing a concentration guided task, even the olfactory bulb circuit could be altered by top-down circuits in such a way as to generate a sniff-invariant representation of concentration using information about the sniff dynamics.
Our results show that for response latency or FR on the single-cell level, fast sniffing at low concentration looks very similar to slow sniffing at high concentration. We hypothesize that the mechanism for the reduced latency of response for both increased nasal flow and increased concentration is similar—in both cases, the concentration profile in the naris is steeper, and OSNs depolarize to threshold more rapidly. At the lower end of the concentration scale, this would even occur for the most highly sensitive “first responding” MTCs that have been hypothesized to account for sniff- and concentration-invariant odor identity codes (Wilson et al., 2017). We would thus predict that, for single-cell latency or FR responses, even a large population of cells similar in properties to those recorded here would not help distinguish the two scenarios. It is possible, however, that there is a small and specialized MTC subtype that might encode odor concentration in a simple, sniff-invariant manner. We deem this less likely, since the accompanying study records from a much larger portion of MTCs and finds that sniff variance still renders concentrations very difficult to discriminate (Shusterman et al., 2018). Alternatively, population level codes could be employed for concentration encoding (e.g., via spike synchrony or higher-order features), which are robust in the face of sniff change but elude identification with single-cell recordings. It is also possible that sniff-invariant features appear in the OB after training on the concentration task; however, it must be noted that, in our hands, our mice could detect even the relatively small difference in concentration within the first presentation of the novel stimulus (Fig. 3F), suggesting that fine odor discrimination occurs readily in a sniff-independent manner, not requiring any extensive training.
In conclusion, concentration changes in the naris can be either self-generated through changes in sniffing or the consequence of a true change in environmental concentration, yet mice can perform sniff-invariant concentration discrimination. The olfactory bulb contains information about the odor concentration alongside the inhalation dynamics, which together may allow inference about whether a sniff change or a concentration change occurred, overall enabling sniff-invariant concentration perception.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments: We thank Martyn Stopps and Nicholas Burczyk for assistance with custom-made equipment, Mostafa Nashaat and Edward Bracey for initial support with behavioral training, Christoph Schmidt-Hieber for advice on whole-cell recording in vivo, and Roma Shusterman, Andrew Erskine, Christina Marin, Izumi Fukunaga, and Kevin Bolding for helpful comments on this manuscript.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
This work was supported by the Francis Crick Institute, which receives its core funding from Cancer Research UK (FC001153), the UK Medical Research Council (FC001153), and the Wellcome Trust (FC001153); by the UK Medical Research Council (grant reference MC_UP_1202/5); by the DFG (SPP 1392); and a Boehringer Ingelheim Fonds PhD fellowship to RJ. AS is a Wellcome Trust Investigator (110174/Z/15/Z).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.