Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleConfirmation, Disorders of the Nervous System

Novel Quantitative Analyses of Spontaneous Synaptic Events in Cortical Pyramidal Cells Reveal Subtle Parvalbumin-Expressing Interneuron Dysfunction in a Knock-In Mouse Model of Alzheimer’s Disease

Lingxuan Chen, Takashi Saito, Takaomi C. Saido and Istvan Mody
eNeuro 30 July 2018, 5 (4) ENEURO.0059-18.2018; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0059-18.2018
Lingxuan Chen
1Molecular, Cellular, and Integrative Physiology Interdepartmental Ph.D. Program, The David Geffen School of Medicine at University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Lingxuan Chen
Takashi Saito
3Laboratory for Proteolytic Neuroscience, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Wako, Saitama 351-0198, Japan
4Department of Neuroscience and Pathology, Research Institute of Environmental Medicine, Nagoya University 464-01, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Takashi Saito
Takaomi C. Saido
3Laboratory for Proteolytic Neuroscience, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Wako, Saitama 351-0198, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Takaomi C. Saido
Istvan Mody
1Molecular, Cellular, and Integrative Physiology Interdepartmental Ph.D. Program, The David Geffen School of Medicine at University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095
2Department of Neurology, the David Geffen School of Medicine at University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Istvan Mody
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disorder that has become a compelling global public health concern. Besides pathological hallmarks such as extracellular amyloid plaques, intracellular neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs), and loss of neurons and synapses, clinical reports have shown that epileptiform activity, even seizures, can occur early in the disease. Aberrant synaptic and network activities as well as epileptiform discharges have also been observed in various mouse models of AD. The new AppNL-F mouse model is generated by a gene knock-in approach and there are limited studies on basic synaptic properties in AppNL-F mice. Therefore, we applied quantitative methods to analyze spontaneous excitatory and inhibitory synaptic events in parietal cortex layer 2/3 pyramidal cells. First, by an objective amplitude distribution analysis, we found decreased amplitudes of spontaneous IPSCs (sIPSCs) in aged AppNL-F mice caused by a reduction in the amplitudes of the large sIPSCs with fast rates of rise, consistent with deficits in the function of parvalbumin-expressing interneurons (PV INs). Second, we calculated the burstiness and memory in a series of successive synaptic events. Lastly, by using a novel approach to determine the excitation-to-inhibition (E/I) ratio, we found no changes in the AppNL-F mice, indicating that homeostatic mechanisms may have maintained the overall balance of excitation and inhibition in spite of a mildly impaired PV IN function.

  • Alzheimer’s disease
  • App knock-in mouse
  • excitation
  • inhibition
  • interneurons
  • parvalbumin

Significance Statement

Using novel quantitative analyses of spontaneous synaptic currents in L2/3 pyramidal cells, we revealed subtle deficits in the function of parvalbumin-expressing interneurons (PV INs) in a new mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the AppNL-F mice. We applied novel statistical and analytical methods to further characterize the properties of spontaneous postsynaptic currents (sPSCs). Our approach provides rigorous quantitative tools for the analysis of synaptic events in mouse models of disease for objectively and reliably revealing even subtle disease-related alterations.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease that has become a global public health concern with the gradually aging population (Ferri et al., 2005; Mayeux and Stern, 2012). The pathologic hallmarks of AD include amyloid β (Aβ) plaques, neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs), and neuronal degeneration (Serrano-Pozo et al., 2011). Meanwhile, neuronal hyperactivity, network hyperexcitability, and spontaneous epileptiform activity have been observed in human amyloid precursor protein (hAPP) transgenic mouse models with high levels of Aβ (Palop et al., 2007, 2011; Busche et al., 2008; Palop and Mucke, 2009; Harris et al., 2010; Roberson et al., 2011; Yan et al., 2012; Busche and Konnerth, 2016). Hypersynchronous network activity, also observed in AD patients (Lam et al., 2017), may further contribute to the emergence of AD symptoms such as cognitive impairment in early stages of the disease (Noebels, 2011; Bakker et al., 2012; Scharfman, 2012). Specifically, electrophysiological studies in hAPPJ20 mice have revealed decreased intrinsic excitability of GABAergic parvalbumin-expressing interneurons (PV INs), which then led to aberrant network activity and cognitive deficits in the animals (Verret et al., 2012).

A key molecule in AD is Aβ, which is a 40 or 42 amino acid peptide derived from APP. Most of the studies on pathologic Aβ and subsequent alterations in synaptic and network activities have been using transgenic mouse models that overproduce mutant hAPP. Although these mouse models simulate several key aspects of human AD (Games et al., 1995; Hsiao et al., 1996; Sturchler-Pierrat et al., 1997; Götz et al., 2004; Cheng et al., 2007; Cissé et al., 2011; Verret et al., 2012), in these animals, Aβ and other APP fragments maybe overproduced and/or ectopically expressed (Chang and Suh, 2005; Mitani et al., 2012; Nicolas and Hassan, 2014; Kerridge et al., 2015; Nhan et al., 2015; Willem et al., 2015; Xia et al., 2016; Sasaguri et al., 2017). The AppNL-F mouse model of AD was generated by manipulating the mouse App gene using a knock-in strategy (Nilsson et al., 2014; Saito et al., 2014), and this new mouse model is expected to advance our understanding of AD pathology. The paucity of studies characterizing the electrophysiological phenotype, such as synaptic transmission, in this model, prompted us to develop reliable and objective analytical approaches to identify possible synaptic changes in AppNL-F mice.

Here, we have studied synaptic transmission in parietal cortex layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in the AppNL-F mouse model of AD. The results showed a significant reduction in the average amplitude and average rate of rise (RR) of spontaneous IPSCs (sIPSCs) in old homozygous AppNL-F mice compared to age-matched controls. Moreover, through amplitude distribution analysis, we found that the changes in the amplitude and RR were confined to fast-rising large amplitude sIPSCs but not the small ones, indicating potential impairment in perisomatic-targeting PV INs which has been described in the hAPPJ20 mice (Verret et al., 2012). We also used novel quantitative approaches to further examine the properties of spontaneous postsynaptic currents (sPSCs). Firstly, we applied methods previously used in characterizing the bursty nature of various successive events (Goh and Barabási, 2008; Schleiss et al., 2016) to evaluate the memory and burstiness of sPSCs, thus adding a new angle to the analysis of synaptic events. Secondly, we have developed a novel approach to calculate the excitation-to-inhibition (E/I) ratio with higher accuracy that incorporates all properties of sPSCs. Taken together, the new approaches can further our understanding of the occurrence of spontaneous synaptic events and provide quantitative tools to study synaptic transmission in AD and other disease models.

Materials and Methods

Animal

All animal use was approved by the University of California, Los Angeles Chancellor’s Committee on Animal Welfare.

AppNL-F homozygous mice (AD; 18.3 ± 2.2 months; n = 8, three males and five females) were originally obtained from the research group led by Dr. Takaomi C. Saido at the Laboratory for Proteolytic Neuroscience, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Saitama, Japan (Nilsson et al., 2014; Saito et al., 2014). Wild-type control mice (WT; 20.5 ± 3.2 months; n = 6, four males and two females) were either aged littermates from the AppNL-F mice breeding or obtained from the National Institute on Aging. Mice were held in the vivarium on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle with free access to water and food.

Electrophysiology

Slice preparation

Mice were anesthetized by isoflurane through inhalation, followed by rapid decapitation. Coronal acute brain slices were prepared on a Vibratome at 350-μm thickness in ice-cold cutting solution (135 mM NMDG, 10 mM D-glucose, 4 mM MgCl2, 0.5 mM CaCl2, 1 mM KCl, 1.2 mM KH2PO4, 20 mM HEPES, 3 mM kynurenic acid; ∼20 sucrose to adjust the osmolality; 305–310 mmol/kg; pH 7.35; bubbled with 100% O2). Then, slices were kept on a floating mesh platform in a recovery chamber filled with sucrose-based artificial CSF (sACSF; 55 mM sucrose, 85 mM NaCl, 25 mM D-glucose, 2.5 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 0.5 mM CaCl2, 4 mM MgCl2, 26 mM NaHCO3; 300–305 mmol/kg; bubbled with 95%O2/5% CO2). The recovery chamber was kept in 34°C water bath for 30 min and then moved to room temperature.

Whole-cell recording

For whole-cell recording, ACSF was used (126 mM NaCl, 10 mM D-glucose, 2 mM MgCl2, 2 mM CaCl2, 2.5 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 1.5 mM sodium pyruvate, 1 mM L-glutamine; 295–300 mmol/kg; bubbled with 95%O2/5% CO2). Acute brain slices were placed in a submerged chamber filled with ACSF at a flow rate of 6–7 ml/min and a temperature of 32–34°C. An intracellular solution (140 mM cesium methanesulfonate, 5 mM CsCl, 2 mM MgCl2, 2 mM Na-ATP, 2 mM Na-GTP, 200 μM EGTA, 10 mM HEPES; 275–280 mmol/kg; pH 7.35) was used for the recording of spontaneous synaptic events.

Data acquisition and analysis

Spontaneous excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (sEPSCs and sIPSCs) were recorded at a holding voltage of -60 and 0 mV, respectively, using an Axopatch 200B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Inc.). All recordings were low-pass filtered at 1.5 kHz and digitized at a sampling frequency of 10 kHz, then recorded using a custom-written LabVIEW-based software (EVAN). Spontaneous events were detected with EVAN for the analysis of frequency and amplitude. The measurement of phasic discharge for the E/I ratio calculation was done using Igor Pro 6.3.7.2 (WaveMetrics, Inc.). The Igor code for the procedure will be provided on request. Other analyses on the synaptic events were done in Excel 2011 (Microsoft Corporation) with customized and built-in functions. Nested one-way ANOVA test was used to analyze E/I ratio differences, and it was done with an Excel spreadsheet from an online source (http://www.biostathandbook.com/nestedanova.html). All other statistical tests were done in Prism 6 (GraphPad Software, Inc.) by using non-parametric Mann–Whitney test or paired Wilcoxon test, and p < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. All the group statistics are presented as mean ± SEM (standard error of mean).

Separating small and large events

To identify the large amplitude sEPSC and sIPSC events from all detected synaptic events, we used an approach that could objectively calculate a threshold for large events based on the amplitude distribution of all events from a cell. The cumulative distribution of all amplitudes was plotted and fitted with either one or two cumulative normal distributions using the NORM.DIST function in Excel.

The goodness of the two fits was compared with an F test. The F value was calculated as:Embedded Image (1)

In Equation 1, RSS1 and RSS2 are the residual sum of squares when fitting with one and two distributions, respectively; p1 and p2 are the number of parameters used for fitting with one and two distributions, respectively; and n is the number of data points used for fitting. Then, a p value was calculated using the F.DIST function in Excel. For all the recordings in both groups, the p values approached 0, indicating that two distributions always fit the cumulative curves significantly better than one distribution. Next, a threshold for detecting large PSCs was chosen as the x value (amplitude) that corresponds to the y value (cumulative distribution) where the first distribution ends.

Burstiness and memory calculation

The interevent intervals (IEIs) of synaptic events were measured as the time difference between two adjacent PSCs. Then, burstiness and memory of these events were calculated with equations in previous published papers (Goh and Barabási, 2008; Schleiss et al., 2016):Embedded Image (2) Embedded Image (3)

In Equation 2, B is burstiness; and σ and μ are the standard deviation (SD) and mean of the IEIs, respectively, of each cell. In Equation 3, M is memory; and Embedded Image is the Spearman rank auto-correlation of the IEI array at a lag of 1.

Calculation of E/I ratio

Measurement of phasic charge

We have previously described an approach to objectively measure the tonic and phasic components of inhibitory or excitatory events (Glykys and Mody, 2007). This method does not rely on subjective thresholds to detect spontaneous events. For the analysis of E/I ratio based on this approach, the raw recording traces of certain durations were analyzed. Using a custom-written Igor procedure, an all-point histogram (bin width = 1 pA) was plotted for segments of 15 s from either sEPSC or sIPSC recordings and smoothed by Savitzky–Golay algorithm (2nd order 17 points smoothing) to obtain the peak value. This histogram was always skewed to one side by the presence of spontaneous synaptic events. The non-skewed side of the histogram was fitted with a Gaussian distribution from the end of the non-skewed side to 95% of the peak value over the peak to the skewed side. The mean of this fitted Gaussian distribution was considered to be the mean holding current. Then, this Gaussian curve was mirror-imaged to the skewed side, and the area between the Gaussian distribution and the all-point histogram divided by the total number of points in this area was considered to be the mean phasic charge (in pC). The E/I was determined as the ratio between the excitatory and the inhibitory phasic charges over 15-s recordings. For each cell, we have randomly selected 10 segments from sEPSC and sIPSC recordings (see below for details) and obtained 100 E/I values.

Driving forces for sEPSCs and sIPSCs

To get a more accurate measurement of the E/I ratios, we have also taken the driving force for sEPSCs and sIPSCs into account. The holding voltages for recording sEPSCs and sIPSCs were -60 and 0 mV, respectively. The reversal potentials of the currents activated by AMPA and NMDA are close to 0 mV, and that calculated for the GABA receptors is approximately -70 mV. Therefore, at Vh = -60 mV, the driving forces for sEPSCs and sIPSCs were ∼60 and 10 mV, respectively; whereas at Vh = 0 mV, the driving forces for sEPSCs and sIPSCs were ∼0 and 70 mV, respectively. At Vh = -60 mV where sEPSCs were recorded, it is possible that the recordings were contaminated with small outward sIPSCs. Because this contamination should be absent in picrotoxin (PTX), for some recordings at Vh = -60 mV, we plotted the all-point histograms of one randomly chosen 15 s segment before and one after washing in 50 μM PTX. We then compared the SD (σ) of the Gaussian fits of the outward currents (non-skewed sides, as described above) of the two histograms. A significant amount of small contaminating sIPSCs in the sEPSC recording would significantly decrease the σ after PTX. In total, we have got three WT and seven AD cells with PTX wash in at -60 mV, and the variance before and after PTX was not significantly different in these 10 cells (WT: σ = 3.92 vs 3.79; p = 0.75; AD: σ = 2.54 vs 3.06; p = 0.22; before vs after PTX; Wilcoxon paired test). Therefore, there was no detectable contamination of small outward sIPSCs in our sEPSC recordings at Vh = -60 mV.

Determination of sample size

With the approach above, we can get the phasic charge by randomly choosing m segments from sEPSC recording and n segments from sIPSC recording for each cell, and then get m × n E/I ratios. To determine the minimum m and n value needed to get the best estimation of the real population E/I value, we used the equation described in a previously published paper (Eckblad, 1991) for estimating the fractional deviation of the sample mean from the real population mean:Embedded Image (4)

In Equation 4, t is the value from a Student’s t distribution with a degree of freedom equal to one less than the sample size and with a 95% confidence interval; σ is the sample SD; μ is the sample mean; and N is the sample size.

Firstly, we have determined a way to randomly select segments of recording from a trace. Random integer numbers between the start and end time (in s) of the recording were generated successively using the RANDBETWEEN function in Excel, and the random numbers will be used as the start time of different segments. If two segments had an overlap, then the start time of the latter-generated segment was regenerated. If one segment ran into the seal test period, the start time was also regenerated. In this way, we have randomly selected 10 segments of 15 s from sEPSC and sIPSC recording for each cell, yielding 100 E/I ratios (N = 100). We then randomly selected different number of values (N = 10, 20, 30, …, 90) from these 100 values for calculating the σ and μ of different sample sizes. With Equation 4, we calculated the fractional deviation of different sample sizes (N = 10, 20, …, 100) for each cell. We also calculated the fractional deviation using the σ and μ of N = 100 for all sample sizes and estimated the fractional deviation of more sample sizes. With this approach applied to every cell, we found that when N = 100, the fractional deviation of the sample mean from the real population mean reached a sufficiently low level. Therefore, considering the accuracy of estimating the real E/I ratio and the convenience of choosing samples, we decided to choose 10 segments from sEPSC recording and 10 from sIPSC recording, thus obtaining all possible 100 E/I values for each cell.

Statistical test for phasic charges and E/I ratios

Since we have recordings from different animals, and for each animal we had recording(s) from one or multiple cells, we want to take all variables into account when we compare the Es and Is as well as the E/I ratios between groups. Therefore, we decided to use a nested one-way ANOVA to determine the difference between the WT and AD group. The test was done using an Excel spreadsheet from an online source (http://www.biostathandbook.com/nestedanova.html). Briefly, there were groups (WT and AD), subgroups (for a two-level nested ANOVA), and sub subgroups (for a three-level nested ANOVA) in this test. The phasic E and I charges of WT and AD cells were compared by a two-level nested one-way ANOVA, in which each animal was treated as a subgroup, and the average of the 10 Es and 10 Is of each cell from this animal was listed as values within the subgroup. Then, the p values of the difference between groups (WT and AD) as well as among subgroups within each group (six and eight mice in the WT and AD group, respectively) were calculated. The E/I ratios of WT and AD cells were compared by a three-level nested one-way ANOVA, because we did not want to use the average of 100 E/I ratios as a single value for each cell and lose the accuracy (see the determination of sample size). Therefore, besides groups (WT and AD) and subgroups (different animals), there were also sub subgroups, which are different cells from each animal, and then the 100 E/I ratios were listed as values within the sub subgroups. Similarly, the p values of the difference between groups (WT and AD), among subgroups within each group (six mice in the WT group; eight mice in the AD group), as well as among sub subgroups within each subgroup (9 and 12 cells in the WT and AD group, respectively) were calculated.

Results

Reduced sIPSC amplitudes in AppNL-F mice

Previous studies have shown altered glutamatergic and GABAergic transmission in parietal cortex in various mouse models of AD (Busche et al., 2008; Roberson et al., 2011; Verret et al., 2012; Hazra et al., 2013; Perez et al., 2016). We wanted to investigate whether there are alterations in excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in the AppNL-F mouse model of AD. We have recorded sEPSCs and sIPSCs (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 1A) in parietal cortex layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in both WT (Appwt/wt; WT) and AppNL-F homozygous (APPNL-F/NL-F; AD) mice. We chose to study synaptic transmission in the parietal cortex because in this region, altered synaptic transmission and aberrant network activities were observed in hAPP mice (Roberson et al., 2011; Verret et al., 2012), and atrophies and hypoperfusion were observed in AD patients (Jacobs et al., 2011, 2012). The average ages were comparable between the two groups (20.5 ± 3.2 vs 18.3 ± 2.2 months; n = 6 vs 8 mice; WT vs AD; p = 0.5088; Mann–Whitney test). For each cell we analyzed the same number of consecutively occurring sEPSCs or sIPSCs. The events were randomly chosen from all detected events throughout the whole recording period (12 cells for each group; 161 sEPSCs and 626 sIPSCs for each cell). The average frequencies of both sEPSCs and sIPSCs remained unchanged in the AD mice (sEPSC frequency = 7.42 ± 0.97 vs 5.79 ± 1.08 Hz, p = 0.2415; sIPSC frequency = 11.10 ± 0.99 vs 9.91 ± 0.98 Hz, p = 0.2415; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 1B). The average amplitude of sIPSCs was significantly reduced while that of sEPSCs remained constant in the AD animals (sEPSC amplitude = 25.97 ± 1.52 vs 23.39 ± 1.07 pA, p = 0.1432; sIPSC amplitude = 51.55 ± 5.16 vs 37.06 ± 2.50 pA, p = 0.0145; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 1C). Since we were using WT mice from two different sources as control, either from littermates in the AppNL-F colony (three cells; two mice) or from NIA (nine cells; four mice), we have examined whether there was a significant difference within the two subtypes of our controls. Statistical analysis revealed no significant difference either in the frequency and amplitude of sEPSCs (p = 0.6000 and p = 0.4818; sEPSC frequency and amplitude, respectively; littermates vs NIA; Mann–Whitney test) or in those of sIPSCs (p = 0.6000 and p = 0.4818; sIPSC frequency and amplitude, respectively; littermates vs NIA; Mann–Whitney test) between the AppNL-F littermates and the NIA mice.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

The average amplitude of sIPSCs was reduced in AD pyramidal cells. A, Example raw recording traces of sEPSCs (left) and sIPSCs (right), with a holding voltage (Vh) of -60 and 0 mV, respectively. B, The average frequency of either sEPSCs (left) or sIPSCs (right) remained unchanged in the AD group. C, The average amplitude of sEPSCs remained constant (left), while that of sIPSCs was significantly decreased (right) in the AD group. *p < 0.05, n.s.: not significant.

The reduction of sIPSC amplitudes in AppNL-F mice was caused by a decrease in large amplitude events

The presence of large amplitude events during sIPSC recording led us to further investigate whether small and large events contributed differently to the overall changes in the amplitudes of sPSCs. We were specifically interested in analyzing the large sIPSCs because they potentially originate from perisomatic-targeting PV INs which generate strong inhibition on postsynaptic neurons (Freund and Katona, 2007). Since previous research has shown a deficiency in the intrinsic excitability of GABAergic PV INs in hAPPJ20 mouse model of AD (Verret et al., 2012), we wanted to examine whether the reduction in the sIPSC amplitudes we observed could be related to PV IN dysfunction in the novel AppNL-F AD model.

To study the small and large sPSCs separately, we first tested whether the events could be objectively divided into two groups (see Materials and Methods). We found that the cumulative amplitude distributions of all sPSCs for each individual cell could be significantly better fitted by two cumulative normal distributions rather than one, as indicated by an F test (see Materials and Methods; data not shown), thus sPSCs in all cells could be separated into two groups according to their amplitudes (Fig. 2A). Based on the fitted curve, we chose to use the value on the x-axis (amplitudes) corresponding to the end of the first distribution on the y-axis (cumulative probability) as the threshold for large events (Ath; Fig. 2A). Then, sPSCs larger than Ath were classified as large events, while those smaller than Ath were considered as small events. The averaged Ath for sEPSCs was comparable between WT and AD, while for sIPSCs the averaged Ath was significantly decreased in AD mice (sEPSC Ath = 23.75 ± 0.96 vs 22.18 ± 0.75 pA, p = 0.1735; sIPSC Ath = 41.64 ± 3.14 vs 33.24 ± 1.64 pA, p = 0.0332; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 2B), which is consistent with the reduced amplitude of sIPSCs (Fig. 1C).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

The sEPSCs and sIPSCs from each cell can be divided into two groups according to the bimodal distribution of their amplitudes. A, Example of choosing an objective amplitude threshold (Ath) to divide the sIPSCs into small and large groups. The cumulative distribution of all sIPSC amplitudes was fitted with either one (dashed line) or two (solid line) cumulative normal distributions. When fitting with two distributions, the amplitude threshold (Ath) is the corresponding X value on the fitted curve at the start of the second distribution. Inset, the difference between the predicted and the actual amplitude values (residuals) when fitting with one (dashed line) or two (solid line) cumulative normal distributions. B, The Ath of sEPSCs and sIPSCs in WT and AD cells. The Ath of sEPSCs remained constant (left), while that of sIPSCs was reduced (right) in AD cells. *p < 0.05, n.s.: not significant.

From the cumulative distribution curves of all sPSCs detected from all cells in WT and AD mice, qualitatively the amplitude distribution of sEPSCs were close between WT and AD group, while the distribution curve of sIPSCs of AD mice were significantly left-shifted compared to that of WT mice (Fig. 3A), which is consistent with a decreased average sIPSC amplitude in AD animals (Fig. 1C). Moreover, the two curves of sIPSC amplitudes became further separated when the events were larger than Ath (Fig. 3A, arrowheads), revealing a more significant reduction in large sIPSCs than in small sIPSCs. When small and large events were analyzed separately, the amplitudes of both large and small sEPSCs remained constant (small sEPSC amplitude = 17.80 ± 0.48 vs 17.60 ± 0.41 pA, p = 0.7987; large sEPSC amplitude = 37.90 ± 3.52 vs 30.81 ± 1.43 pA, p = 0.1135; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 3B,C), which is consistent with the unchanged overall sEPSC amplitude (Fig. 1C). For sIPSCs, the amplitude of large sIPSCs was decreased in AD mice while that of the small sIPSCs remained unchanged (small sIPSC amplitude = 28.35 ± 1.54 vs 24.07 ± 1.00 pA, p = 0.0519; large sIPSC amplitude = 95.36 ± 13.81 vs 57.31 ± 7.27 pA, p = 0.0284; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 3B,C), consistent with a decrease in the overall sIPSC amplitude (Fig. 1C) and the trend in the distribution curves (Fig. 3A).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

The average amplitude of large sIPSCs was significantly reduced in AD pyramidal cells. A, The cumulative distributions of sEPSC (left) and sIPSC (right) amplitudes of all cells recorded in WT (gray curve) and AD (black curve) mice. The arrows are showing the mean of the first and second distribution when fitting with two cumulative normal distributions for WT (gray arrows) and AD (black arrows) cells. The arrowheads are showing the Ath values for WT (gray arrowheads) and AD (black arrowheads) cells. B, The average amplitudes of small sEPSCs (left) and small sIPSCs (right) were both not significantly altered in the AD group. C, The average amplitude of large sEPSCs was unchanged (left), while that of large sIPSCs was significantly reduced (right) in the AD group. *p < 0.05, n.s.: not significant.

Therefore, we have revealed that there are two populations in all sPSCs, and that they can be separated by an objectively calculated amplitude threshold. Further, we have demonstrated that the decrease in the overall sIPSC average amplitude was primarily contributed by the large events rather than the small ones, which is consistent with our PV IN dysfunction hypothesis in the AppNL-F mice.

Rates of rise of sIPSCs were decreased in AppNL-F mice

The decreased amplitudes of large sIPSCs in AppNL-F mice is consistent with a PV-IN dysfunction. However, the origin of the large sIPSCs needs to be further analyzed, since they may be a summation of multiple events (Williams et al., 1998), or they could be generated by other types of perisomatic-targeting interneurons (Freund and Katona, 2007). Since PV-INs target the α1 subunit-containing GABAA receptors, they mainly generate fast-rising PSCs (Thomson et al., 2000; Nyíri et al., 2001; Klausberger et al., 2002). To further elucidate the origination of the large sIPSCs, we also examined the RR of the synaptic currents. We defined the RR of an event as the ratio between 80% of the peak amplitude and RT 10-90, which is the time it takes for the current to rise from 10% to 90% of the peak amplitude (Fig. 4A). Thus, the RR of an event is the slope of the main rising phase and indicates how fast an event rises to its peak. We found that the RRs of sEPSCs was unchanged in the AD group, while those of sIPSCs was significantly decreased in AD animals (sEPSC RR = 24.92 ± 1.41 vs 29.33 ± 3.37 pA/ms, p = 0.5137; sIPSC RR = 49.82 ± 3.62 vs 33.63 ± 5.06 pA/ms, p = 0.0100; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 4B).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

The analysis of RR revealed a significant decrease only in the large sIPSCs of AD pyramidal cells. A, The RR was calculated as the ratio between the increase in current and the rise time (RT) from 10% (10% Amp) to 90% (90% Amp) of the peak. B, The average RR of all sEPSCs was unchanged (left), while that of all sIPSCs was reduced (right) in the AD group. C, The average RR of either small sEPSCs (left) or small sIPSCs (right) was not altered in the AD group. D, The average RR of large sEPSCs remained constant (left), while that of large sIPSCs was significantly reduced (right) in the AD group. *p < 0.05, n.s.: not significant.

When we separated the events based on their amplitudes into small and large ones, the results showed that the RRs of small sEPSCs were comparable to those of the large ones for both WT and AD mice (p = 0.6707, WT; p = 1432, AD; Mann–Whitney test). Whereas for sIPSCs, the RRs of large events were significantly higher than those of the small ones in both groups (p < 0.0001, WT; p = 0.0029, AD; Mann–Whitney test). These results are consistent with the idea that large amplitude sIPSCs comprise events originating from soma-innervating PV INs that synapse onto α1 subunit-containing GABAA receptors. Furthermore, the RRs of both small and large sEPSCs were comparable between WT and AD group (small sEPSC RR = 24.07 ± 1.63 vs 24.47 ± 2.56 pA/ms, p = 0.7987; large sEPSC RR = 25.76 ± 1.64 vs 34.18 ± 4.49 pA/ms, p = 0.3474; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 4C,D). For sIPSCs, while the RRs of the small events were similar, those of the large events were significantly decreased in AD mice (small sIPSC RR = 24.97 ± 2.55 vs 22.21 ± 1.33 pA/ms, p = 0.6707; large sIPSC RR = 74.67 ± 7.47 vs 45.06 ± 9.27 pA/ms, p = 0.0083; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 4C,D), indicating again that the changes in large amplitude and fast-rising sIPSCs led to the overall reduction of RRs in AD mice.

In conclusion, our results from analyzing the RR further indicated that the large sIPSCs could be generated by soma-targeting PV INs according to their fast rising phase. Moreover, the reduction in the overall rates of rise in AD cells is contributed by large events but not the small ones, which is again consistent with a PV IN dysfunction.

Burstiness and memory of sPSCs

Besides analyzing the frequency and amplitude of sEPSCs and sIPSCs, we also wanted to investigate whether there are specific firing patterns within those events, such as bursts. to examine the burstiness of sPSCs on pyramidal cells in WT and AD mice, we first determined the IEIs of all sPSCs as well as those of the large sPSCs. Then, we used previously described methods to quantitatively determine the burstiness (B) and memory (M) in the firing of a single cell (Goh and Barabási, 2008; Schleiss et al., 2016; see Materials and Methods). By default, the values of both burstiness and memory will be within the range (-1, 1). For burstiness, a value closer to 1 (i.e., when SD is very large) means the firing pattern is more “bursty,” while a value closer to -1 (i.e., when SD is very low) indicates a more regular firing pattern. In between, a Poisson process will result in a burstiness value of 0. For memory, a positive value means that short IEIs tend to be followed by short IEIs and long IEIs by long IEIs, whereas a negative value means that short IEIs tend to be followed by long IEIs or vice versa. A value near 0 indicates no memory in the system.

The results from burstiness analysis showed that for sEPSCs and sIPSCs in both WT and AD cells, the average values were all negative (BEPSC_WT = -0.19 ± 0.04; BIPSC_WT = -0.35 ± 0.04; BEPSC_AD = -0.13 ± 0.04; BIPSC_AD = -0.31 ± 0.04), indicating that the occurrence of sPSCs was more regular than bursty. Also, there was no significant difference in burstiness either for sEPSCs or for sIPSCs between WT and AD cells (data not shown). However, when we compared the burstiness of small events versus that of large events, we found that in general, small sPSCs had more negative B values than large sPSCs for both WT (BSmall EPSC = -0.06 ± 0.03 vs BLarge EPSC = -0.01 ± 0.03; BSmall IPSC = -0.14 ± 0.02 vs BLarge IPSC = -0.08 ± 0.02; Fig. 5A) and AD (BSmall EPSC = -0.05 ± 0.02 vs BLarge EPSC = -0.01 ± 0.02; BSmall IPSC = -0.12 ± 0.02 vs BLarge IPSC = -0.07 ± 0.02; Fig. 5A) mice, and that difference was significant for sEPSCs of both WT and AD cells (p = 0.0425 for WT; p = 0.0425 for AD; Wilcoxon paired test; Fig. 5A) and for sIPSCs of AD cells only (p = 0.0522 for WT; p = 0.0122 for AD; Wilcoxon paired test; Fig. 5A). These results indicate that for both sEPSCs and sIPSCs, the small events occurred regularly throughout the recordings, while the large events were generated in a more random pattern. For memory, the averaged memory for all sEPSCs and all sIPSCs was close to 0 (MEPSC_WT = 0.07 ± 0.02; MIPSC_WT = 0.03 ± 0.01; MEPSC_AD = 0.08 ± 0.02; MIPSC_AD = 0.06 ± 0.02), indicating that there is little memory in the system. Also, there was no significant difference in memory either between small and large events or between WT and AD groups (data not shown).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

The analysis of burstiness (B) and memory (M) showed different firing patterns of small and large events. A, Left, The burstiness of large sEPSCs was significantly closer to 0 (indicating a Poisson process) than that of small sEPSCs for both WT and AD pyramidal cells. Right, the burstiness of large sIPSCs was significantly closer to 0 than that of small sIPSCs for AD cells, but not for WT cells. B, The distribution of B and M values calculated with all sEPSCs (left) and sIPSCs (right) from each cell in a B-M plane showed negative B values, indicating regular firing pattern, and close-to-zero M values, indicating little memory in both WT and AD pyramidal cells. *p < 0.05.

According to the original study (Goh and Barabási, 2008), the “burstiness” of a system can have two qualitatively different origins, which are presented by parameters B and M described above. Therefore, it is helpful to place B and M values in a (B, M) space which can give an intuitive presentation of certain properties of a system, which, in our case, is the pattern of incidence of sPSCs (Fig. 5B). From the (B, M) plots of cell averaged memory and burstiness of sEPSCs and sIPSCs in WT and AD group, it is evident that both currents lack memory and are not bursty. On the contrary, the sPSCs appeared to emerge in a rather regular pattern.

A novel approach for determining the synaptic E/I ratio

A proper balance between excitation (E) and inhibition (I) is difficult to define consistently under all circumstances (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011), but it is considered to be crucial for a normal circuit function. Studies have shown that a disrupted E/I balance can cause network dysfunction and various diseases (Fernandez et al., 2007; Kehrer et al., 2008; Gogolla et al., 2009), including AD (Schmitt, 2005; Rissman and Mobley, 2011; Busche and Konnerth, 2016). Therefore, after examining the excitatory and inhibitory currents separately in pyramidal cells, we also wanted to check whether the E/I balance is altered in AppNL-F mice. Previous methods have been using multiple indices to calculate the E/I ratio, such as the peaks, charges (Bartley and Dobrunz, 2015) and conductance (Wehr and Zador, 2003; Cruikshank et al., 2007) of PSCs. These existing methods usually use only one averaged property of the events, and they usually acquire the E and I value over a short period of recording. Here, we introduce a new approach, which can incorporate all properties of sPSCs into a single index and is calculated multiple times over a long duration of recording to determine the E/I ratio.

Our previous work has described a method to objectively separate the tonic and phasic components from raw electrophysiological recordings (Glykys and Mody, 2007; see Materials and Methods). There are several advantages of using this method to determine the phasic E and I exerted onto a cell: firstly, no subjective thresholds are needed to detect spontaneous events, and tonic and phasic activities are separated objectively according to the all-point histograms of raw recording traces; secondly, the values of E and I depend on all properties of the phasic currents, such as frequency, amplitude, charge, etc.; and thirdly, it allows the selection of multiple segments from the entire recording and acquire a vast number of E/I values.

We decided to randomly choose m segments from sEPSC recordings (E) and n segments from sIPSC recordings (I) for each cell, and each segment with a duration of 15 s. Then, we will have m E and n I values, resulting in m × n E/I ratios. To determine the proper values for m and n, we did a sample size analysis using a previously described method (Eckblad, 1991; see Materials and Methods). The fractional deviation of the sample mean from the real population mean is negatively correlated with the sample size (Fig. 6A). For the cell shown in Figure 6A, when the number of E/I ratios reached 100, the fractional deviation decreased to 0.09, which means that the sample mean is ±9% within the real population mean. Next, we did the same analysis for each cell in the WT and AD groups. The results showed that when calculating 100 E/I ratios, the fractional deviation in predicting the real population mean would be sufficiently low (FWT = 0.12 ± 0.02 vs FAD = 0.12 ± 0.01, p > 0.9999; n = 9 vs n = 12; WT vs AD; Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 6B). Therefore, for each cell, we decided to randomly select 10 segments of 15 s from both the E and I raw recording traces, thus obtaining 10 E and 10 I values and 100 E/I ratios for one cell (Fig. 6C).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

A new approach to determine the E/I ratio can predict the real E/I value of each cell with high accuracy. A, The fractional deviation of the sample mean from the real population mean decreases with the increase of sample size (N). The curve shows deviations calculated with the mean (μ) and SD (σ) of 100 samples (N = 100), and the 10 dots are deviation values calculated with the μ and σ of N = 10, 20, …, 100 samples, from left to right. B, When acquiring 100 E/I ratios, for both WT and AD cells, the fractional deviation reaches a low value (∼12%), and it is not significantly different in the two groups. C, Randomly selecting 10 segments of 15 s from the raw recording traces of sEPSCs (top) and sIPSCs (middle) can yield 100 E/I ratios when crossing over the 10 phasic E and 10 phasic I charges (bottom). n.s.: not significant.

We first examined the phasic E and I values in WT and AD cells. As described above, each group there will be N × 10 values of E and I, in which N is the number of cells in each group (NWT = 9; NAD = 12). Considering that these values were collected from different animals (six and eight mice in the WT and group, respectively) and different cells (9 and 12 cells in the WT and AD group, respectively), besides comparing the average of all values in each group, we also included the variance among different animals and cells into the analysis. Therefore, we decided to use a two-level nested one-way ANOVA test (see Materials and Methods). The results showed that there was no significant difference either between groups (pEs = 0.1276; pIs = 0.2602; WT vs AD), or among subgroups within each group (pEs = 0.2080; pIs = 0.0973; among animals). The results from analyzing detected PSCs indicated a significant difference in the average sIPSC amplitude but not the average sEPSC amplitude between groups (Fig. 1C). The absence of significance in the phasic I charge analysis may be caused by the large variance among subgroups, i.e., among different animals (31.98% and 52.83% of total variance, Es and Is, respectively), as well as within subgroups, i.e., among different cells (53.15% and 44.70% of total variance, Es and Is, respectively). In comparison, the variance between the WT and AD group was smaller (14.87% and 2.47% of total variance, Es and Is, respectively). Moreover, in the phasic charge analysis, when there were no subjective detection thresholds chosen, many more very small events (below detection threshold) contributed to the phasic component of a recording. As we have shown above, the large events but not the small ones contributed to the overall difference in amplitudes (Fig. 3B,C) as well as rates of rise (Fig. 4C,D) between groups. Therefore, the considerably more small events below detection threshold contributing to the phasic charge resulted in the overall unchanged phasic E and I; however, in the histograms of all the values, we could see a left-shift in both Es and Is in the AD group (Fig. 7A).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

The E/I ratios calculated with the new approach showed a tendency of reduction in the AD group. A, Histogram of phasic E (left) and phasic I (right) charges showed a reduction in both E and I in AD pyramidal cells (10 E and 10 I values for each cell). B, Histogram of E/I ratio showed a shift to the left in the AD group, however the E/I was not significantly altered according to a nested two-way ANOVA test. C, The E/I values for the AD group showed large variations among the 100 values in each cell (shown as each bar), among different cells in each animal (shown as each #), and among different animals. Box: 25th to 75th percentiles, line in the box: median, whiskers: min to max, +: mean.

For the calculation of the E/I ratio, firstly we’ve taken into account the driving forces for sEPSCs and sIPSCs. In our recording scenario, the driving forces for sEPSCs and sIPSCs were ∼60 and 70 mV, respectively (see Materials and Methods). Therefore, to counterbalance the difference in driving forces, the E/I ratio was determined as Embedded Image . To compare the E/I ratio between WT and AD animals, we chose to use a three-level nested one-way ANOVA test (see Materials and Methods), because now we want to use all the 100 E/I ratios for a higher accuracy, there was an extra level, sub subgroups, which were different cells from each animal. Then, the 100 values from each cell were listed under each sub subgroup. As a result of the unchanged phasic E and I values, the E/I ratio remained constant in the AD group as well (E/I = 0.54 ± 0.02 vs 0.47 ± 0.02, p = 0.3882), although there was a left-shift in the histogram of all E/I ratios in the AD group (Fig. 7B). Another reason that might result in the unchanged E/I ratio might be the large variance among different animals and cells. As an example, in the AD group, the E/I ratios varied a lot from animal to animal as well as among different cells in the same animal (Fig. 7C).

Discussion

We investigated synaptic alterations by analyzing sPSCs in parietal cortex layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in the AppNL-F mouse model of AD. Through objective amplitude distribution analysis, we found that while the properties of small sIPSCs and all sEPSCs remained constant, the amplitudes of large and fast-rising sIPSCs were reduced in AD mice, which is consistent with deficits in PV INs. To further characterize the properties of the spontaneous synaptic currents as part of a chain of successive events, we introduced the calculation of burstiness and memory into the analysis. We also used a novel method to determine the E/I ratio with a high accuracy. Owing to the large variance among different animals and cells, we found no significant alteration in the overall E/I ratio in AD mice. It remains to be determined whether homeostatic mechanisms or experimental conditions are responsible for the unchanged E/I ratio in AppNL-F mice. Together, our results have shown the advantage of using various quantitative approaches in the field of synaptic physiology, and provided ground for future research in elucidating the synaptic and network properties of the AppNL-F mouse model as well as in seeking potential therapeutic targets to treat symptoms associated with, and contributing to, the development of AD.

In our endeavor to thoroughly study synaptic changes in the cortices of AppNL-F mice, we developed reliable and objective methods to analyze spontaneous synaptic events. One key element of our approach was to avoid using subjective and randomly chosen thresholds. For the separation of small and large synaptic events, we used the cumulative distribution of all event amplitudes to determine the threshold of separation between small and large amplitude currents. For obtaining the E/I ratio, we used the all-point histogram and Gaussian fits to calculate the phasic charges, where no subjective thresholds were needed for the detection of synaptic currents. While it is considered to be crucial for a normal circuit function, the E/I ratio remains difficult to define consistently under all circumstances (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011). Existing methods have used one or multiple parameters of mainly stimulus-evoked PSCs, such as peak amplitudes and charges, over short periods of recording to determine the E/I ratio (Wehr and Zador, 2003; Cruikshank et al., 2007; Bartley and Dobrunz, 2015). Considering potential drawbacks of these methods, we developed an approach which could (1) incorporate all properties of the PSCs such as frequency, amplitude, charge, etc. and (2) increase the accuracy of E/I calculation by including multiple segments over relatively long periods of recording (4–6 min) since the E/I balance is dynamically changing in the brain.

In the AppNL-F mice, using our new approach, the E/I ratios were not significantly altered compared to controls, neither were the phasic E and I charges. The main reason for the phasic I charge being unchanged while detected sIPSC amplitudes being reduced could be that the former was actually incorporating very small currents that remained undetected with traditional thresholding. Inclusion of these events may have weakened the effect of large sIPSCs on reducing the overall sIPSC amplitudes in AD mice. Thus, the unchanged phasic E and I charges resulted in an unchanged E/I ratio. It is also possible that there were compensatory mechanisms taking place in the AD brain to counterbalance the outcomes of decreased inhibition. This possibility will require further investigation. The large variance among different animals and cells when measuring the E/I ratios with our method has to be noted. This could be a result of us choosing multiple segments over long recording durations and the dynamic nature of the E/I balance in the brain. Our E and I recordings were done at different time points at distinct holding voltages. Although we have corrected for the different driving forces, other conditions might be varied when recording E and I. Future analysis using this method should measure E and I at an intermediate voltage (between EGABA and EGlutamate) at the same time to control these variations.

Through objective amplitude analysis on the sPSCs, we were able to examine the changes in small and large currents separately. Results revealed that the reduction in the overall average amplitude of the sIPSCs in AppNL-F mice was caused by a decrease in the average amplitude of the large sIPSCs but not the small ones. Moreover, large sIPSCs had significantly faster rates of rise than small ones, and the high RR of large sIPSCs was reduced in AppNL-F mice. These alterations are consistent with a possible PV IN dysfunction in the AppNL-F mice, because PV INs target α1 subunit-containing GABAA receptors on the somatic and perisomatic compartments of their postsynaptic targets, thus generating fast-rising sIPSCs with large amplitudes (Thomson et al., 2000; Nyíri et al., 2001; Klausberger et al., 2002; Freund and Katona, 2007). Altered PV IN inhibition on pyramidal cells is closely linked to abnormal network oscillations and cognitive functions. Network hyperexcitability and epileptiform events have been observed in most mouse models of AD (Palop et al., 2007, 2011; Busche et al., 2008; Palop and Mucke, 2009; Harris et al., 2010; Roberson et al., 2011; Yan et al., 2012; Busche and Konnerth, 2016) and in AD patients using high resolution recordings (Lam et al., 2017). Hypersynchronous network activities have been shown to emerge during reduced gamma oscillation, which is a rhythm that depends on the activities of fast-spiking PV INs and contributes to cognitive functions (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009; Buzsáki and Wang, 2012; Sohal, 2012; Cho et al., 2015). Specifically, PV IN defects due to a reduced expression of voltage-gated sodium channels Nav1.1 were found in hAPPJ20 mice, and reversing the Nav1.1 reduction by Nav1.1-BAC bacterial rescued PV IN synaptic currents and gamma oscillations and reduced network hypersynchrony and memory deficits in these mice (Verret et al., 2012). A recent study showed that there was no change in the overall Nav1.1 level in either APP-overexpressing or AppNL-F mice brain homogenates, suggesting that Nav1.1 downregulation may be a phenotype unique to hAPPJ20 mice (Saito et al., 2016). However, the fraction of PV IN in brain homogenates, depending on the brain area, may be too small to detect changes in this specific population of interneurons. Therefore, a brain region specific reduction of PV IN Nav1.1 may still be present in AppNL-F mice. Our results based on rigorous sIPSC analyses are consistent with this possibility. Compared to existing hAPP mouse lines widely used to study cerebral Aβ amyloidosis as well as synaptic, network and behavioral dysfunctions (Götz and Ittner, 2008; LaFerla and Green, 2012), the AppNL-F mouse model we used was generated by a gene knock-in approach (Nilsson et al., 2014; Saito et al., 2014) with no hAPP overexpression. However, we have not identified alterations in excitation and inhibition in the cortex in hAPP-overexpressing models, and further studies in those models should be done using the methods described here to investigate the role of hAPP overexpression in E/I balance. Also, studies on the functioning of PV INs in the AppNL-F model are needed, and research on network activities and related cognitive functions could reveal new phenotypes beyond those observed in the hAPP mice.

Studying abnormal synaptic transmission and related aberrant network activities and cognitive deficits in mouse models of AD have valuable translational implications for human AD research. Epilepsy with clinical seizures are particularly more prevalent in sporadic and familial AD patients at younger ages, while overt seizures are not reported in most AD cases (Scarmeas et al., 2009). Therefore, it remains difficult to study potential epileptiform activities in patients with conventional EEG electrodes because the abberant activity takes place in deep temporal structures. A recent study using foramen ovale electrodes positioned adjacent to the mesial temporal lobe revealed clinically silent hippocampal seizures and epileptiform spikes during sleep, a period especially important for memory consolidation (Lam et al., 2017). In urethane-anesthetized AppNL-F mice, abnormal gamma oscillations were observed in the entorhinal cortex, including reduced theta-γ coupling and impaired phase-locking of layer 2/3 pyramidal cell spiking activities (Nakazono et al., 2017). Since PV INs are essential for generating gamma oscillations and for suppressing hypersynchronous network activity, an impairment in PV IN function shared among different mouse models of AD and present during the initial stages of the disease may have highly relevant clinical and therapeutic consequences. The development of specific Na+ channel modulators is an obvious start in this direction (Anderson et al., 2017; Frederiksen et al., 2017).

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements: We thank the late R. Main Lazaro for animal care.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This research was supported by the China Scholarship Council Scholarship 2011632034 (to L.C.) and by the National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS030549 and the Coelho Endowment (to I.M.).

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Anderson LL, Hawkins NA, Thompson CH, Kearney JA, George AL (2017) Unexpected efficacy of a novel sodium channel modulator in Dravet syndrome. Sci Rep 7:1682. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-01851-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Bakker A, Krauss GL, Albert MS, Speck CL, Jones LR, Stark CE, Yassa MA, Bassett SS, Shelton AL, Gallagher M (2012) Reduction of hippocampal hyperactivity improves cognition in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuron 74:467–474. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.023 pmid:22578498
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Bartley AF, Dobrunz LE (2015) Short-term plasticity regulates the excitation/inhibition ratio and the temporal window for spike integration in CA1 pyramidal cells. Eur J Neurosci 41:1402–1415. doi:10.1111/ejn.12898 pmid:25903384
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Busche MA, Konnerth A (2016) Impairments of neural circuit function in Alzheimer’s disease. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 371. doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0429
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    Busche MA, Eichhoff G, Adelsberger H, Abramowski D, Wiederhold K-H, Haass C, Staufenbiel M, Konnerth A, Garaschuk O (2008) Clusters of hyperactive neurons near amyloid plaques in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Science 321:1686–1689. doi:10.1126/science.1162844
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Buzsáki G, Wang X-J (2012) Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:203–225. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444 pmid:22443509
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Cardin JA, Carlén M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai L-H, Moore CI (2009) Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459:663–667. doi:10.1038/nature08002 pmid:19396156
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Chang KA, Suh YH (2005) Pathophysiological roles of amyloidogenic carboxy-terminal fragments of the β-amyloid precursor protein in Alzheimer’s disease. J Pharmacol Sci 97:461–471. doi:10.1254/jphs.CR0050014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Cheng IH, Scearce-Levie K, Legleiter J, Palop JJ, Gerstein H, Bien-Ly N, Puoliväli J, Lesné S, Ashe KH, Muchowski PJ, Mucke L (2007) Accelerating amyloid-beta fibrillization reduces oligomer levels and functional deficits in Alzheimer disease mouse models. J Biol Chem 282:23818–23828. doi:10.1074/jbc.M701078200 pmid:17548355
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Cho KKA, Hoch R, Lee AT, Patel T, Rubenstein JLR, Sohal VS (2015) Gamma rhythms link prefrontal interneuron dysfunction with cognitive inflexibility in Dlx5/6(+/-) mice. Neuron 85:1332–1343. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Cissé M, Halabisky B, Harris J, Devidze N, Dubal DB, Sun B, Orr A, Lotz G, Kim DH, Hamto P, Ho K, Yu GQ, Mucke L (2011) Reversing EphB2 depletion rescues cognitive functions in Alzheimer model. Nature 469:47–52. doi:10.1038/nature09635 pmid:21113149
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Cruikshank SJ, Lewis TJ, Connors BW (2007) Synaptic basis for intense thalamocortical activation of feedforward inhibitory cells in neocortex. Nat Neurosci 10:462. doi:10.1038/nn1861 pmid:17334362
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Eckblad JW (1991) How many samples should be taken? Bioscience 41:346–348. doi:10.2307/1311590
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. ↵
    Fernandez F, Morishita W, Zuniga E, Nguyen J, Blank M, Malenka RC, Garner CC (2007) Pharmacotherapy for cognitive impairment in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Nat Neurosci 10:411. doi:10.1038/nn1860 pmid:17322876
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Brodaty H, Fratiglioni L, Ganguli M, Hall K, Hasegawa K, Hendrie H, Huang Y, Jorm A, Mathers C, Menezes PR, Rimmer E, Scazufca M (2005) Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet 366:2112–2117. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67889-0 pmid:16360788
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Frederiksen K, Lu D, Yang J, Jensen HS, Bastlund JF, Larsen PH, Liu H, Crestey F, Dekermendjian K, Badolo L, Laursen M, Hougaard C, Yang C, Svenstrup N, Grunnet M (2017) A small molecule activator of Nav1.1 channels increases fast-spiking interneuron excitability and GABAergic transmission in vitro and has anti-convulsive effects in vivo. Eur J Neurosci 46:1887–1896. doi:10.1111/ejn.13626
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Freund TF, Katona I (2007) Perisomatic inhibition. Neuron 56:33–42. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.012 pmid:17920013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Games D, Adams D, Alessandrini R, Barbour R, Berthelette P, Blackwell C, Carr T, Clemens J, Donaldson T, Gillespie F (1995) Alzheimer-type neuropathology in transgenic mice overexpressing V717F beta-amyloid precursor protein. Nature 373:523–527. doi:10.1038/373523a0 pmid:7845465
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Glykys J, Mody I (2007) The main source of ambient GABA responsible for tonic inhibition in the mouse hippocampus. J Physiol 582:1163–1178. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2007.134460 pmid:17525114
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Gogolla N, LeBlanc JJ, Quast KB, Südhof TC, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK (2009) Common circuit defect of excitatory-inhibitory balance in mouse models of autism. J Neurodev Disord 1:172–181. doi:10.1007/s11689-009-9023-x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Goh KI, Barabási AL (2008) Burstiness and memory in complex systems. EPL (Europhysics Lett) 81:48002. doi:10.1209/0295-5075/81/48002
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    Götz J, Ittner LM (2008) Animal models of Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:532–544. doi:10.1038/nrn2420 pmid:18568014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Götz J, Streffer JR, David D, Schild A, Hoerndli F, Pennanen L, Kurosinski P, Chen F (2004) Transgenic animal models of Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders: histopathology, behavior and therapy. Mol Psychiatry 9:664–683. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001508 pmid:15052274
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Harris JA, Devidze N, Verret L, Ho K, Halabisky B, Thwin MT, Kim D, Hamto P, Lo I, Yu G-Q, Palop JJ, Masliah E, Mucke L (2010) Transsynaptic progression of amyloid-β-induced neuronal dysfunction within the entorhinal-hippocampal network. Neuron 68:428–441. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.020 pmid:21040845
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Hazra A, Gu F, Aulakh A, Berridge C, Eriksen JL, Žiburkus J (2013) Inhibitory neuron and hippocampal circuit dysfunction in an aged mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One 8:e64318. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064318
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Hsiao K, Chapman P, Nilsen S, Eckman C, Harigaya Y, Younkin S, Yang F, Cole G (1996) Correlative memory deficits, Abeta elevation, and amyloid plaques in transgenic mice. Science 274:99–102. doi:10.1126/science.274.5284.99
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    Isaacson JS, Scanziani M (2011) How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron 72:231–243. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027 pmid:22017986
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Jacobs HI, Van Boxtel MP, Uylings HB, Gronenschild EH, Verhey FR, Jolles J (2011) Atrophy of the parietal lobe in preclinical dementia. Brain Cogn 75:154–163. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.003 pmid:21130554
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Jacobs HIL, Van Boxtel MPJ, Jolles J, Verhey FRJ, Uylings HBM (2012) Parietal cortex matters in Alzheimer’s disease: an overview of structural, functional and metabolic findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:297–309. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.06.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Kehrer C, Maziashvili N, Dugladze T, Gloveli T (2008) Altered excitatory-inhibitory balance in the NMDA-hypofunction model of schizophrenia. Front Mol Neurosci 1:6. doi:10.3389/neuro.02.006.2008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Kerridge C, Kozlova DI, Nalivaeva NN, Turner AJ (2015) Hypoxia affects neprilysin expression through caspase activation and an APP intracellular domain-dependent mechanism. Front Neurosci 9:426. doi:10.3389/fnins.2015.00426 pmid:26617481
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Klausberger T, Roberts JDB, Somogyi P (2002) Cell type- and input-specific differences in the number and subtypes of synaptic GABA(A) receptors in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 22:2513–2521. pmid:20026228 pmid:11923416
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    LaFerla FM, Green KN (2012) Animal models of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2:a006320. doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a006320
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Lam AD, Deck G, Goldman A, Eskandar EN, Noebels J, Cole AJ (2017) Silent hippocampal seizures and spikes identified by foramen ovale electrodes in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Med 23:678–680. doi:10.1038/nm.4330
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  35. ↵
    Mayeux R, Stern Y (2012) Epidemiology of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2:a006239. doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a006239
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Mitani Y, Yarimizu J, Saita K, Uchino H, Akashiba H, Shitaka Y, Ni K, Matsuoka N (2012) Differential effects between γ-secretase inhibitors and modulators on cognitive function in amyloid precursor protein-transgenic and nontransgenic mice. J Neurosci 32:2037–2050. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4264-11.2012 pmid:22323718
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Nakazono T, Lam TN, Patel AY, Kitazawa M, Saito T, Saido TC, Igarashi KM (2017) Impaired in vivo gamma oscillations in the medial entorhinal cortex of knock-in Alzheimer model. Front Syst Neurosci 11:48. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2017.00048
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  38. ↵
    Nhan HS, Chiang K, Koo EH (2015) The multifaceted nature of amyloid precursor protein and its proteolytic fragments: friends and foes. Acta Neuropathol 129:1–19. doi:10.1007/s00401-014-1347-2 pmid:25287911
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Nicolas M, Hassan BA (2014) Amyloid precursor protein and neural development. Development 141:2543–2548. doi:10.1242/dev.108712 pmid:24961795
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    Nilsson P, Saito T, Saido TC (2014) New mouse model of Alzheimer’s. ACS Chem Neurosci 5:499–502. doi:10.1021/cn500105p pmid:24852598
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Noebels J (2011) A perfect storm: converging paths of epilepsy and Alzheimer’s dementia intersect in the hippocampal formation. Epilepsia 52:39–46. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02909.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Nyíri G, Freund TF, Somogyi P (2001) Input-dependent synaptic targeting of alpha(2)-subunit-containing GABA(A) receptors in synapses of hippocampal pyramidal cells of the rat. Eur J Neurosci 13:428–442. pmid:11168550
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Palop JJ, Mucke L (2009) Epilepsy and cognitive impairments in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 66:435–440. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2009.15 pmid:19204149
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Palop JJ, Mucke L, Roberson ED (2011) Quantifying biomarkers of cognitive dysfunction and neuronal network hyperexcitability in mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease: depletion of calcium-dependent proteins and inhibitory hippocampal remodeling. Methods Mol Biol 670:245–262. doi:10.1007/978-1-60761-744-0_17 pmid:20967595
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Palop JJ, Chin J, Roberson ED, Wang J, Thwin MT, Bien-Ly N, Yoo J, Ho KO, Yu GQ, Kreitzer A, Finkbeiner S, Noebels JL, Mucke L (2007) Aberrant excitatory neuronal activity and compensatory remodeling of inhibitory hippocampal circuits in mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuron 55:697–711. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.025 pmid:17785178
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Perez C, Ziburkus J, Ullah G (2016) Analyzing and modeling the dysfunction of inhibitory neurons in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One 11:e0168800. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0168800 pmid:28036398
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Rissman RA, Mobley WC (2011) Implications for treatment: GABAA receptors in aging, Down syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurochem 117:613–622. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2011.07237.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Roberson ED, Halabisky B, Yoo JW, Yao J, Chin J, Yan F, Wu T, Hamto P, Devidze N, Yu G-Q, Palop JJ, Noebels JL, Mucke L (2011) Amyloid-β/Fyn-induced synaptic, network, and cognitive impairments depend on tau levels in multiple mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 31:700–711. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4152-10.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    Saito T, Matsuba Y, Mihira N, Takano J, Nilsson P, Itohara S, Iwata N, Saido TC (2014) Single App knock-in mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Neurosci 17:661–663. doi:10.1038/nn.3697 pmid:24728269
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Saito T, Matsuba Y, Yamazaki N, Hashimoto S, Saido TC (2016) Calpain activation in Alzheimer’s model mice is an artifact of APP and presenilin overexpression. J Neurosci 36:9933–9936. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1907-16.2016 pmid:27656030
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    Sasaguri H, Nilsson P, Hashimoto S, Nagata K, Saito T, De Strooper B, Hardy J, Vassar R, Winblad B, Saido TC (2017) APP mouse models for Alzheimer’s disease preclinical studies. EMBO J 36:2473–2487 . doi:10.15252/embj.201797397
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    Scarmeas N, Honig LS, Choi H, Cantero J, Brandt J, Blacker D, Albert M, Amatniek JC, Marder K, Bell K, Hauser WA, Stern Y (2009) Seizures in Alzheimer disease: who, when, and how common? Arch Neurol 66:992–997. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2009.130 pmid:19667221
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Scharfman HE (2012) “Untangling” Alzheimer’s disease and epilepsy. Epilepsy Curr 12:178–183. doi:10.5698/1535-7511-12.5.178 pmid:23118602
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Schleiss M, Smith JA, Schleiss M, Smith JA (2016) Two simple metrics for quantifying rainfall intermittency: the burstiness and memory of interamount times. J Hydrometeorol 17:421–436. doi:10.1175/JHM-D-15-0078.1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  55. ↵
    Schmitt HP (2005) On the paradox of ion channel blockade and its benefits in the treatment of Alzheimer disease. Med Hypotheses 65:259–265. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2005.03.011 pmid:15922097
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Serrano-Pozo A, Frosch MP, Masliah E, Hyman BT (2011) Neuropathological alterations in Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 1:a006189. doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a006189 pmid:22229116
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    Sohal VS (2012) Insights into cortical oscillations arising from optogenetic studies. Biol Psychiatry 71:1039–1045. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.024 pmid:22381731
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K (2009) Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature 459:698–702. doi:10.1038/nature07991 pmid:19396159
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Sturchler-Pierrat C, Abramowski D, Duke M, Wiederhold K-H, Mistl C, Rothacher S, Ledermann B, Bürki K, Frey P, Paganetti PA, Waridel C, Calhoun ME, Jucker M, Probst A, Staufenbiel M, Sommer B (1997) Two amyloid precursor protein transgenic mouse models with Alzheimer disease-like pathology. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:13287–13292. pmid:9371838
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    Thomson AM, Bannister AP, Hughes DI, Pawelzik H (2000) Differential sensitivity to Zolpidem of IPSPs activated by morphologically identified CA1 interneurons in slices of rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 12:425–436. pmid:10712623
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Verret L, Mann EO, Hang GB, Barth AMI, Cobos I, Ho K, Devidze N, Masliah E, Kreitzer AC, Mody I, Mucke L, Palop JJ (2012) Inhibitory interneuron deficit links altered network activity and cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer model. Cell 149:708–721. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2012.02.046
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Wehr M, Zador AM (2003) Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature 426:442–446. doi:10.1038/nature02116 pmid:14647382
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Willem M, Tahirovic S, Busche MA, Ovsepian SV, Chafai M, Kootar S, Hornburg D, Evans LD, Moore S, Daria A, Hampel H, Müller V, Giudici C, Nuscher B, Wenninger-Weinzierl A, Kremmer E, Heneka MT, Thal DR, Giedraitis V, Lannfelt L, et al. (2015) η-Secretase processing of APP inhibits neuronal activity in the hippocampus. Nature 526:443–447. doi:10.1038/nature14864 pmid:26322584
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Williams SR, Buhl EH, Mody I (1998) The dynamics of synchronized neurotransmitter release determined from compound spontaneous IPSCs in rat dentate granule neurones in vitro . J Physiol 510:477–497. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.477bk.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Xia D, Gutmann JM, Götz J (2016) Mobility and subcellular localization of endogenous, gene-edited tau differs from that of over-expressed human wild-type and P301L mutant tau. Sci Rep 6:29074. doi:10.1038/srep29074 pmid:27378256
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    Yan XX, Cai Y, Shelton J, Deng SH, Luo XG, Oddo S, Laferla FM, Cai H, Rose GM, Patrylo PR (2012) Chronic temporal lobe epilepsy is associated with enhanced Alzheimer-like neuropathology in 3×Tg-AD mice. PLoS One 7:e48782. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048782 pmid:23155407
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Lorna Role, Stony Brook

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Grace Stutzmann, Geoffrey Murphy. Note: If this manuscript was transferred from JNeurosci and a decision was made to accept the manuscript without peer review, a brief statement to this effect will instead be what is listed below.

This is a moderately revised manuscript which has identified a reduction in large sIPSCs and their RR, in the parietal cortex of 18-20 month old APP KI mouse created by Saido et al. The authors demonstrate a modest imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in parietal cortex layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in this AppNL-F KI mouse model of Alzheimer's disease (AD). Specifically, the study reveals that spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents of large amplitude are reduced in the AD mice, while the amplitudes of spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic currents are unaltered.

Given the limited description of the AppNL-F KI mice to date, the characterization per se is important. Nevertheless, the reviewers concurred that while interesting, the majority of data from this study are negative, and the source, mechanism, and implications of the single significant difference are unclear.

In fact, the strength of the paper resides in the methodology for sPSCS acquisition and analysis and in the discussion about network properties. Most interesting are the approaches used to quantify the balance between excitatory/inhibitory synaptic inputs. This coupled with the methods for quantification of the “burstiness” and “memory” aspects of spontaneous postsynaptic currents represent a significant step forward in the field of synaptic physiology.

The reviewers also noted that although the manuscript discusses the advantages of the KI model there was no explicit comparison to conventional transgenic/overexpressing APP mouse models. As such it is unclear if there are distinct advantages to using this model for examining cortical network properties.

Overall, the reviewers concluded that the focus of this paper is stronger from a methods and interpretation perspective, than in terms of the modest findings reported. As such, the reviewers recommend that the presentation is restructured so that the manuscript (starting with rewriting the abstract) better reflects the inherent strengths of the methodological approaches described, rather than in trying to oversell a relatively minor observation.

Minor comments and code related:

1. Please include age of mice in the Methods section.

2. The authors used a custom Igor written procedure to calculate the E/I ratios. They did not provide the code but they do describe what the code accomplished. It would be great if they provided the code or at least stated that they would be willing to share the code (with proper credit).

Author Response

Synthesis of Reviews:

Computational Neuroscience Model Code Accessibility Comments for Author (Required):

The authors used a custom Igor written procedure to calculate the E/I ratios. They did not provide the code but they do describe what the code accomplished. It would be great if they provided the code or at least stated that they would be willing to share the code (with proper credit).

A statement about sharing the code upon request is now included in the Methods section. Significance Statement Comments for Author (Required):

The significance statement should emphasize the methodological strengths and contributions of the paper, rather than the observation of a modest change in E/I balance.

The significance statement has been revised accordingly Comments on the Visual Abstract for Author (Required): n/a

Synthesis Statement for Author (Required):

This is a moderately revised manuscript which has identified a reduction in large sIPSCs and their RR, in the parietal cortex of 18-20 month old APP KI mouse created by Saido et al. The authors demonstrate a modest imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in parietal cortex layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in this AppNL-F KI mouse model of Alzheimer's disease (AD). Specifically, the study reveals that spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents of large amplitude are reduced in the AD mice, while the amplitudes of spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic currents are unaltered.

Given the limited description of the AppNL-F KI mice to date, the characterization per se is important. Nevertheless, the reviewers concurred that while interesting, the majority of data from this study are negative, and the source, mechanism, and implications of the single significant difference are unclear.

The reduced large amplitude fast rise rate sIPSCs are consistent with a dysfunction of PV+ interneurons in the parietal cortex, but identifying the precise mechanism(s) for the dysfunction were outside the scope of our investigation. Nevertheless, we have performed a thorough analysis of the spontaneous synaptic events, and have shown that compensatory mechanisms re-balance the excitation/inhibition in the AD mice. The significance of these, at first sight, subtle alterations on the development of the Ab plaques remains to be determined.

In fact, the strength of the paper resides in the methodology for sPSCS acquisition and analysis and in the discussion about network properties. Most interesting are the approaches used to quantify the balance between excitatory/inhibitory synaptic inputs. This coupled with the methods for quantification

of the “burstiness” and “memory” aspects of spontaneous postsynaptic currents represent a significant step forward in the field of synaptic physiology.

We thank the reviewers for highlighting the strengths of our study. We have made revisions according to the suggestions.

The reviewers also noted that although the manuscript discusses the advantages of the KI model there was no explicit comparison to conventional transgenic/overexpressing APP mouse models. As such it is unclear if there are distinct advantages to using this model for examining cortical network properties.

As outlined in the ms and references therein, many of the APP overexpressing transgenic mouse models of AD are considered to produce artifacts due to the ectopic expression of mutant human APP on top of the mouse APP. Therefore, our intent was not to compare the APP(NL-F) mice to these previous

models, but to build a database for the synaptic function in these mice for later comparison to other

second and third generation mouse models of AD.

Overall, the reviewers concluded that the focus of this paper is stronger from a methods and interpretation perspective, than in terms of the modest findings reported. As such, the reviewers recommend that the presentation is restructured so that the manuscript (starting with rewriting the abstract) better reflects the inherent strengths of the methodological approaches described, rather than in trying to oversell a relatively minor observation.

We have revised the ms accordingly. Minor comments and code related:

1. Please include age of mice in the Methods section.

The age and sex of the mice are now included in the Methods section

2. The authors used a custom Igor written procedure to calculate the E/I ratios. They did not provide the code but they do describe what the code accomplished. It would be great if they provided the code or at least stated that they would be willing to share the code (with proper credit).

A statement about sharing the code upon request is now included in the Methods section.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 5 (4)
eNeuro
Vol. 5, Issue 4
July/August 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Novel Quantitative Analyses of Spontaneous Synaptic Events in Cortical Pyramidal Cells Reveal Subtle Parvalbumin-Expressing Interneuron Dysfunction in a Knock-In Mouse Model of Alzheimer’s Disease
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Novel Quantitative Analyses of Spontaneous Synaptic Events in Cortical Pyramidal Cells Reveal Subtle Parvalbumin-Expressing Interneuron Dysfunction in a Knock-In Mouse Model of Alzheimer’s Disease
Lingxuan Chen, Takashi Saito, Takaomi C. Saido, Istvan Mody
eNeuro 30 July 2018, 5 (4) ENEURO.0059-18.2018; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0059-18.2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Novel Quantitative Analyses of Spontaneous Synaptic Events in Cortical Pyramidal Cells Reveal Subtle Parvalbumin-Expressing Interneuron Dysfunction in a Knock-In Mouse Model of Alzheimer’s Disease
Lingxuan Chen, Takashi Saito, Takaomi C. Saido, Istvan Mody
eNeuro 30 July 2018, 5 (4) ENEURO.0059-18.2018; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0059-18.2018
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
    • Author Response
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • Alzheimer’s disease
  • App knock-in mouse
  • excitation
  • inhibition
  • interneurons
  • parvalbumin

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Confirmation

  • Supramodal representation of the sense of body ownership in the human parieto-premotor and extrastriate cortices
  • Evaluating the Burstlet Theory of Inspiratory Rhythm and Pattern Generation
  • Sex and Individual Differences in Alcohol Intake Are Associated with Differences in Ketamine Self-Administration Behaviors and Nucleus Accumbens Dendritic Spine Density
Show more Confirmation

Disorders of the Nervous System

  • Increased physiological GDNF levels have no effect on dopamine neuron protection and restoration in a proteasome inhibition mouse model of Parkinson's disease
  • Microglial Expression of the Wnt Signaling Modulator DKK2 Differs between Human Alzheimer’s Disease Brains and Mouse Neurodegeneration Models
  • Spinal Cord Injury AIS Predictions Using Machine Learning
Show more Disorders of the Nervous System

Subjects

  • Disorders of the Nervous System

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.