Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleNew Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

Serotonin Selectively Increases Detectability of Motion Stimuli in the Electrosensory System

Mariana M. Marquez and Maurice J. Chacron
eNeuro 18 May 2018, 5 (3) ENEURO.0013-18.2018; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0013-18.2018
Mariana M. Marquez
1Department of Physiology, McGill University, Montreal H3G1Y6, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Maurice J. Chacron
1Department of Physiology, McGill University, Montreal H3G1Y6, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Maurice J. Chacron
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Serotonergic innervation of sensory areas is found ubiquitously across the central nervous system of vertebrates. Here, we used a system’s level approach to investigate the role of serotonin on processing motion stimuli in the electrosensory system of the weakly electric fish Apteronotus albifrons. We found that exogenous serotonin application increased the firing activity of pyramidal neural responses to both looming and receding motion. Separating spikes belonging to bursts from those that were isolated revealed that this effect was primarily due to increased burst firing. Moreover, when investigating whether firing activity during stimulation could be discriminated from baseline (i.e., in the absence of stimulation), we found that serotonin increased stimulus discriminability only for some stimuli. This is because increased burst firing was most prominent for these. Further, the effects of serotonin were highly heterogeneous, with some neurons displaying large while others instead displaying minimal changes in responsiveness following serotonin application. Further analysis revealed that serotonin application had the greatest effect on neurons with low baseline firing rates and little to no effect on neurons with high baseline firing rates. Finally, the effects of serotonin on sensory neuron responses were largely independent of object velocity. Our results therefore reveal a novel function for the serotonergic system in selectively enhancing discriminability for motion stimuli.

  • burst firing
  • feedback
  • neuromodulation
  • serotonin
  • weakly electric fish

Significance Statement

Previous studies have suggested that the role of serotonin in the electrosensory system was to enhance perception of and neural responses to stimuli associated with same-sex conspecifics. However, these have focused on stationary stimuli. Here, we focused on motion stimuli that are typically associated with a different behavioral context (e.g., prey capture). Although exogenous serotonin application increased burst firing to both looming and receding motion, detectability was significantly enhanced only for receding motion that gave rise to a large excitatory response. We propose that serotonin selectively enhances neural responses to stimuli that elicit electrosensory feedback.

Introduction

Understanding how sensory input is processed by the brain to give rise to behavioral responses remains a central problem in systems neuroscience. Such understanding is complicated by the fact that sensory systems must constantly adapt to natural stimuli whose statistics vary in time (Wark et al., 2007; Sharpee et al., 2014). Such adaptation is thought to be achieved in part by neuromodulators such as serotonin (Hurley et al., 2004; Berger et al., 2009; Marder, 2012). Centrifugal serotonergic fibers emanate from the Raphe nuclei and innervate multiple sensory brain areas. Although evolutionary studies have shown remarkable conservation of this system across vertebrate species (Parent, 1981), previous studies have shown that serotonergic fibers make diverse connection patterns (Foehring et al., 2002; Thompson and Hurley, 2004), thereby causing a wide range of effects on neural activity (Waterhouse et al., 1990; Hurley and Pollak, 2001, 2005; Petzold et al., 2009; Hurley and Sullivan, 2012), and thereby suggesting that the functional role of serotonin is to selectively enhance/suppress neural responses to stimuli associated with different behavioral context(s). Significant insight as to the functional role of serotonergic pathways is likely to be gained by studying sensory systems with well-characterized neural circuits whose responses to natural stimuli associated with different behavioral contexts are well-understood.

The electrosensory system of weakly electric fish provides a convenient model system to study neuromodulation in vertebrate sensory systems because of well-characterized anatomy and physiology, as well as well-described natural stimuli that can easily be reproduced in the laboratory and will give rise to appropriate behavioral responses (Berman and Maler, 1999; Chacron et al., 2011; Márquez et al., 2013; Clarke et al., 2015a). These fish generate a quasi-sinusoidal signal called the electric organ discharge (EOD) around their body, which allows them to explore the environment and communicate with conspecifics. Peripheral electrosensory afferents detect changes in EOD amplitude and relay this information to pyramidal cells within the electrosensory lateral line lobe (ELL). Pyramidal cells also receive large amounts of feedback including neuromodulatory input (Sas and Maler, 1983, 1987; Johnston et al., 1990; Deemyad et al., 2011; Toscano-Márquez et al., 2013) whose functions comprise gain control (Bastian, 1986b), adaptive cancellation of redundant stimuli (Bastian, 1999; Bastian et al., 2004; Bol et al., 2011), and selective enhancement of neural responses (Ellis et al., 2007a; Deemyad et al., 2013). In particular, recent studies have focused on understanding the role of serotonergic projections onto ELL pyramidal cells: it was found that such input increases ELL pyramidal cell responsivity to stimuli associated with same-sex conspecifics (Deemyad et al., 2013) by increasing excitability through inhibition of potassium currents (Deemyad et al., 2011). Interestingly, another study has used in vivo voltammetry to measure serotonin levels in ELL: it was found that levels increased following stimulation associated with conspecifics (Fotowat et al., 2016). These studies suggest that the primary role of the serotonergic system is to facilitate neural processing of stimuli associated with social interactions. However, weakly electric fish must also electrolocate (e.g., find relevant objects such as prey) in their environment, which is associated with movement (Nelson and MacIver, 1999). While ELL pyramidal cell responses to lateral (Bastian, 1981; Chacron et al., 2009; Khosravi-Hashemi and Chacron, 2014) as well looming and receding motion (Clarke et al., 2014, 2015b; Clarke and Maler, 2017) have been studied, the effects of neuromodulatory input on these has not been investigated to date.

Here, we investigated the effects of serotonergic input on the responses of ELL pyramidal cells in response to looming and receding motion. Previous studies have extensively investigated ELL pyramidal cell responses to such motion (Clarke et al., 2014, 2015b; Clarke and Maler, 2017). In particular, it was found that objects that inhibit pyramidal cell activity during the looming phase of motion will cause a large burst of spikes during the receding phase (Clarke et al., 2014, 2015b). This receding response occurs even when the object is stationary for a few seconds and is generated by descending pathways while the response to looming motion is instead generated by feedforward pathways (Clarke and Maler, 2017). We found that exogenous serotonin application increased the firing activity of pyramidal cells during both the looming and receding phases of motion, which was due to increased burst firing. However, serotonin enhanced discriminability for receding motion only when this stimulus elicited a prominent excitatory response mainly generated by feedback inputs, independently of the object’s velocity. Our results thus provide the first experimental evidence that the serotonergic system is involved in increasing discriminability of neural responses to receding but not looming motion.

Materials and Methods

Animals and surgery

Specimens of the weakly electric fish Apteronotus albifrons were acquired from tropical fish suppliers and acclimated to laboratory conditions according to published guidelines (Hitschfeld et al., 2009). A total of 27 animals of either sex were used in these experiments. All animal procedures were performed in accordance with the institutional animal care committee’s regulations.

Surgical procedures have been described in detail elsewhere (Martinez et al., 2016; Hofmann and Chacron, 2017). Briefly, the fish was paralyzed by intramuscular injection of tubocurarine (1 µg/g; Sigma-Aldrich), placed in the recording tank and respirated with oxygenated water flowing at a constant rate of ∼ 10 ml/min. A portion of the animal’s head was kept out of water and anesthetized with topical application of lidocaine ointment (2%, Western Medical Supply). A small incision was made over the hindbrain and a metal post was glued to the most anterior section of the exposed skull to stabilize the animal’s position in space. A small craniotomy was then made to access the ELL.

Electrophysiology

The brain anatomy of A. albifrons is very similar to that of Apteronotus leptorhynchus (Maler, 1979, 1981; Maler et al., 1991). We recorded extracellularly record from ELL pyramidal cells (n = 27) using techniques similar to those used previously (Martinez et al., 2016). Based on recording depth as well as electrode placement relative to surface landmarks (e.g., To vein and its afferents; Krahe et al., 2008), it is likely that our recordings were from the lateral segment, although it is possible that some recordings were from the adjacent centro-lateral segment. Previous studies performed in A. leptorhynchus have shown that the lateral segment displayed the greatest density of serotonergic innervation (Deemyad et al., 2011). Recordings were made using electrodes filled with Woods Metal and plated with both gold and platinum (Frank and Becker, 1964). The electrodes tip diameter was typically ∼5 µm. All recordings were amplified (A-M Systems 1700), digitized at a 10-kHz sampling rate (CED 1401; Spike2 version 8.1 software; Cambridge Electronic Design), and stored for subsequent analysis.

Pharmacology

Glutamate (3 mM; Sigma-Aldrich) and serotonin (1 mM; Sigma-Aldrich) were dissolved in saline (111 mM NaCl, 2 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgSO4, 1 mM NaHCO3, and 0.5 mM NaH2PO4; Sigma-Aldrich) for application. Drug application electrodes were two-barrel KG-33 glass micropipettes (OD = 1.5 mm, ID = 0.86 mm, A-M Systems) pulled by a vertical micropipette puller (Stoelting) and subsequently broken to attain a final tip diameter of ∼ 10 µm. During recordings we used a picospritzer (Parker Hannifin) for separate delivering small puffs of glutamate or serotonin. We first used excitatory responses to glutamate to verify that we were in the vicinity of the cell being recorded from, as evidenced from a short latency (<2 ms) increase in spiking activity following drug application. Drugs were typically delivered at 15–25 psi during 150 ms, as done previously (Toporikova and Chacron, 2009; Deemyad et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2016). We note that previous studies have repeatedly shown that application of saline alone in this manner does not significantly alter ELL pyramidal cell activity (Bastian, 1993; Toporikova and Chacron, 2009; Deemyad et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2016).

Stimulation

Moving object

A. albifrons has a neurogenic electric organ which discharge (EOD) is not affected by immobilization with tubocurarine (Hitschfeld et al., 2009). As a consequence, the immobilized fish is still able to sense local perturbations in its EOD amplitude caused by objects with different conductivity than the surrounded water, such as plastic or metal. The stimulus consisted of a plastic or metal sphere (1.5 cm in diameter) controlled by a pen plotter (HP 7035B) and located at a given cell’s receptive field (RF) center (see section <On and off type cell classification>). The object’s trajectory was a sequence of looming motion toward the fish over a distance of 6 cm followed by a 2-s pause at 0.5 cm away from the skin surface and receding motion over the same distance followed by a 2-s pause at 6.5 cm away from the animal. The stimulation protocol consisted of 50 repetitions or trials played at four different velocities: 3, 6, 8, and 12 cm/s. These values were chosen to match the behaviorally relevant range observed during locomotion studies (Bastian, 1982; Rose and Canfield, 1993a,b; Nelson and MacIver, 1999; Cowan and Fortune, 2007).

ON and OFF type cell classification

Within the ELL, there are two types of pyramidal cells that can be classified based on their responses to stimuli (Bastian, 1981; Martinez et al., 2016): ON cells respond preferentially to increases in EOD amplitude while OFF cells respond preferentially to decreases in EOD amplitude. We used the same methodology as Martinez et al. (2016) to classify each pyramidal cell recorded from. Specifically, a noisy amplitude modulation stimulus (0–120 Hz) was played via two electrodes located 15 cm on each side of the animal. The spike-triggered average (STA) is the mean stimulus waveform that triggers an action potential and was obtained by averaging the stimulus waveforms within a 50-ms time window surrounding each spike:Embedded Image where S(t) is the time-varying noisy AM stimulus. The cell was classified as being ON-type if the slope of the STA within a time window of 10 ms centered at 7 ms was positive and classified as being OFF-type if the slope of the STA was negative. We note that the negative offset of 7 ms was used to account for the axonal transmission delay from the skin surface to the ELL (Chacron et al., 2003). RFs from ELL pyramidal cells were located using a local stimulus dipole and a 4-Hz sinusoidal amplitude modulation as done previously (Bastian et al., 2002; Chacron et al., 2009). Previous studies have shown that receding motion triggers paradoxical responses to electrosensory contrast (i.e., ON-type cells respond preferentially to receding motion of a negative contrast object while OFF-type cells respond preferentially to receding motion of a positive contrast object; Clarke et al., 2014). Recordings were made when stimulating with either of a matching or non-matching contrast paradigm. For the matching contrast paradigm (n = 9), we used a metal sphere when recoding from ON cells and a plastic sphere when recording from OFF cells as done previously (Clarke et al., 2015b; Clarke and Maler, 2017). For the non-matching contrast paradigm (n = 18), we used a metal sphere when recoding from OFF cells and a plastic sphere when recording from ON cells as done previously (Clarke et al., 2015b; Clarke and Maler, 2017).

Data analysis

All analysis was performed offline using Spike2 and custom written scripts in MATLAB (MathWorks). Action potential times were defined as the times for which the signal crossed a suitably chosen threshold.

Spontaneous activity

The spontaneous firing rate of every neuron was calculated from 100 s of activity before any stimulus was presented. All quantities are reported as mean ± SE.

Distinguishing bursts from isolated spikes

We used an algorithm to distinguish between spikes that belong to bursts (i.e., burst spikes) and those that do not (i.e., isolated spikes). Specifically, spikes are part of bursts if they are separated by a time interval that is less than the threshold and are considered isolated spikes otherwise. This algorithm has been used extensively before in the electrosensory system (Oswald et al., 2004; Avila-Akerberg et al., 2010; Khosravi-Hashemi et al., 2011; Khosravi-Hashemi and Chacron, 2012, 2014). We chose a threshold of 10 ms in accordance with these previous studies. Furthermore, this value corresponded to a well-defined mode of the interspike interval distribution under spontaneous activity for our dataset (Fig. 1). Burst fraction was computed as the ratio of the number of spikes that belong to bursts to the total number of spikes.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Effects of serotonin on spontaneous ELL pyramidal cell activity. A, Top, Interspike interval (ISI) histogram from a typical ELL pyramidal cell before serotonin application with the x-axis plotted on a logarithmic scale. Inset, ISI histogram from the same cell but with the x-axis plotted on a linear scale. Bottom, ISI histogram from the cell after serotonin application with the x-axis plotted on a logarithmic scale. Inset, ISI histogram from the same cell after serotonin application but with the x-axis plotted on a linear scale. Note that a clear bimodality can be seen from the main panels. The vertical gray lines indicate the chosen burst threshold of 10 ms. B, Change in burst fraction due to serotonin application averaged over our dataset as a function of the burst threshold. The solid line shows the population average and the gray band shows ±1 SEM.

Response to moving objects

Responses to moving objects were accumulated as sequences of time where action potentials occurred (spike times) and were converted to binary sequences by discretizing time into bins of 0.1 ms width. Peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were generated by building a histogram from the binary sequences of spike times, diving the values by the bin size, and then multiplying the result by the number of trials and smoothing with a 200-ms-long box car filter. We used receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis (Green and Swets, 1966) to quantify the ability of an ideal observer to distinguish between neural responses to either looming or receding motion stimuli and those obtained when the object was stationary and far away (6.5 cm) from the animal (i.e., baseline). Specifically, spike count distributions were obtained from neural responses over the course of the entire looming and receding phases of motion and compared to that obtained from baseline over the same time period. The probabilities of correct detection [P(correct)] and false alarm [P(false alarm)] were computing by integrating the spike count distributions up to a variable threshold. The ROC curve was then obtained by plotting P(correct) as a function of P(false alarm) while systematically varying the threshold. Detectability was quantified by computing the as the area under the ROC curve (auROC). A value of 1 for auROC indicates perfect discrimination while a value of 0.5 indicates chance level.

Statistics

Statistical significance was assessed through a Student’s t test or a Wilcoxon signed-rank test for paired measurements at the p = 0.05 level. For multiple comparisons, statistical significance was assessed through one-way ANOVA at the p = 0.05 level. Correlations were calculated using a Pearson correlation test or Spearman correlation test.

Results

Serotonin application increases pyramidal cell excitability and burst firing

We recorded extracellularly from ELL LS pyramidal neurons (n = 27) in A. albifrons and measured their responses to moving objects in awake behaving animals before and after exogenous serotonin application (Fig. 2A). Focal serotonin application was achieved by inserting a double-barrel electrode in the ELL molecular layer with one barrel containing glutamate and the other serotonin that was connected to a picrospritzer (Fig. 2A, right). Glutamate ejection was used to ascertain that the double barrel electrode was located near the apical dendritic tree of the pyramidal cell being recorded from. Previous studies performed in A. leptorhynchus have shown that, when using a non-matching contrast paradigm (i.e., stimulating ON cells with a plastic object and OFF cells with a metal object), ELL pyramidal neurons respond to receding motion by increasing their firing rate and more specifically, by firing more burst spikes (Clarke et al., 2014). Our results show that ELL pyramidal cells in A. albifrons respond to receding motion in a similar manner (Fig. 2B, top). Consistent with previous results obtained in A. leptorhynchus (Deemyad et al., 2013), we found that serotonin application increased pyramidal cell excitability for spontaneous activity (i.e., in the absence of stimulation but in the presence of the animal’s unmodulated EOD; Fig. 2C). Specifically, pyramidal cells displayed an increased tendency to fire packets of action potentials followed by quiescence (i.e., bursts), as revealed by separating the spike train into burst (Fig. 2C, magenta) and isolated spikes (Fig. 2C, cyan) using an interspike interval threshold criterion. Overall, serotonin application increased the cell’s tendency to fire bursts, as quantified by a large increase in burst fraction (i.e., the fraction of spikes that belong to bursts; Fig. 2D, left), which led to an increase in the overall firing rate (Fig. 2D, right).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Serotonin affects ELL pyramidal cell responses to moving objects through burst firing. A, Schematic representation of the experimental setup. The stimulus (plastic or metal sphere) is aligned to the RF of a given cell and follows a looming (dashed gray line) or a receding (solid gray line) trajectory while the cell’s response is recorded extracellularly. The inset shows the recording electrode that is placed near the cell and a double barrel pipette with glutamate and serotonin that is placed near the cell’s apical dendritic tree. B, Schematics showing one full stimulus cycle and the response of a representative neuron. In a non-matching contrast paradigm, during control condition (black trace) the neuron responds preferentially to receding motion with a group of action potentials occurring at a high frequency rate of discharge (i.e., a burst). This response is enhanced after serotonin application (red trace, compare insets). C, Spiking responses under control (black) and after (red) serotonin application. Shown are the full spike trains (brown), burst spikes (magenta), and isolated spikes (cyan). D, Population-averaged burst fraction (i.e., the fraction of spikes that belong to bursts; left) and mean firing rate (right) before (black) and after (red) serotonin application (n = 13). Stars indicate statistical significance using a signed-rank test (p = 0.0002) and a paired t test (t(12) = −3.4530, p = 0.005), respectively.

Serotonin enhances the firing rate through increased burst firing during all phases of stimulation

We next investigated the effects of serotonin on responses to looming and receding motion. While looming motion causes decreased spiking activity (Fig. 3A,B, dashed gray arrow), receding motion instead causes increased spiking activity (Fig. 3A,B, solid gray arrow). Overall, serotonin application gave rise to increased burst firing during all phases of stimulation (Fig. 3A,B). While there was some variability in responses to repeated stimulus presentations (Fig. 3A,B), responses showed strong similarity and were thus averaged to the PSTH. The PSTH response of a typical ELL pyramidal cell before and after serotonin application are shown in the bottom panel of Figure 3A. Overall, looming motion led to a decrease in firing rate which somewhat recovered while the object remains stationary and close to the animal (Fig. 3A, dashed gray arrow). There was a sharp increase in firing rate following the onset of receding motion (Fig. 3A, solid gray arrow) that then slowly adapted back toward the baseline value while the object remained stationary and far away from the animal.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Serotonin promotes burst firing in response to motion stimuli during a non-matching contrast paradigm. A, Top, object position (lateral distance to the animal’s skin surface) as a function of time. The object’s speed during both looming and receding motion was 8 cm/s. Middle, Raster plot showing an example ELL pyramidal neuron’s spiking response to 25 stimulus presentations (i.e., trials) during control condition. The spikes that belong to bursts are shown in magenta, whereas isolated spikes are shown in cyan. Bottom, PSTH of the neuron computed from 50 trials using all spikes. B, Top, raster plot showing the example neuron’s response to 25 trials after serotonin application. Bottom, PSTH computed from 50 trials using all spikes.

Serotonin application led to an overall increase in the average firing rate of ELL pyramidal cell during all phases of stimulation (Fig. 4A, bottom panel). Since our results have shown that the effect of serotonin was to increase burst firing during baseline conditions, we hypothesized that increased responses were due to increases in the tendency to fire bursts during receding motion. To test this hypothesis, we only considered spikes that were part of bursts (i.e., burst spikes) or not (i.e., isolated spikes) to compute the PSTH. Our results confirmed our hypothesis in that the PSTHs obtained from burst spikes but not isolated spikes showed a large increase in firing during the receding phase of motion (Fig. 4B,C). These results were seen across our dataset, as serotonin increased the burst fraction during both looming and receding motion, as well as when the object was stationary and far away from the animal (i.e., “baseline”; Fig. 5A,B). However, the increase in burst fraction during looming motion and during baseline were similar, while the increase during receding motion was more substantial (Fig. 5B).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Serotonin increases the firing rate during all phases of stimulation. A, Top, object position (lateral distance to the animal’s skin surface) as a function of time. The object’s speed during both looming and receding motion was 8 cm/s. Bottom, PSTH from an example ELL pyramidal neuron computed from all spikes before (black) and after (red) serotonin application. There was a decrease in firing rate during the looming phase (dashed arrow). The firing rate reached its maximum value right after the onset of receding motion during both control (black trace) and after serotonin application (red trace). Note that the firing rate was overall higher during all stimulus phases after serotonin application. B, PSTH from the example ELL pyramidal neuron computed from burst spikes before (black) and after (red) serotonin application. C, PSTH from the example neuron computed using isolated spikes before (black) and after (red) serotonin application.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Serotonin increases stimulus detectability for receding but not for looming motion. A, Object position (lateral distance to the animal’s skin surface) as a function of time. The neural responses during looming (dashed gray box) and receding (middle solid gray box) gray boxes were compared to the baseline activity while the object was stationary and located far away from the animal (right gray box). B, Population-averaged burst fractions before (black) and after (red) serotonin application during looming (left), receding (middle), and baseline (right). Stars indicate statistical significance using a paired t test (looming: t(12) = −2.70, p = 0.03, n = 13; receding: t(12) = −3.35, p = 0.009, n = 13; baseline: t(12) = −3.08, p = 0.01, n = 13). C, Top, spike count distributions obtained during baseline (gray) and during receding stimulation (black or red) before (left) and after (right) serotonin application for an example ELL pyramidal cell. Best-fit Gaussian curves are superimposed on each distribution. Bottom, ROC curves from this same example neuron before (black) and after (red) serotonin application. D, Same as C but for looming motion. E, Population-averaged values for the auROC before (black) and after (red) serotonin application for receding motion. The horizontal brown line indicates the chance level. A significant increase was observed after serotonin application (signed-rank test, p = 0.0007, n = 13). F, Same as E but for looming motion. No significant change was observed (t test, t(12) = 1.08, p = 0.301, n = 13).

Serotonin application increases the detectability of receding but not looming motion when using a non-matching contrast paradigm

It is important to note that, to be detected, sensory input must perturb the ongoing activity of ELL pyramidal cells in the absence of stimulation. Thus, we next used ROC analysis to test whether serotonin affected the discriminability of looming and receding motion. Specifically, spike count distributions obtained during looming and receding stimulation were compared to that obtained during the baseline (Fig. 5A). The upper panels of Figure 5C show the spike count distributions from an example cell before (upper left) and after (upper right) serotonin application. It is seen that the distributions are more separable after serotonin application, which is reflected in the ROC curve (Fig. 5C, middle panel). Overall, serotonin application significantly increased receding motion stimulus detectability as quantified by computing the auROC (Fig. 5C, bottom panel). For comparison, spike count distributions obtained during looming motion are shown in the upper panel of Figure 5D. It is seen that serotonin application did not change the discriminability between the spike count distributions (Fig. 5D, compare left and right upper panels), which is reflected in the ROC curves (Fig. 5D, middle panel). Overall, serotonin application did not affect looming motion stimulus detectability as the auROC values obtained before and after serotonin application were not significantly different from one another (Fig. 5D, bottom panel).

Comparison of Figure 5B,C suggests that increased stimulus detectability during receding motion stimulation was due to a stronger increase in burst firing than during baseline. To test this hypothesis, we separated the spike trains during receding motion stimulation and during baseline into bursts and isolated spikes and then applied ROC analysis to both. Our results confirmed our hypothesis, as serotonin application significantly increased the discriminability of the burst spike count distributions (Fig. 6A, compare left and middle panels), as reflected in the ROC curves (Fig. 6A, right panel). In contrast, serotonin application did not affect the discriminability of the isolated spike count distributions (Fig. 6B, compare left and middle panels), as reflected in the ROC curves (Fig. 6B, right panel). Across our dataset, receding motion stimulus detectability as quantified by the auROC was significantly increased after serotonin application when considering bursts but not when considering isolated spikes (Fig. 6C).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Serotonin increases receding motion stimulus detectability through enhanced burst firing. A, Left, spike count distributions obtained during baseline (gray) and during receding stimulation (black) before serotonin application. Middle, spike count distributions obtained during baseline (gray) and during receding stimulation (red) after serotonin application. Right, ROC curves before (black) and after (red) serotonin application. We used the same example ELL pyramidal cell as in Figure 4. B, Same as A but for isolated spikes. C, Population-averaged auROC values for bursts (left) and isolated spikes (right) before (black) and after (red) serotonin application. A significant increase was observed for burst but not for isolated spikes (burst spikes: signed-rank test, p =.0134; isolated spikes: t test, t(12) = 0.34, p = 0.737, n = 13).

Serotonin application does not alter detectability when using a matching contrast paradigm

We further tested the effects of serotonin on responses to motion using a matching contrast paradigm (i.e., ON cells were stimulated with a metal object while OFF cells were stimulated with a plastic object). We found that looming motion caused a strong increase in spiking activity (Fig. 7A,B, dashed gray arrow), while receding motion instead caused decreased spiking activity (Fig. 7A,B, solid gray arrow). Serotonin application significantly increased burst firing during all phases of motion (Fig. 8A,B), which is consistent with results obtained above using the non-matching contrast paradigm. However, we found that enhanced burst firing during either of looming or receding motion did not lead to enhanced detectability (Fig. 8C–F). This is most likely due to the fact that increases in burst firing during both looming and receding motion were similar to those observed during baseline activity (Fig. 8B).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Serotonin enhances burst firing during a matching contrast paradigm. A, Top, object position (lateral distance to the animal’s skin surface) as a function of time. The object’s speed during both looming and receding motion was 8 cm/s. Middle, Raster plot showing an example ELL pyramidal neuron’s spiking response to 25 stimulus presentations (i.e., trials) during control condition. The spikes that belong to bursts are shown in magenta whereas isolated spikes are shown in cyan. Bottom, PSTH of the neuron computed from 50 trials using all spikes. B, Top, raster plot showing the example neuron’s response to 25 trials after serotonin application. Bottom, PSTH computed from 50 trials using all spikes.

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Serotonin does not alter stimulus detectability during at matching contrast paradigm. A, Same as in Figure 4A, object position during a matching contrast paradigm. B, Population-averaged burst fractions before (black) and after (red) serotonin application during looming (left), receding (middle), and baseline (right). Stars indicate statistical significance using a signed-rank test (looming: p = 0.0391, n = 9; receding: p = 0.0273, n = 9; baseline: p = 0.0039, n = 9). C, Top, spike count distributions obtained during baseline (gray) and during looming stimulation (black or red) before (left) and after (right) serotonin application for an example ELL pyramidal cell. Best-fit Gaussian curves are superimposed on each distribution. Bottom, ROC curves from this example neuron before (black) and after (red) serotonin application. D, Same as C but for receding motion. E, Population-averaged auROC before (black) and after (red) serotonin application corresponding to looming motion. The horizontal brown line indicates the chance level. The observed decrease after serotonin application was not significant (signed-rank, p = 0.25, n = 9). F, Same as E but for receding motion. No significant change was observed (signed-rank, p = 0.7344, n = 9).

The effects of serotonin application are negatively correlated with the cell’s spontaneous firing rate

We found that the effects of serotonin were heterogeneous as we observed strong effects for cells with lower spontaneous firing rates (Fig. 9A) and weaker effects for cells with higher spontaneous firing rates (Fig. 9B). Indeed, there was a strong negative correlation between the changes in burst fraction as well as firing rate due to serotonin application, and the cell’s spontaneous firing rate before serotonin application (Fig. 9C,D, respectively). Moreover, while there was no significant correlation between the relative change in detectability and the spontaneous firing rate for looming motion (Fig. 9E), we observed a significant correlation between both quantities for receding motion (Fig. 9F), when using a non-matching contrast paradigm. These results therefore confirm our hypothesis that the effect of serotonin is greatest in cells with low spontaneous firing rates and weakest in cells with high spontaneous firing rates.

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

The magnitude of the effect of serotonin on ELL pyramidal cell’s responses to moving objects was negatively correlated with spontaneous firing rate. A, PSTH of a representative cell with low (9.1 Hz) spontaneous firing rate during control (black) and after serotonin application (red). B, Same as A but for a representative cell with high (28.9 Hz) spontaneous firing rate. For both panels, the time scale is the same and the object’s speed during looming and receding motion was 8 cm/s. C, Percentage change in burst fraction [i.e., 100*(BFserotonin – BFcontrol)/BFcontrol] between control and serotonin conditions (measured during baseline period) as a function of spontaneous firing rate measured before serotonin application. Both quantities were significantly correlated (r = −0.68, p = 0.003, n = 15). D, Relative change in firing rate [i.e., 100*(FRserotonin – FRcontrol)/FRcontrol] as a function of the spontaneous firing rate. Both quantities were significantly correlated (r = −0.54, p = 0.02, n = 15). E, Percentage change in auROC [i.e., 100*(auROCserotonin − auROCcontrol)/auROCcontrol] as a function of spontaneous firing rate for looming motion. No significant correlation was observed (r = −0.06, p = 0.84, n = 15). F, Same as E but for receding motion. Both quantities were significantly correlated (r = −0.6, p = 0.03, n = 15).

Effects of serotonin on ELL pyramidal cell responses to motion stimuli are speed invariant

Finally, we investigated the effects of varying the speed of the moving object during both looming and receding motion when using a non-matching contrast paradigm. We tested velocities of 3, 6, 8, and 12 cm/s that are within the behaviorally relevant range (Bastian, 1982; Rose and Canfield, 1993a,b; Nelson and MacIver, 1999; Cowan and Fortune, 2007). The temporal profiles of the different stimuli are shown in Figure 10A. The responses of a typical ELL pyramidal cell before (black) and after (red) serotonin application are shown in Figure 10B for different object velocities. Overall, similar profiles were observed for each velocity in that the looming motion caused a decrease in firing rate that somewhat recovered while the object remained close to the animal. Receding motion furthermore caused a large increase in firing rate for all object velocities. The effects of serotonin were furthermore largely independent of object velocity in that there was an overall increase in firing rate and a large increase in the response to receding motion (Fig. 10B).

Figure 10.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 10.

Serotonergic modulation of ELL pyramidal cell activity to motion is speed invariant. A, Object position as a function of time. The object was moved at four different velocities: 3 cm/s (green), 6 cm/s (purple), 8 cm/s (pink), and 12 cm/s (orange). B, PSTH of a representative neuron for each stimulus speed. All the curves follow the same shape with a strong increase in firing rate right after the onset of receding motion and inhibition right at the end of looming motion. Note that the firing rate is higher after serotonin application (red trace) than during control condition (black trace).

We then quantified the effects of serotonin on both looming and receding motion stimulus detectability using ROC analysis. Overall, we found that auROC values for looming motion stimuli before and after serotonin application were not significantly different from one another (3 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.69 ± 0.03; serotonin auROC = 0.71 ± 0.04; paired t test, t(11) = −0.7010, p = 0.4979, n = 12; 6 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.65 ± 0.03; serotonin auROC = 0.68 ± 0.04; paired t test, t(11) = −0.8437, p = 0.4153, n = 12; 8 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.63 ± 0.02; serotonin auROC = 0.60 ± 0.02; paired t test, t(12) = 1.0812, p = 0.3009, n = 13; 12 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.64 ± 0.03; serotonin auROC = 0.63 ± 0.03; paired t test, t(11) = 0.2727, p = 0.7902, n = 12). Overall, these values were not significantly different when comparing across the different speeds (one-way ANOVA, F(3,12) = 0.84, p = 0.4768). In contrast, auROC values for receding motion stimuli significantly increased after serotonin application for all speeds (3 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.83 ± 0.04; serotonin auROC = 0.90 ± 0.04; paired t test, t(11) = −2.4420, p = 0.0327, n = 12; 6 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.90 ± 0.04; serotonin auROC = 0.97 ± 0.01; signed-rank test, p = 0.0522, n = 12; 8 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.86 ± 0.03; serotonin auROC = 0.97 ± 0.01; signed-rank test, p = 0.0007, n = 12; 12 cm/s: control: auROC = 0.86 ± 0.04; serotonin auROC = 0.98 ± 0.01; signed-rank test, p = 0.0020, n = 12). However, these values were not significantly different when comparing across the different speeds (one-way ANOVA, F(3,12) = 0.43, p = 0.7304).

Discussion

Summary of results

We investigated the effects of serotonin on processing of looming and motion stimuli by ELL pyramidal cells. Overall, focal exogenous serotonin application increased pyramidal excitability, thereby leading to increases in firing rate as well as the tendency to fire bursts of action potentials during both looming and receding motion stimulation, as well as during the absence of stimulation and baseline. When using a non-matching contrast paradigm, we found that serotonin application increased detectability of receding but not looming motion stimuli because increases in burst firing were more pronounced during receding motion stimulation than during looming stimulation or baseline. Instead, when using a matching contrast paradigm, we found that serotonin application did not alter detectability of either of receding or looming motion because increases in burst firing during receding motion stimulation were similar to those occurring during looming stimulation or baseline. The effects of serotonin were greatest for pyramidal cells with low spontaneous firing rates and weakest for cells with high spontaneous firing rates. Finally, we showed that the effects of serotonin on responses to both looming and receding motion stimuli were robust as they were independent of object speed.

Physiologic mechanisms by which serotonin enhances responses to receding motion

What are the underlying mechanisms by which serotonin enhances burst firing? The great similarity between the brain anatomies of A. albifrons and A. leptorhynchus, electrophysiological results showing largely similar ELL pyramidal cell neural responses (Bastian, 1981; Krahe et al., 2008; Khosravi-Hashemi and Chacron, 2014; Martinez et al., 2016), together with our results showing that serotonin application has an effect on pyramidal cell excitability that is similar to that observed previously for A. leptorhynchus (Deemyad et al., 2013), all strongly suggest that the mode of action of serotonin is shared between both species. In A. leptorhynchus, serotonin enhances pyramidal cell excitability in vitro by inhibiting outward currents mediated by SK and M-type channels (Deemyad et al., 2011), an effect that is mediated by 5-HT2-like receptors (Larson et al., 2014). Both currents give rise to a pronounced afterhyperpolarization (AHP) following an action potential, which opposes burst firing (Toporikova and Chacron, 2009; Deemyad et al., 2011). Strong attenuation of the AHP by serotonin allows for increased burst firing, which is mediated by a somato-dendritic interaction (Lemon and Turner, 2000). Specifically, somatic action potentials backpropagate into the proximal apical dendrites where they trigger a wider dendritic spike that propagates back to the soma, leading to a depolarizing after potential (DAP). The DAP at the soma grows in size throughout the burst, which leads to a progressive depolarization and a shortening of the interspike interval throughout the burst. The burst then terminates with a characteristic doublet when the interspike interval becomes shorter than the dendritic refractory period (Noonan et al., 2003). Indeed, intracellular recordings performed in vivo in A. leptorhynchus have shown that serotonin application decreases the AHP after the action potential, thereby revealing the DAP (Deemyad et al., 2013). Although further studies are needed to confirm this, it is very likely that serotonin application increases ELL pyramidal cell excitability in A. albifrons by inhibiting currents mediated by SK and M-type channels.

How does inhibition of SK and M-type currents lead to selective enhancement of detectability of receding stimuli when using a non-matching contrast paradigm? In A. leptorhynchus, only the SK1 and SK2 channel subtypes are expressed in the ELL (Ellis et al., 2007b, 2008), with SK1 channels expressed on the dendrites of ON and OFF-type ELL pyramidal cells and SK2 channels expressed near the somata of ON-type cells only (for review, see Huang and Chacron, 2017). Since the effects of serotonin on ELL pyramidal cell excitability were similar for both ON- and OFF-type cells (Deemyad et al., 2013), it is thought that serotonin primarily inhibits SK1 channels, which is supported by anatomic results showing rich density of serotonergic fibers within the molecular layer (Deemyad et al., 2011) where pyramidal cell apical dendrites are located. Previous results obtained in A. leptorhynchus have shown that, when using a non-matching contrast paradigm, the increased firing rate response to receding motion was mediated by descending input from higher centers (Clarke and Maler, 2017). Instead, when using a matching contrast paradigm, Clarke and Maler (2017) have shown that excitatory responses were mediated primarily by ascending input from EA’s. ELL pyramidal cells receive large amounts of descending input that terminates within the molecular layer (Sas and Maler, 1983, 1987; Berman and Maler, 1999) and whose functions include gain control (Bastian, 1986a,b) as well as cancelation of self-generated and low frequency stimuli generated by conspecifics (Bastian, 1999; Bastian et al., 2004; Bol et al., 2011; Mejias et al., 2013). In contrast, ascending input from EA’s terminates on the basilar dendrites and somata of ELL pyramidal cells (Maler, 1979; Maler et al., 1981). As such, it is likely that the mechanism by which detectability of receding motion is enhanced by serotonin in A. albifrons is through inhibition of dendritic SK1 (as well as possibly M-type) channels, which suppresses the underlying AHP and enhances burst firing more effectively in response to descending rather than in response to ascending input. We hypothesize that this is because synapses from descending pathways are located closer to SK1 (and possible M-type) channels. If true, then this would explain our results showing that serotonin application selectively increased detectability during receding motion when using a non-matching contrast paradigm. Further studies are needed to verify these predictions.

We also note that previous studies in A. leptorhynchus have shown a strong correlation between pyramidal cell morphology and spontaneous firing rate (Bastian and Nguyenkim, 2001; Bastian et al., 2004). Indeed, cells with the lowest spontaneous firing rates tend to display the largest apical dendrites and receive the largest amount of feedback, while cells with the highest spontaneous firing rates instead tend to display small apical dendrites and receive weak if non-existent feedback (for review, see Maler, 2009). Interestingly, cells with the highest spontaneous firing rates tend to display weak SK channel expression, whereas cells with the lowest spontaneous firing rates tend to display the strongest SK channel expression (Ellis et al., 2007b, 2008; for review, see Huang and Chacron, 2017). As such, our results showing that serotonin had the strongest effect on cells with low spontaneous firing rates and minimal to no effect on the activity of cells with high spontaneous firing rates in A. albifrons is consistent with the hypothesis that serotonin downregulates SK1 channels, which are most expressed in cells with low spontaneous firing rates. Further studies are however needed to confirm this.

Functional role of serotonergic pathways in the electrosensory system

What is the function of the serotonergic input to ELL pyramidal cells? One of the main effects of serotonin is to decrease aggressive behaviors across species (Fachinelli et al., 1989; Montoya et al., 2012) including weakly electric fish (Maler and Ellis, 1987; Deemyad et al., 2013). Importantly, serotonin levels are higher in more submissive individuals than in dominant ones (Larson and Summers, 2001). While it has been known for a long time that ELL pyramidal cells receive serotonergic input from the raphe nuclei (Johnston et al., 1990), the functional role of such input has remained largely unknown until recently. Deemyad et al. (2013) have shown that both exogenous and endogenous release of serotonin gave rise to increased responses to stimuli associated with same-sex conspecifics in A. leptorhynchus (Deemyad et al., 2013). Specifically, because of sex differences, females in this species tend to display lower EOD frequencies than males (Zakon et al., 2002). As such, the beat frequency (which is given by the difference in EOD frequency) tends to be lower during encounters between two males or two females than between a male and a female. Moreover, A. leptorhynchus also generate communication signals called “chirps” that consist of brief increases in EOD frequency that mainly consist of two types: Type I chirps that are elicited primarily during male-female interactions and Type II chirps that are elicited primarily during male-male interactions (Zakon et al., 2002). Interestingly, Deemyad et al. (2013) showed that serotonin application only enhanced ELL pyramidal neural responses to low frequency (<50 Hz) beats when these were spatially diffuse and Type II chirps. However, responses to spatially localized beat stimuli and to Type I chirps were unaffected. They therefore concluded that the function of the serotonergic pathway was to selectively enhance neural responses to stimuli associated with same-sex conspecifics in more submissive individuals. While the results of Deemyad et al. (2013) were obtained in A. leptorhynchus, the arguments made above strongly suggest that these will also apply to A. albifrons.

However, weakly electric fish also use their electrosense to detect objects in their environment. Behavioral studies have shown important looming and receding motion during prey capture behavior (Nelson and MacIver, 1999) as well as during exploration of novel objects (Graff et al., 2004; Hofmann et al., 2013). Our results showing that serotonin increases the detectability of receding motion stimuli suggests that serotonin will also affect responses to moving stimuli. The dense serotonergic innervation of ELL pyramidal cells within LS could thus be beneficial for determining the object’s location in 3D relative to that of the animal. The results of theoretical studies showing that cells with large RFs are beneficial for object localization in three dimensions (Brown and Bäcker, 2006), as observed for LS pyramidal cells (Shumway, 1989), supports our hypothesis. We propose that higher serotonin levels observed for more submissive individuals will also be beneficial for electrolocation in general. Such studies will require measuring serotonin levels in the ELL (e.g., using in vivo voltammetry) as done recently (Fotowat et al., 2016) of both submissive and dominant individuals and are beyond the scope of this paper. In general, our results suggest that the functional role of serotonergic pathways is to enhance ELL pyramidal neuron responses to descending input. As such, the detectability of a stimulus for which the response is at least partially mediated by feedback input should then be enhanced. Recent evidence suggests that responses to spatially diffuse Type II chirps (Marsat and Maler, 2012) as well as low frequency beats are mediated by descending input in A. leptorhynchus (Bastian et al., 2004; Chacron et al., 2005; Chacron, 2006). In contrast, both pharmacological inactivation and lesion of descending input have no significant effect on either high frequency beats or spatially localized stationary stimuli (Chacron et al., 2005; Chacron, 2006). We note that the effects of descending input on Type I chirps have not been investigated systematically to date. However, current experimental data supports our hypothesis that the function of the serotonergic system is to enhance ELL pyramidal cell responses to stimuli that elicit feedback input. Further studies should focus on how serotonin affects responses to other stimuli that have not been considered before, such as changes in the beat amplitude (i.e., the envelope) that occur during movement that have been the focus of recent studies (Huang and Chacron, 2016; Huang et al., 2016; Martinez et al., 2016; Zhang and Chacron, 2016).

Implications for other sensory systems

Previous studies have shown strong similarities between the electrosensory system and the visual, auditory, and vestibular systems of mammals (Clarke et al., 2015a; Metzen et al., 2015). Together with the fact that the serotonergic system displays high conservation across vertebrate species (Parent, 1981), this suggests that our results will be applicable to other systems. In particular, as the ELL is a cerebellar-like structure, it is likely that our results will be applicable to other cerebellar-like structures such as the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN) that also receive large serotonergic innervation. A recent study is shown that serotonin application increases the excitability of DCN neurons via 5-HT2 receptors but had heterogeneous effects on neural responses to stimuli (Felix et al., 2017). We hypothesize that the function of the serotonergic input to DCN will also be to enhance neural responses to stimuli that elicit descending input. Recent evidence has shown that descending input, which is found ubiquitously in the central nervous system (Cajal, 1909; Holländer, 1970; Ostapoff et al., 1990; Sherman and Guillery, 2002), plays key roles in determining neural responses to sensory input (Manita et al., 2015; Kwon et al., 2016; Takahashi et al., 2016). Further studies are needed to understand how neuromodulators affect processing by modulating how sensory neurons integrate descending input.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This work was supported by The Canadian Institutes of Health Research (M.J.C.).

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Avila-Akerberg O, Krahe R, Chacron MJ (2010) Neural heterogeneities and stimulus properties affect burst coding in vivo. Neuroscience 168:300–313. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.03.012 pmid:20298764
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Bastian J (1981) Electrolocation. II. The effects of moving objects and other electrical stimuli on the activities of two categories of posterior lateral line lobe cells in Apteronotus albifrons . J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 144:481–494.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    Bastian J (1982) Vision and electroreception. Integration of sensory information in the optic tectum of the weakly electric fish Apteronotus Albifrons. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 147:287–297. doi:10.1007/BF00609662
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    Bastian J (1986a) Gain control in the electrosensory system mediated by descending inputs to the electrosensory lateral line lobe. J Neurosci 6:553–562. pmid:3950710
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    Bastian J (1986b) Gain control in the electrosensory system. A role for descending projections to the lateral electrosensory lateral line lobe. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 158:505–515. doi:10.1007/BF00603796
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Bastian J (1993) The role of amino acid neurotransmitters in the descending control of electroreception. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 172:409–423. pmid:7686228
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Bastian J (1999) Plasticity of feedback inputs in the apteronotid electrosensory system. J Exp Biol 202:1327–1337. pmid:10210673
    OpenUrlAbstract
  8. ↵
    Bastian J, Nguyenkim J (2001) Dendritic modulation of burst-like firing in sensory neurons. J Neurophysiol 85:10–22. doi:10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.10 pmid:11152701
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Bastian J, Chacron MJ, Maler L (2002) Receptive field organization determines pyramidal cell stimulus-encoding capability and spatial stimulus selectivity. J Neurosci 22:4577–4590. pmid:20026423 pmid:12040065
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Bastian J, Chacron MJ, Maler L (2004) Plastic and non-plastic cells perform unique roles in a network capable of adaptive redundancy reduction. Neuron 41:767–779. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00071-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Berger M, Gray JA, Roth BL (2009) The expanded biology of serotonin. Annu Rev Med 60:355–366. doi:10.1146/annurev.med.60.042307.110802 pmid:19630576
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Berman NJ, Maler L (1999) Neural architecture of the electrosensory lateral line lobe: adaptations for coincidence detection, a sensory searchlight and frequency-dependent adaptive filtering. J Exp Biol 202:1243–1253. pmid:10210665
    OpenUrlAbstract
  13. ↵
    Bol K, Marsat G, Harvey-Girard E, Longtin A, Maler L (2011) Frequency-tuned cerebellar channels and burst-induced LTD lead to the cancellation of redundant sensory inputs. J Neurosci 31:11028–11038. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0193-11.2011 pmid:21795551
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Brown WM, Bäcker A (2006) Optimal neuronal tuning for finite stimulus spaces. Neural Comput 18:1511–1526. doi:10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1511 pmid:16764512
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Cajal RS (1909) Histologie du système nerveux de l'homme et des vertébrés. Paris: Maloine.
  16. ↵
    Chacron MJ (2006) Nonlinear information processing in a model sensory system. J Neurophysiol 95:2933–2946. doi:10.1152/jn.01296.2005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Chacron MJ, Doiron B, Maler L, Longtin A, Bastian J (2003) Non-classical receptive field mediates switch in a sensory neuron's frequency tuning. Nature 423:77–81. doi:10.1038/nature01684
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Chacron MJ, Maler L, Bastian J (2005) Feedback and feedforward control of frequency tuning to naturalistic stimuli. J Neurosci 25:5521–5532. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0445-05.2005 pmid:15944380
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Chacron MJ, Toporikova N, Fortune ES (2009) Differences in the time course of short-term depression across receptive fields are correlated with directional selectivity in electrosensory neurons. J Neurophysiol 102:3270–3279. doi:10.1152/jn.00645.2009 pmid:19793877
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Chacron MJ, Longtin A, Maler L (2011) Efficient computation via sparse coding in electrosensory neural networks. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:752–760. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.016 pmid:21683574
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Clarke SE, Maler L (2017) Feedback synthesizes neural codes for motion. Curr Biol 27:1356–1361. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.068 pmid:28457872
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Clarke SE, Longtin A, Maler L (2014) A neural code for looming and receding motion is distributed over a population of electrosensory ON and OFF contrast cells. J Neurosci 34:5583–5594. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4988-13.2014 pmid:24741048
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    Clarke SE, Longtin A, Maler L (2015a) Contrast coding in the electrosensory system: parallels with visual computation. Nat Rev Neurosci 16:733–744. doi:10.1038/nrn4037
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Clarke SE, Longtin A, Maler L (2015b) The neural dynamics of sensory focus. Nat Commun 6:8764. doi:10.1038/ncomms9764 pmid:26549346
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Cowan NJ, Fortune ES (2007) The critical role of locomotion mechanics in decoding sensory systems. J Neurosci 27:1123–1128. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4198-06.2007 pmid:17267567
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Deemyad T, Maler L, Chacron MJ (2011) Inhibition of SK and M channel-mediated currents by 5-HT enables parallel processing by bursts and isolated spikes. J Neurophysiol 105:1276–1294. doi:10.1152/jn.00792.2010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Deemyad T, Metzen MG, Pan Y, Chacron MJ (2013) Serotonin selectively enhances perception and sensory neural responses to stimuli generated by same-sex conspecifics. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:19609–19614. doi:10.1073/pnas.1314008110
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Ellis LD, Krahe R, Bourque CW, Dunn RJ, Chacron MJ (2007a) Muscarinic receptors control frequency tuning through the downregulation of an A-type potassium current. J Neurophysiol 98:1526–1537. doi:10.1152/jn.00564.2007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Ellis LD, Mehaffey WH, Harvey-Girard E, Turner RW, Maler L, Dunn RJ (2007b) SK channels provide a novel mechanism for the control of frequency tuning in electrosensory neurons. J Neurosci 27:9491–9502. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1106-07.2007
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    Ellis LD, Maler L, Dunn RJ (2008) Differential distribution of SK channel subtypes in the brain of the weakly electric fish Apteronotus leptorhynchus. J Comp Neur 507:1964–1978. doi:10.1002/cne.21597 pmid:18273887
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Fachinelli C, Sargo S, Bataller R, Rodríguez Echandía EL (1989) Effect of 5-HTP and ketanserine on the aggressive reaction induced by food competition in dominant and submissive pigeons (Columba livia). Behav Brain Res 35:265–270. pmid:2597343
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Felix RA 2nd, Elde CJ, Nevue AA, Portfors CV (2017) Serotonin modulates response properties of neurons in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of the mouse. Hear Res 344:13–23. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.017 pmid:27838373
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Foehring RC, van Brederode JF, Kinney GA, Spain WJ (2002) Serotonergic modulation of supragranular neurons in rat sensorimotor cortex. J Neurosci 22:8238–8250. pmid:12223578
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Fotowat H, Harvey-Girard E, Cheer JF, Krahe R, Maler L (2016) Subsecond sensory-evoked serotonin dynamics and its relation to ongoing communication behavior. eNeuro 3.
  35. ↵
    Frank K, Becker MC (1964) Microelectrodes for recording and stimulation. In: Physical techniques in biological research (Nastuk WL, ed), pp 23-84. New York: Academic.
  36. ↵
    Graff C, Kaminski G, Gresty M, Ohlmann T (2004) Fish perform spatial pattern recognition and abstraction by exclusive use of active electrolocation. Curr Biol 14:818–823. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.039 pmid:15120076
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Green D, Swets J (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
  38. ↵
    Hitschfeld EM, Stamper SA, Vonderschen K, Fortune ES, Chacron MJ (2009) Effects of restraint and immobilization on electrosensory behaviors of weakly electric fish. ILAR J 50:361–372. pmid:19949252
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Hofmann V, Chacron MJ (2017) Differential receptive field organizations give rise to nearly identical neural correlations across three parallel sensory maps in weakly electric fish. PLoS Comput Biol 13:e1005716. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005716 pmid:28863136
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Hofmann V, Sanguinetti-Scheck JI, Künzel S, Geurten B, Gómez-Sena L, Engelmann J (2013) Sensory flow shaped by active sensing: sensorimotor strategies in electric fish. J Exp Biol 216:2487–2500. doi:10.1242/jeb.082420 pmid:23761474
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Holländer H (1970) The projection from the visual cortex to the lateral geniculate body (LGB). An experimental study with silver impregnation methods in the cat. Exp Brain Res 10:219–235. doi:10.1007/BF00235047
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Huang CG, Chacron MJ (2016) Optimized parallel coding of second-order stimulus features by heterogeneous neural populations. J Neurosci 36:9859–9872. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1433-16.2016 pmid:27656024
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Huang CG, Chacron MJ (2017) SK channel subtypes enable parallel optimized coding of behaviorally relevant stimulus attributes: a review. Channels (Austin) 11:281–304. doi:10.1080/19336950.2017.1299835 pmid:28277938
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Huang CG, Zhang ZD, Chacron MJ (2016) Temporal decorrelation by SK channels enables efficient neural coding and perception of natural stimuli. Nat Commun 7:11353. doi:10.1038/ncomms11353 pmid:27088670
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Hurley LM, Pollak GD (2001) Serotonin effects on frequency tuning of inferior colliculus neurons. J Neurophysiol 85:828–842. doi:10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.828 pmid:11160516
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Hurley LM, Pollak GD (2005) Serotonin shifts first-spike latencies of inferior colliculus neurons. J Neurosci 25:7876–7886. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1178-05.2005 pmid:16120790
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    Hurley LM, Sullivan MR (2012) From behavioral context to receptors: serotonergic modulatory pathways in the IC. Front Neural Circuits 6:58. doi:10.3389/fncir.2012.00058 pmid:22973195
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Hurley LM, Devilbiss DM, Waterhouse BD (2004) A matter of focus: monoaminergic modulation of stimulus coding in mammalian sensory networks. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:488–495. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.06.007 pmid:15321070
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Johnston SA, Maler L, Tinner B (1990) The distribution of serotonin in the brain of Apteronotus leptorhynchus: an immunohistochemical study. J Chem Neuroanat 3:429–465. pmid:2291814
    OpenUrlPubMed
  50. ↵
    Khosravi-Hashemi N, Fortune ES, Chacron MJ (2011) Coding movement direction by burst firing in electrosensory neurons. J Neurophysiol 106:1954–1968. doi:10.1152/jn.00116.2011 pmid:21775723
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Khosravi-Hashemi N, Chacron MJ (2012) Bursts and isolated spikes code for opposite movement directions in midbrain electrosensory neurons. PLoS One 7:e40339. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0040339 pmid:22768279
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Khosravi-Hashemi N, Chacron MJ (2014) Motion processing across multiple topographic maps in the electrosensory system. Physiol Rep 2:e00253. doi:10.1002/phy2.253 pmid:24760508
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    Krahe R, Bastian J, Chacron MJ (2008) Temporal processing across multiple topographic maps in the electrosensory system. J Neurophysiol 100:852–867. doi:10.1152/jn.90300.2008 pmid:18509073
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Kwon SE, Yang H, Minamisawa G, O'Connor DH (2016) Sensory and decision-related activity propagate in a cortical feedback loop during touch perception. Nat Neurosci 19:1243–1249. doi:10.1038/nn.4356
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Larson EA, Metzen MG, Chacron MJ (2014) Serotonin modulates electrosensory processing and behavior via 5-HT2-like receptors. Neuroscience 271:108–118. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.033 pmid:24780766
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Larson ET, Summers CH (2001) Serotonin reverses dominant social status. Behav Brain Res 121:95–102. pmid:11275287
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Lemon N, Turner RW (2000) Conditional spike backpropagation generates burst discharge in a sensory neuron. J Neurophysiol 84:1519–1530. doi:10.1152/jn.2000.84.3.1519
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Maler L (1979) The posterior lateral line lobe of certain gymnotiform fish. Quantitative light microscopy. J Comp Neur 183:323–363. doi:10.1002/cne.901830208 pmid:762262
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Maler L (2009) Receptive field organization across multiple electrosensory maps. I. Columnar organization and estimation of receptive field size. J Comp Neur 516:376–393. doi:10.1002/cne.22124 pmid:19655387
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    Maler L, Ellis WG (1987) Inter-male aggressive signals in weakly electric fish are modulated by monoamines. Behav Brain Res 25:75–81. pmid:3620087
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Maler L, Sas EK, Rogers J (1981) The cytology of the posterior lateral line lobe of high frequency weakly electric fish (Gymnotidae): differentiation and synaptic specificity in a simple cortex. J Comp Neur 195:87–139. doi:10.1002/cne.901950107
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Maler L, Sas E, Johnston S, Ellis W (1991) An atlas of the brain of the weakly electric fish Apteronotus leptorhynchus . J Chem Neuroanat 4:1–38. doi:10.1016/0891-0618(91)90030-G
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Manita S, Suzuki T, Homma C, Matsumoto T, Odagawa M, Yamada K, Ota K, Matsubara C, Inutsuka A, Sato M, Ohkura M, Yamanaka A, Yanagawa Y, Nakai J, Hayashi Y, Larkum ME, Murayama M (2015) A top-down cortical circuit for accurate sensory perception. Neuron 86:1304–1316. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.006 pmid:26004915
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Marder E (2012) Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron 76:1–11. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010 pmid:23040802
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Márquez BT, Krahe R, Chacron MJ (2013) Neuromodulation of early electrosensory processing in gymnotiform weakly electric fish. J Exp Biol 216:2442–2450. doi:10.1242/jeb.082370 pmid:23761469
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    Marsat G, Maler L (2012) Preparing for the unpredictable: adaptive feedback enhances the response to unexpected communication signals. J Neurophysiol 107:1241–1246. doi:10.1152/jn.00982.2011 pmid:22157118
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Martinez D, Metzen MG, Chacron MJ (2016) Electrosensory processing in Apteronotus albifrons: implications for general and specific neural coding strategies across wave-type weakly electric fish species. J Neurophysiol 116:2909–2921. doi:10.1152/jn.00594.2016 pmid:27683890
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    Mejias JF, Marsat G, Bol K, Maler L, Longtin A (2013) Learning contrast-invariant cancellation of redundant signals in neural systems. PLoS Comput Biol 9:e1003180. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003180 pmid:24068898
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    Metzen MG, Jamali M, Carriot J, Ávila-Åkerberg O, Cullen KE, Chacron MJ (2015) Coding of envelopes by correlated but not single-neuron activity requires neural variability. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:4791–4796. doi:10.1073/pnas.1418224112 pmid:25825717
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  70. ↵
    Montoya ER, Terburg D, Bos PA, van Honk J (2012) Testosterone, cortisol, and serotonin as key regulators of social aggression: a review and theoretical perspective. Motiv Emot 36:65–73. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9264-3 pmid:22448079
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    Nelson ME, MacIver MA (1999) Prey capture in the weakly electric fish Apteronotus albifrons: sensory acquisition strategies and electrosensory consequences. J Exp Biol 202:1195–1203. pmid:10210661
    OpenUrlAbstract
  72. ↵
    Noonan L, Doiron B, Laing C, Longtin A, Turner RW (2003) A dynamic dendritic refractory period regulates burst discharge in the electrosensory lobe of weakly electric fish. J Neurosci 23:1524–1534. pmid:12598641
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    Ostapoff EM, Morest DK, Potashner SJ (1990) Uptake and retrograde transport of [3H]GABA from the cochlear nucleus to the superior olive in the guinea pig. J Chem Neuroanat 3:285–295. pmid:1697753
    OpenUrlPubMed
  74. ↵
    Oswald AMM, Chacron MJ, Doiron B, Bastian J, Maler L (2004) Parallel processing of sensory input by bursts and isolated spikes. J Neurosci 24:4351–4362. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0459-04.2004 pmid:15128849
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    Parent A (1981) Comparative anatomy of the serotoninergic systems. J Physiol (Paris) 77:147–156. pmid:7288635
    OpenUrlPubMed
  76. ↵
    Petzold GC, Hagiwara A, Murthy VN (2009) Serotonergic modulation of odor input to the mammalian olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 12:784–791. doi:10.1038/nn.2335 pmid:19430472
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    Rose GJ, Canfield JG (1993a) Longitudinal tracking responses of the weakly electric fish, Sternopygus. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 171:791–798. doi:10.1007/BF00213075
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  78. ↵
    Rose GJ, Canfield JG (1993b) Longitudinal tracking responses of Eigenmannia and Sternopygus . J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 173:698–700.
    OpenUrl
  79. ↵
    Sas E, Maler L (1983) The nucleus praeeminentialis: a golgi study of a feedback center in the electrosensory system of gymnotid fish. J Comp Neur 221:127–144. doi:10.1002/cne.902210202 pmid:6655077
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    Sas E, Maler L (1987) The organization of afferent input to the caudal lobe of the cerebellum of the gymnotid fish Apteronotus leptorhynchus . Anat Embryol 177:55–79. pmid:3439638
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    Sharpee TO, Calhoun AJ, Chalasani SH (2014) Information theory of adaptation in neurons, behavior, and mood. Curr Opin Neurobiol 25:47–53. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2013.11.007 pmid:24709600
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    Sherman SM, Guillery RW (2002) The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357:1695–1708. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1161
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  83. ↵
    Shumway C (1989) Multiple electrosensory maps in the medulla of weakly electric gymnotiform fish. I. Physiological differences. J Neurosci 9:4388–4399. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-12-04388.1989
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  84. ↵
    Takahashi N, Oertner TG, Hegemann P, Larkum ME (2016) Active cortical dendrites modulate perception. Science 354:1587–1590. doi:10.1126/science.aah6066 pmid:28008068
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  85. ↵
    Thompson AM, Hurley LM (2004) Dense serotonergic innervation of principal nuclei of the superior olivary complex in mouse. Neurosci Lett 356:179–182. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2003.11.052 pmid:15036624
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    Toporikova N, Chacron MJ (2009) SK channels gate information processing in vivo by regulating an intrinsic bursting mechanism seen in vitro . J Neurophysiol 102:2273–2287. doi:10.1152/jn.00282.2009 pmid:19675292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. Toscano-Márquez B, Dunn RJ, Krahe R (2013) Distribution of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor mRNA in the brain of the weakly electric fish Apteronotus leptorhynchus . J Comp Neur 521:1054–1072. doi:10.1002/cne.23218 pmid:22911562
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    Wark B, Lundstrom BN, Fairhall A (2007) Sensory adaptation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17:423–429. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.001 pmid:17714934
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    Waterhouse BD, Azizi SA, Burne RA, Woodward DJ (1990) Modulation of rat cortical area 17 neuronal responses to moving visual stimuli during norepinephrine and serotonin microiontophoresis. Brain Res 514:276–292. pmid:2357542
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    Zakon H, Oestreich J, Tallarovic S, Triefenbach F (2002) EOD modulations of brown ghost electric fish: JARs, chirps, rises, and dips. J Physiol (Paris) 96:451–458. doi:10.1016/S0928-4257(03)00012-3 pmid:14692493
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    Zhang ZD, Chacron MJ (2016) Adaptation to second order stimulus features by electrosensory neurons causes ambiguity. Sci Rep 6:28716. doi:10.1038/srep28716 pmid:27349635
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Leonard Maler, University of Ottawa

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below.

The authors have now done the extra experiments called for by the reviewers and editor. They have applied 5-HT during both looming and receding motion and for both matching and non-matching stimulus/cell type conditions.

The results now convincingly show that 5-HT selectively enhances the detectability of receding stimuli. This is an unexpected and interesting result.

I have only one request for a minor change that relates to the definition of ‘proximal’.

Please change:

“We hypothesize that this is because synapses from descending pathways are located more proximally to SK1...”

to

“We hypothesize that this is because synapses from descending pathways are located closer to SK1...”

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 5 (3)
eNeuro
Vol. 5, Issue 3
May/June 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Serotonin Selectively Increases Detectability of Motion Stimuli in the Electrosensory System
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Serotonin Selectively Increases Detectability of Motion Stimuli in the Electrosensory System
Mariana M. Marquez, Maurice J. Chacron
eNeuro 18 May 2018, 5 (3) ENEURO.0013-18.2018; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0013-18.2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Serotonin Selectively Increases Detectability of Motion Stimuli in the Electrosensory System
Mariana M. Marquez, Maurice J. Chacron
eNeuro 18 May 2018, 5 (3) ENEURO.0013-18.2018; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0013-18.2018
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • burst firing
  • feedback
  • neuromodulation
  • serotonin
  • weakly electric fish

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

New Research

  • Deciding while acting - Mid-movement decisions are more strongly affected by action probability than reward amount
  • CaMKIIα promoter-controlled circuit manipulations target both pyramidal cells and inhibitory interneurons in cortical networks
  • Gas7 is a novel dendritic spine initiation factor
Show more New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Different control strategies drive interlimb differences in performance and adaptation during reaching movements in novel dynamics
  • The nasal solitary chemosensory cell signaling pathway triggers mouse avoidance behavior to inhaled nebulized irritants
  • Taste-odor association learning alters the dynamics of intra-oral odor responses in the posterior piriform cortex of awake rats
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.