Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleTheory, Sensory and Motor Systems

Up-Down-Like Background Spiking Can Enhance Neural Information Transmission

Felix Droste and Benjamin Lindner
eNeuro 27 December 2017, 4 (6) ENEURO.0282-17.2017; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0282-17.2017
Felix Droste
1 Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Berlin 10115, Germany
2Department of Physics, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Berlin 12489, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Benjamin Lindner
1 Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Berlin 10115, Germany
2Department of Physics, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Berlin 12489, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

How neurons transmit information about sensory or internal signals is strongly influenced by ongoing internal activity. Depending on brain state, this background spiking can occur asynchronously or clustered in up states, periods of collective firing that are interspersed by silent down states. Here, we study which effect such up-down (UD) transitions have on signal transmission. In a simple model, we obtain numerical and analytical results for information theoretic measures. We find that, surprisingly, an UD background can benefit information transmission: when background activity is sparse, it is advantageous to distribute spikes into up states rather than uniformly in time. We reproduce the same effect in a more realistic recurrent network and show that signal transmission is further improved by incorporating that up states propagate across cortex as traveling waves. We propose that traveling UD activity might represent a compromise between reducing metabolic strain and maintaining information transmission capabilities.

  • information transmission
  • modelling
  • network
  • spontaneous activity
  • up-down states

Significance Statement

Spontaneous background activity in the cortex shapes the information transmission between neural populations. While asynchronous-irregular (AI) spontaneous activity has received lots of attention from theoreticians, the important case where the background switches between levels of high and low activity (up and down states) is still poorly understood. Here, we put forward a theoretical framework for computing the information transmission in the presence of up-down (UD) transitions. We show that an UD background can benefit information transmission when the firing rate in the background is low, indicating that such regimes may be well suited to maintain basic information transmission capabilities at lowered firing rates.

Introduction

The behavioral state of an animal is reflected in the activity of neural populations, the so-called brain state. Roughly, two extremes of a continuum of brain states have been distinguished (Harris and Thiele, 2011): an asynchronous-irregular (AI) or desynchronized regime (Renart et al., 2010), in which neurons fire independently at an approximately constant rate, and an up-down (UD) or synchronized regime, where the firing rate jumps between a high and a low level, termed up and down states (Steriade et al., 1993; Cowan and Wilson, 1994). The AI regime is typically observed during attentive, task-related behavior, while UD switching is traditionally associated with anesthesia or slow-wave sleep (Steriade et al., 2001), but has more recently also been observed during quiet wakefulness (Petersen et al., 2003; Luczak et al., 2007; Poulet et al., 2012; Luczak et al., 2013).

Neural information processing involves the communication between populations of neurons (Gerstner et al., 2014). For a signal transmitted by one population, the spontaneous activity of other populations constitutes a background (Chance et al., 2002; Destexhe and Contreras, 2006), the nature of which depends on brain state. Experiments studying the influence of background activity have focused on the response to sensory signals, which is strongly influenced by ongoing activity (Arieli et al., 1996) and top-down input from higher cortical regions (Gilbert and Sigman, 2007). It seems likely that non-sensory internal signals are equally influenced by background populations, although disentangling signal and background experimentally is, of course, difficult in this case.

How signal transmission is influenced by an AI background has been thoroughly studied (Brunel et al., 2001; Lindner and Schimansky-Geier, 2001; Chance et al., 2002; Larkum et al., 2004; Vogels and Abbott, 2005; Kumar et al., 2008), but in theoretical work the case of a UD background has largely been neglected. For one, sensory signals may face such a background during quiet wakefulness. A growing number of experimental studies have therefore investigated the effect that a UD regime has on the transmission of time-dependent sensory stimuli (Goard and Dan, 2009; Marguet and Harris, 2011; Luczak et al., 2013; Zagha et al., 2013; Pachitariu et al., 2015) and, most recently, UD-like dynamics has been even linked to selective attention (Engel et al., 2016). But also in the case of UD activity during slow-wave sleep, it is important to understand consequences for information transmission. Here, internal signals, mediating, for instance, the consolidation of memories (Diekelmann and Born, 2010), need to be transmitted. This calls for an extension of the theoretical framework that has been successfully used to study AI backgrounds to the UD case.

A better theoretical understanding of information transmission under UD regimes may also help to elucidate their functional role, which is still debated. Closely linked is the question about the purpose of sleep, for which various hypotheses exist (Watson and Buzsáki, 2015), the most prominent one of which is certainly memory consolidation (Maquet, 2001; Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Rasch and Born, 2013). Of interest for our work is that the UD regime in slow-wave sleep has been hypothesized to allow synaptic homeostasis (Tononi and Cirelli, 2006) and to enable regenerative cellular mechanisms that cannot function under the strain of higher overall firing rates (Vyazovskiy and Harris, 2013). This last hypothesis raises the question why a lower firing rate is realized by introducing pauses, the down states, rather than in a way that maintains an AI regime. In this theoretical study, we propose that the answer may lie in the respective signal transmission properties. We compare how a time-dependent signal is transmitted by a neural population that is subject to either a UD or an AI background and show that a UD regime may be preferable when background rates are low, because, for a given budget of background spikes, it allows more information to be transmitted. In other words, if overall background rates are lowered, the optimal kind of background switches from an AI spiking to a UD regime and that is why the latter regime is indeed observed in brain states with low overall firing rate.

Materials and Methods

We first consider a simplified setup, in which the neurons of the readout population are uncoupled, the signal enters as a current, and UD durations are exponentially distributed. Then, we describe its extension to a recurrent network with rate-coded signal and gamma-distributed UD durations, as well as a simple way to incorporate traveling UD states. The parameters used in all model variants are summarized in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Parameters used if not indicated otherwise

Readout neuron dynamics in the uncoupled case

The readout population consists of N = 1000 leaky integrate-and-fire (LIF) neurons. Their dynamics between spikes is given by Embedded Image (1) where τ = 20 ms is the membrane time constant, V0 the resting potential, Embedded Image the input signal, Embedded Image the set of arrival times of all background spikes to the nth neuron, and Embedded Image the weight of the ith background spike. Equation 1 is supplemented by a fire-and-reset rule: whenever Embedded Image mV, a spike is registered; mathematically, this is represented as a δ-peak in the neuron’s spike train xn(t). The voltage is then reset to Embedded Image mV where it is clamped for an absolute refractory period Embedded Image ms.

Background input

The weights of background spikes, Embedded Image, are drawn from an exponential distribution with mean Jext. Such a skewed distribution is a more realistic assumption than identical spike weights; however, our primary reason for using this is the analytical tractability it brings along (Richardson and Swarbrick, 2010; Droste and Lindner, 2017a). Background spikes occur according to an inhomogeneous Poisson process with rate Embedded Image. Here, NB is the number of background neurons from which each readout neuron receives inputs and Embedded Image (2) where η(t) is a Markovian dichotomous process that jumps between up states (Embedded Image) and down states (η(t) = 0) at constant rates Embedded Image (up to down) and Embedded Image (down to up).

Signal

The signal s(t) is band-limited Gaussian noise with a flat spectrum given by Embedded Image. This choice implies that s(t) has unit variance; the variance of Embedded Image is then Embedded Image. In simulations, signals are generated by randomly drawing frequency components, consistent with Embedded Image, which are then transformed back to the time domain.

Output

The output of the readout population which we consider to assess information transmission is its population activity, i.e., the normalized sum over the neurons’ spike trains, xn(t), Embedded Image (3)

In simulations, we measure a binned version of a(t) with bin width Embedded Image ms.

Extension to a recurrent network with rate-coded signal and more regular UD switching

We now consider an excitatory (E) and an inhibitory (I) population. Each neuron has a fixed number of CE = 1000 (CI = 250) randomly chosen presynaptic partners from the E (I) population (autapses are excluded). The neural dynamics are now governed by Embedded Image (4) where Embedded Image. Here, J = 0.1 mV is the weight of recurrent connections, D = 1.5 ms is the transmission delay and xn,k(t) (xn,l(t)) is the spike train of the nth neuron’s kth (lth) excitatory (inhibitory) presynaptic partner. The Poisson input now also comprises spikes from the signal population, Embedded Image (5)

The two-state process Embedded Image is no longer Markovian; instead, residence times are now distributed according to the γ distribution, Embedded Image (6) where Embedded Image is the shape parameter. For Embedded Image, this recovers the case of exponentially distributed times. With increasing Embedded Image , switching becomes more regular. We chose Embedded Image such that the mean residence time Embedded Image remains constant.

Traveling waves

As a simple way of incorporating traveling waves, we assign the background population of the nth readout neuron a position Embedded Image (drawn from a uniform distribution between 0 and l = 4 mm). Spikes are then drawn with the rate Embedded Image (7) where c is the propagation speed of the wave.

Spectral measures

We use the following convention for the Fourier transform, Embedded Image (8) where the back-transform differs only by the sign in the exponent. The power spectrum/cross-spectrum of two time series x(t) and y(t) is defined asEmbedded Image (9) where * denotes complex conjugation. For stationary processes, the power/cross-spectrum corresponds to the Fourier transform of the auto/cross-correlation function. In simulations, we use the Fourier transform with a finite time window T, Embedded Image, and approximate the spectrum as Embedded Image (10)

Theoretical expressions

Using the definition of the population activity Equation 3, the coherence function, Equation 23, can be written asEmbedded Image (11) where Embedded Image is the cross-spectrum between the signal and a single spike train, Sxx(f) is the power spectrum of a single spike train and Embedded Image the cross-spectrum between spike trains of different neurons. For sufficiently weak signals, one can assume linear response and write Embedded Image, where χ(f) is the susceptibility or dynamical transfer function of the neuron with respect to current modulations. For the AI case, Embedded Image and Embedded Image are known (Richardson and Swarbrick, 2010; Droste and Lindner, 2017a). Here, spike trains are only correlated by the signal, so that Embedded Image.

For the UD case (with exponentially distributed UD durations and readout neurons that are uncoupled, though still strongly correlated due to the common UD switching), we have derived novel approximations for the spectral quantities, that we state below. The basic assumption of the approximation is that the down states are long compared to the membrane time constant. For an extensive exact treatment of the case of a pure telegraph noise (without shot-noise component), see Droste and Lindner (2017b).

Simple approximations for power spectrum and susceptibility are given by Embedded Image (12) Embedded Image (13)

The quantities marked by a + superscript refer to those of a LIF driven by an excitatory homogeneous Poisson process (the AI case) but at the up-state rate Embedded Image. In essence, the susceptibility is just a scaled down version of the AI susceptibility, while the power spectrum contains an additional term that corresponds to the power spectrum of a dichotomous process that jumps between 0 and Embedded Image, where Embedded Image is the (readout) firing rate in an up state. It can be approximated as Embedded Image (14)where Embedded Image is the stationary firing rate of an LIF driven by excitatory shot noise with exponentially distributed weights (occurring at the up-state rate Embedded Image; Richardson and Swarbrick, 2010; Droste and Lindner, 2017a), and Embedded Image is the continuous part of the Fourier-transformed time-dependent rate of such an LIF with the initial condition v(t = 0) = μ, which can be expressed asEmbedded Image (15)

Here, Embedded Image and Embedded Image are given in terms of confluent hypergeometric functions, Embedded Image (16) Embedded Image (17)

Note that more elaborate approximations for Embedded Image and Embedded Image, which contain Equation 12 and Equation 13 as limit cases, have been derived (Droste, 2015). Here, the simple versions can be used because the frequency dependence of the coherence is dominated by the cross-spectrum.

Calculating the cross-spectrum is more complicated. Here, we need to distinguish whether all readout neurons receive the same UD process (the Embedded Image limit in the traveling wave scenario) or whether they all receive independent background input (Embedded Image). In the latter case, they are again only correlated by the signal, while in the former, we approximate the cross-spectrum as the sum of a signal-induced part and Embedded Image, the cross-spectrum of two neurons driven only by Poissonian shot noise with a common two-state rate modulation. We have Embedded Image (18)

We can express Embedded Image in terms of convolutions, involving again the Fourier transform of the time-dependent rate, Embedded Image (Eq. 5),Embedded Image (19) where * denotes convolution and we have used the abbreviations Embedded Image (20)

and Embedded Image (21)

An analytical approximation for the silence fraction is obtained by exploiting that the UD process is Markovian and noting that the time that a down states needs to propagate across the population is given by l / c, Embedded Image (22)

Correlation measures

To quantify correlations among neurons within one trial, we average the Pearson correlation coefficient between the spike counts of two neurons in bins of length T = 100 ms over time and all pairs of neurons; to assess correlations across trials, we fix the signal (frozen noise stimulus) but not the UD switching and average the Pearson correlation coefficient over time and all pairs of trials.

Results

Our basic modeling approach is motivated by the observation that UD switching often seems stochastic (Stern et al., 1997; Johnson et al., 2010; Mochol et al., 2015). Rather than picking a specific mechanism that generates such transitions dynamically (Contreras et al., 1996; Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; Compte et al., 2003; Holcman and Tsodyks, 2006; Mochol et al., 2015), we prescribe the statistical distributions of UD state durations in the background. This permits a clean comparison between the effects of UD versus AI backgrounds on signal transmission. Moreover, it allows us to derive analytical results for the statistics of information flow in the presence of UD states.

UD transitions in the background enable better tracking of a time-dependent signal

We first consider a population of N = 1000 uncoupled LIF neurons (the readout population), each of which receives the same signal (Fig. 1, sketch). Additionally, each readout neuron receives input from 1000 neurons that belong to the background population. These background neurons are modeled by Poisson processes that share a common time-dependent rate. We distinguish two regimes of the background population: it is either in an AI regime, in which case the rate is constant in time, or in a UD regime, in which case the rate is a stochastic process that jumps between two levels so that the time spent in each state is exponentially distributed with mean Embedded Image (up) and Embedded Image (down), respectively. We model the signal as band-limited Gaussian white noise with a cutoff frequency of fC = 75 Hz. See the Methods section for details of this setup.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

How do the transmission properties of the readout population depend on the activity of the background population? 1000 uncoupled neurons receive a common signal. We ask how the information transmission between the signal and the activity of this readout population is influenced by the dynamic regime of the background population. In particular, we compare a regime where background spikes occur uniformly in time to one where they have been redistributed into up states.

For comparing signal transmission with an AI background and a UD background (Fig. 2A,B), we keep the mean background rate Embedded Image fixed, i.e., over a long time window, a postsynaptic cell receives on average the same number of background spikes. In both cases, we present the same signal (Fig. 2C,D). Although the mean background rate is the same, the response of the readout population differs drastically: in the AI case (Fig. 2E), very few spikes are elicited. Here, the population activity a(t) seems to be little related to the signal and is not reproducible on repeated stimulation (to illustrate the latter, two trials with a frozen-noise signal are shown in black and blue, respectively). By contrast, with a UD background (Fig. 2F), no spiking occurs during the down states, but during the up states the population activity tracks the signal reliably.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

More information about the signal can be transmitted by the readout population activity a(t) if background activity undergoes transitions between up and down states. A, B, Firing rate of the background population and raster-plot of the spikes received by an arbitrary readout neuron. C, D, Signal realization (frozen noise). E, F, Normalized population activity Embedded Image and spike raster of the readout population. For the population activity, two trials with the same signal (C, D) and, in the UD case, two-state process (B) are shown. G, Coherence between signal and activity of the readout population. H, Lower bound to the mutual information rate and Embedded Image, the mean firing rate of the readout population, as a function of the mean background rate Embedded Image . The dashed line marks the Embedded Image value used in A–G. Parameters, if not indicated otherwise: Embedded Image Hz, Embedded Image ms, Embedded Image ms.

We would like to stress that the improved transmission characteristics are simply due to a higher firing rate of the readout population. While in the AI case background spikes occur too sparsely to push readout neurons across threshold, redistributing them into up states allows the readout population to fire and, thus, transmit information about the signal at least in a fraction of the time.

Quantifying information transmission

To quantify the effect of a UD background, we repeat the procedure for many trials (with different realizations of signal and UD switching) and calculate the spectral coherence between signal and readout activity. The coherence function, Embedded Image (23) attains values between zero and one and indicates how well each frequency component of the signal can be linearly reconstructed from the output. It is calculated from the cross-spectrum, Ssa(f), between signal and readout activity and the power spectra, Saa(f) and Sss(f), of activity and signal, respectively.

For an AI background, theoretical expressions for these quantities are known (Richardson and Swarbrick, 2010; Droste and Lindner, 2017a); for a UD background, we have derived a novel approximation (see Materials and Methods). We plot simulation results and theory in Figure 2G. It can be seen that the AI coherence is low-pass, which is a known result for integrate-and-fire neurons (Vilela and Lindner, 2009). By contrast, the UD coherence is bandpass, almost vanishing at low frequencies but extending to higher frequencies. This is similar to single neurons subject to a broadband stimulus and a slow background noise (Lindner, 2016) and can be understood by considering that the slow two-state switching hampers signal transmission most strongly at low frequencies: looking only at the readout activity, it becomes difficult to know whether slow fluctuations are actually a part of the signal or a manifestation of the UD switching. Remarkably, the reduction at low frequencies can be overcompensated in the higher frequency range, such that the overall information transmission for the UD case can be higher than in the AI case. The overall information transmission is quantified as follows. As the signal is Gaussian, one may use the coherence to obtain a rigorous lower bound for the mutual information rate (Gabbiani, 1996),Embedded Image (24)

In Figure 2H, we show Embedded Image for the two scenarios as the mean background rate Embedded Image is varied. At the intermediate background rate used so far (Fig. 2H, dashed line), the information rate is higher for a UD background, which is in line with what we found above. We have argued that this is a firing-rate effect; indeed, with a UD background, the readout population starts to fire at lower background rates (Fig. 2H, bottom panel). Notably, at higher background rates, information flow is larger with an AI background.

Effect of up/down duration and temporal structure of the signal

In experiments, mean up/down state durations ranging from 100 milliseconds to several seconds have been observed (Steriade et al., 1993; Cowan and Wilson, 1994; Stern et al., 1997). Here, we investigate whether the benefits of a UD background occur robustly over such a wide range. Furthermore, we explore the dependence of information transmission on the times spent in up and down states and their interplay with the time scales of the signal.

The coherence functions shown in Figure 3A demonstrate that the benefits of a UD background for signal transmission are robust with respect to the mean residence times. Note that the background firing rates in the two states as well as the mean background rate remain unchanged. Generally, the slower the switching is, the higher is the overall information rate, going along with a more and more pronounced reduction of coherence at low frequencies. Both can be traced back to the temporal correlations of the UD fluctuations which diminish information transmission in a lower and lower frequency band set by the cutoff frequency Embedded Image .

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Effects of up/down duration and temporal structure of the signal. A, Coherence for AI background (blue) and UD background at three different values for the mean duration Embedded Image. Here, Embedded Image Hz. B, Mutual information rates as a function of the background rate for the cases shown in A. The inset shows the theoretical curves over a wider range; the gray line marks what could optimally be reached by infinitely slow switching. C, D, Signal power spectra and coherence for a lower cutoff f0 of 0 Hz and 25 Hz, respectively. E, Mutual information rates as a function of f0. The dashed line marks cutoff frequency used in A–C. In A–E, Embedded Image ms, Embedded Image ms, Embedded Image Hz.

What information rate is observed in a scenario with an arbitrarily slow UD background? In this limit, the information rate as a function of the mean background rate attains a particularly simple form in terms of the rate for the AI case: Embedded Image (25)

Information is transmitted only during the up states that occur with probability Embedded Image, and the information rate in these up states is the same as in the AI regime, albeit with a rescaled firing rate. Equation 25 (Fig. 3B, inset, gray line) makes clear that the information rate approaches a finite limit and, moreover, suggests that there is a background rate above which a UD background is no longer beneficial (here at Embedded Image Hz), irrespective of residence times.

The observation that slow signals are more severely affected by UD switching suggests that shifting the power of the signal toward higher frequencies should make the beneficial effect of a UD background even more pronounced. In Figure 3C–E, we compare the transmission of signals with constant power in the range Embedded Image. For the sake of illustration, we use here a reduced signal bandwidth Δf = 25 Hz. For f0 = 0, this choice yields slightly better information transmission with an AI background (Fig. 3C,E). Increasing f0 changes this drastically (Fig. 3D,E). In particular, we observe an optimal lower cutoff frequency, which, for the parameters chosen here, is close to 25 Hz.

A UD background is also advantageous in a more realistic recurrent network

Up to here, we have made some simplifying assumptions that allowed us to demonstrate and investigate the beneficial effect of a UD background in a tractable setting. Below, we make the setup more realistic in three aspects and show that the observed effect is robust.

The readout population so far consisted of uncoupled neurons that received only excitatory input. To relax this, we now consider a readout population (N = 1000) that is a subset of a recurrent network, consisting of 10,000 excitatory and 2500 inhibitory neurons (Fig. 4A). The network follows the classical setup by Brunel (2000; for details, see Materials and Methods).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

The beneficial effect of a UD background persists in a more realistic setup. A, Sketch of the modified setup. B, Mutual information rate for different mean background rates Embedded Image. The dashed line marks the value used in C–F. C, Sample traces of the readout population activity for irregular (gray, Embedded Image) and rather regular (red, Embedded Image) UD switching. D, Histogram for the duration of up states in the readout population. To obtain this, population activity is binned (width ΔT = 10 ms); n consecutive bins in each of which at least one readout neuron spikes then define a readout up state of length TU = nΔT. E, Effect of changing the regularity of UD switching on the mutual information rate. F, Effect of changing the recurrent synaptic weights. The dotted line marks the value used in B–E. In all panels, Embedded Image ms.

In the previous sections, the durations of UD states were exponentially distributed. While broad distributions have been reported (Stern et al., 1997; Johnson et al., 2010), the statistics seem to be highly dependent on brain state/anesthesia (Chauvette et al., 2011) and can also be rather regular (Steriade et al., 1993). To control switching regularity, we now draw the times spent in UD states from a gamma-distribution, characterized by a shape parameter Embedded Image . With increasing Embedded Image, switching becomes more regular, while Embedded Image recovers the case of exponentially distributed times.

The third change in the setup concerns the signal, which previously entered the neuronal dynamics directly as a current modulation. Now, we introduce a signal population of Ns = 1000 Poisson neurons which encode the signal in their time-dependent firing rate.

As shown in Figure 4B, a UD background can still be beneficial for information transmission when background rates are low, even in this more realistic setup.

Changing the regularity of the switching between up and down states in the background has a large effect on the regularity of UD switching in the population activity, as evident from sample traces of a(t) (Fig. 4C) and the histogram of up-state duration (Fig. 4D; an up state in the readout population is here defined as a sequence of time bins of Δt = 10 ms in which a(t) > 0 Hz). In particular, the distribution of times the readout population spends in an up state closely follows that of the background population. Nevertheless, the effect of changing the regularity of the switching on the mutual information rate (and the beneficial nature of the UD background in this context) is negligible (Fig. 4E).

Changing the weights of recurrent connections in the network, J (Fig. 4F) has a strong effect on signal transmission properties, both for UD and AI backgrounds. Here, it is plausible that Embedded Image goes to zero in the limit of a very strongly coupled network, in which intrinsic dynamics dominate the population activity. Remarkably, the information rate with UD background shows a pronounced maximum at intermediate synaptic strength. This is indicative of the rich dynamics that recurrent networks exhibit and constitutes an interesting subject for future work.

Effect of the signal on the UD switching

A number of experimental studies have described an effect of the sensory signal on the UD switching (Shu et al., 2003; Reig and Sanchez-Vives, 2007). In our original setup, the UD switching happens in a background population that is unaffected by the signal. It is, however, interesting how such signal-dependence of the UD transitions influences their benefit for information transmission. While for a thorough study, one would need to explicitly model the mechanism underlying the UD transitions to allow the signal to influence them, we pursue a more phenomenological approach here. We assume that the background population also receives the signal and that it enters as a modulation of the switching rates Embedded Image (from up to down and vice versa), which now become time dependent, Embedded Image (26) Embedded Image (27) where the Embedded Image now stand for the original (constant) rates and Embedded Image quantifies how strongly UD transitions are influenced by the signal s(t). We assume that a positive signal tends to keep the population in an up state (thus reducing the rate of leaving it), while it makes a down state shorter; hence the different signs in Equations 26, 27.

In Figure 5, we plot the mutual information rate with an UD background for different values of Embedded Image. We also show sample traces of the signal and the respective time-dependent firing rates of the background population (to make these traces comparable, the same sequences of random numbers have been used). The signal-dependence of the UD switching has only a small effect on the mutual information rate, namely a slight overall increase. This is consistent with the fact that more signal power is fed into the system (hence the increase). This power, however, is strongly low-pass filtered by the comparatively slow UD switching so that only information about low frequencies reaches the neurons in the readout population via the background (hence the weakness of the effect). Consequently, we see a much more pronounced effect of the signal influencing the U/D transitions, if the signal power is restricted to a very low frequency band (Fig. 5B). Qualitatively, in all cases the beneficial effect of UD switching persists.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

A simple model for the effect of the signal on the UD switching We assume that the transition rates between UD states in the background are modulated by the signal. Shown is a realization of the signal and the corresponding background rate for three values of the modulation strength Embedded Image, as well as the mutual information rate over the mean background rate (obtained in simulations) for these Embedded Image. A, Parameters as in Figure 2. B, A much slower signal with fC = 1 Hz. Note the different y-axis scaling in the mutual information plots.

Traveling waves of up states allow continuous signal transmission

Experiments have shown that the switching between up and down states is not always simultaneous for distinct neurons in a cortical region, but instead often occurs in the form of traveling waves (Amzica and Steriade, 1995; Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; Petersen et al., 2003; Luczak et al., 2007). This motivates us to extend the setup from Figure 2 to incorporate traveling UD states in a simple way. We consider the neurons of the background population to be distributed uniformly along one space dimension (length l = 4 mm). Note that the spatial extent of the readout population will in general not correspond to that of the background but can be much smaller, implying a strong overlap in receptive fields of the read-out neurons, which motivates the global nature of our input signal. As before, each readout neuron receives Poisson input with a two-state rate, but now the switching in this rate does not happen simultaneously across neurons but propagates with a constant velocity c across the background population (Fig. 6A, sketch). As c and Embedded Image are the quantities that are typically measured in experiments, we use them as key parameters of the system. This means in particular that the average spatial extent of an up/down state, Embedded Image, varies if we vary c or Embedded Image .

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Traveling up states allow continuous signal transmission A, Sketch of the setup. B, Activity and spike raster of the readout population (neurons ordered by the position of their background population). C, Mutual information rates for different background rates. Red solid and dashed lines correspond to the theoretical limits Embedded Image and Embedded Image , respectively; blue line is theory for AI background. The black dashed line marks the background rate used in the other panels. D, Mutual information rates for varying wave speed c. The dotted line marks the wave speed used in B, C. E, Silence density, i.e., fraction of time bins (length ΔT = 4 ms) in which the population activity is zero. Where nothing else is indicated, the wave speed is c = 10 mm/s and the mean background rate is Embedded Image Hz. In C–E, symbols represent simulation results, while lines are theory.

In the context of information transmission, traveling UD states can remedy an important conceptual problem. As discussed above, information transmission with a UD background benefits if the mean duration of up and down states is long, i.e., if the switching between the states is as slow as possible. However, a high average rate of information transmission (as quantified by Embedded Image) may be of little use to an animal if transient stimuli are missed because they fall into a down-state interval. If UD states do not occur simultaneously but instead propagate across cortex, however, this problem can be circumvented, as has been previously suggested in the context of periodic waves (Ermentrout and Kleinfeld, 2001): for appropriately chosen parameters (such that Embedded Image), there should almost always be a fraction of readout neurons that receive an up-state background and transmit the signal. Indeed, for a physiologically realistic propagation speed c = 10 mm/s (Embedded Image mm), continuous tracking of the signal works surprisingly well (Fig. 6B).

In Figure 6C, we plot the mutual information rate for different background rates. We also plot two limit cases: for waves that propagate infinitely fast (Embedded Image, implying Embedded Image; solid red line), state changes spread instantaneously through the population and one recovers the case where UD switching occurs simultaneously (Fig. 2), while for infinitely slow propagation (Embedded Image; dashed red line), each readout neuron receives an independent two-state process. The latter limit yields higher information transmission rates because compared to the case of instantaneous propagation, in which neurons are synchronized by the simultaneous UD states, they become decorrelated here. Varying c interpolates between these limits (Fig. 6D): the information rate with a traveling UD background is always larger than with simultaneous switching.

To further quantify how traveling UD states enable continuous signal transmission, we measure the silence density (Mochol et al., 2015), defined as the fraction of time bins in which the population activity is zero (shown in Fig. 6E along with a theoretical prediction, see Materials and Methods). For a propagation velocity Embedded Image mm/s, this is practically zero, and already for c = 10 mm/s (dashed line), it has decayed to ≈10% of the value with instantaneous switching.

Discussion

We have studied theoretically how ongoing background activity affects the information transmitted about a signal. We find that signal transmission can benefit from a background that undergoes transitions between up and down states, compared to an AI background with the same mean firing rate. This is a surprising result, bearing in mind that the stochastic UD switching, which is unrelated to the signal, dominates the temporal structure of the readout activity. UD switching is favorable at low background rates, where an AI background elicits only weak spiking in the readout population. Here, up states allow at least brief periods with elevated firing rates, during which information rates are high enough to overcompensate for silent periods and UD-induced correlations. Taking into account that up states propagate as traveling waves makes the gain in information compared to the AI case even stronger. It also enables continuous signal transmission, addressing the concern that short transient signals might be missed during silent periods.

Generality and robustness

For our comparison of AI and UD regimes, we have focused on information transmission as a measure, a paradigm also used in experimental studies. This raises the question whether the beneficial effect of a UD background that we observe carries over to more complex computational tasks, in which information is not merely forwarded, but processed. Indeed, it seems plausible that a UD background also benefits information processing, simply because it is difficult to envision how the very low readout firing rates in the AI case could possibly be a preferable choice. An interesting quantitative approach to this question could be a comparison of UD and AI backgrounds in a network that is able to produce a high-dimensional representation of a signal, which is believed to facilitate computation by downstream neurons (Buonomano and Maass, 2009; Ostojic, 2014).

A deliberate limitation of our model is the clear separation between the UD background and the readout population. An alternative would have been to equip the readout population with a mechanism to generate UD switching dynamically (Contreras et al., 1996; Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000). As it would have become difficult to disentangle the effects of specific mechanisms from general consequences of UD transitions, we chose not to include endogenous generation of UD switching in the readout population.

Other simplifications in our model have only little effect. As we have demonstrated, the beneficial effect of a UD background persists when the readout population contains recurrent connections (including local inhibition), when the statistics of the UD switching are changed, or when the signal is encoded in input spikes, rather than entering as a current. We also have checked that the effect does not hinge on the exact value of the ratio of mean up and down durations within a physiologic range (we used Embedded Image in Fig. 3 and Embedded Image everywhere else). Even if we permit the signal to influence the switching rates of the UD background, this changes the information rates only slightly. The mechanism is robust because it is simple. This suggests that the effect would also persist if we included more realistic neuron models (such as the aEIF model; Brette and Gerstner, 2005), heterogeneity between neurons (Harrison et al., 2015), and recurrent connections (Litwin-Kumar and Doiron, 2012), or an overlap of background spike trains (de La Rocha et al., 2007), to name but a few possibilities.

Transient signals and silent periods

Down states in the background cause collective silent periods in the readout population. This poses a problem for signal transmission, because short but potentially important transient signals might be missed completely. There are at least two possible mechanisms to mitigate this. First, transient stimuli can influence the UD switching (Shu et al., 2003; Reig and Sanchez-Vives, 2007). This suggests that transient inputs could make themselves heard by causing an up state, while spontaneously occurring up states probe the input for more stationary features, in line with Luczak et al.(2013). Second, UD switching does in general not happen simultaneously across a patch of cortex; instead, up-state activity propagates as a traveling wave (Amzica and Steriade, 1995; Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; Petersen et al., 2003; Luczak et al., 2007). As we have demonstrated, this enables nearly continuous signal transmission for physiologically realistic parameters. Note that the idea that traveling waves ensure “that only part of the sensory field is rendered unresponsive” has already been brought up in the context of oscillatory waves (Ermentrout and Kleinfeld, 2001).

Relation to previous studies

Most theoretical studies concerned with UD states have primarily focused on their dynamical generation (Latham et al., 2000; Compte et al., 2003; Holcman and Tsodyks, 2006; Destexhe, 2009; Mochol et al., 2015). Stimulus transmission has received less attention (Curto et al., 2009; Reig et al., 2015), and we are not aware of previous theoretical studies that consider the information transmitted about time-dependent stimuli. Methodologically, we have built on analytical approaches used to describe signal transmission with AI backgrounds (Lindner and Schimansky-Geier, 2001; Fourcaud and Brunel, 2002; Richardson and Swarbrick, 2010). We have extended these approaches to a UD background, modeling the background rate as a two-state process. Comparison with simulations shows that our approximation (mainly based on the assumption of sufficiently long down states) works remarkably well for physiologically realistic parameter values.

Recently, several experimental studies have compared stimulus transmission during AI (desynchronized) and UD (synchronized) regimes (Goard and Dan, 2009; Marguet and Harris, 2011; Luczak et al., 2013; Zagha et al., 2013; Pachitariu et al., 2015). In these studies, multi-unit activity from a sensory cortex was measured during anesthesia or quiet wakefulness where UD switching could be observed. Transitions from UD to AI regimes were either allowed to happen spontaneously or induced. The response to a sensory stimulus was then compared across the two conditions. All studies found that the transmission of the stimulus was strongly improved in AI regimes; during UD regimes, information was also transmitted, but at a lower rate. In the AI case, correlations among simultaneously recorded neurons were reduced while correlations across trials (with a frozen stimulus) were increased (Goard and Dan, 2009; Pachitariu et al., 2015). Similarly, spiking activity could be better predicted from the stimulus in the AI regime, while the local field potential was a better predictor in the UD case (Marguet and Harris, 2011).

It is important to note that the comparison conducted in the cited experimental studies is not equivalent to the comparison in this work: while we contrast the two regimes at a fixed background rate, the experimental studies compare different brain states, which are likely to involve different background rates (Poulet et al., 2012). In Figure 7, we illustrate what such a comparison between brain states (characterized by different background rates) means in our model. We consider a transition from an “attentive state” (marked by 1 in Fig. 7) to an “inattentive state” at a reduced rate. A motivating question of our work was why the rate should be lowered via down states (1 Embedded Image 2 in Fig. 7) instead of in a uniform manner (1 Embedded Image 3 in Fig. 7). Our results suggest that the regime 2 rather than 3 is observed because it is advantageous for information transmission (Fig. 7B). This comparison, the UD regime 2 versus the AI regime 3, is the one we have focused on in this work, whereas the comparison in the cited experimental studies is probably closer to one between the AI regime 1 and the UD regime 2. Indeed, for this second comparison, our results are consistent with the experimental observations: more information is transmitted in the AI case 1 (Fig. 7B), which shows less correlations among simultaneously recorded neurons (noise correlations, Fig. 7C), but higher correlations across trials (signal correlations, Fig. 7D).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

What do our results say about the comparison of different brain states? A, We consider a switch from an attentive state (1) to an inattentive state at a lower mean background firing rate Embedded Image. The background rate can either be lowered by introducing pauses (going to the UD regime (2)) or in a uniform manner (going to the AI regime (3)). B, Switching from (1) to (2) maintains higher information rates than switching to (3); we propose that this is why (2), not (3), is observed. In both cases, the overall information transmission is reduced. C, This goes along with an increase in noise correlations (correlations among neurons within one trial; shown is the mean Pearson correlation coefficient of spike counts in 100-ms time bins; see Materials and Methods) and (D) a decrease in signal correlations (correlations in the binned population activity across trials; see Materials and Methods). All parameters are chosen like in Figure 6; in B–D, symbols represent simulation results.

Besides the UD transitions (slow oscillations) there are a number of other rhythms present in brain activity such as gamma oscillations, thalamic spindles and hippocampal sharp waves and ripples (Steriade, 2006). Spindles and sharp waves supposedly play a major role in the long-term consolidation of memories, e.g., the migration of mnemonic representations from hippocampus to neocortical areas (Buzsáki, 1996), and may be controlled by UD transitions to facilitate the consolidation process (Staresina et al., 2015). Gamma oscillations may be used for multiplexing information transfer efficiently (for an example from the visual system, see Koepsell et al., 2010). More generally, brain oscillations may mediate components of higher-level sensory perception, including perhaps human consciousness (Hopfield and Brody, 2001; Singer, 2001). In contrast to these and other specific roles of brain rhythms in neural information transmission, the beneficial effect of a UD background under the constraint of a low overall firing rate seems to be rather generic and thus potentially relevant for the information transfer in many brain areas.

Functional consequences

Our finding that UD switching can improve information transmission at lower background rates leads us to hypothesize that such a regime could represent a compromise between reducing metabolic cost (allowing cellular regeneration; Vyazovskiy and Harris, 2013) and maintaining information transmission capabilities. Similar to the suggestion that (periodic) traveling waves could be “means to scan the incoming sensory stream for novel features” (Ermentrout and Kleinfeld, 2001) or that up states represent the “sporadic opening of a ‘gate’” for the transmission of sensory signals (Luczak et al., 2013), we propose that below a certain level of activity, UD states emerge because they allow for a maximized information transmission under such constraints.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This work was supported by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) Grant FKZ:01GQ1001A, the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) Research Training Group Grant GRK1589/1, and a DFG Research Grant LI 1046/2-1.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Amzica F, Steriade M (1995) Short- and long-range neuronal synchronization of the slow (<1 Hz) cortical oscillation. J Neurophysiol 73:20–38. doi:10.1152/jn.1995.73.1.20 pmid:7714565
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Arieli A, Sterkin A, Grinvald A, Aertsen A (1996) Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science 273:1868–1871. pmid:8791593
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Brette R, Gerstner W (2005) Adaptive exponential integrate-and-fire model as an effective description of neuronal activity. J Neurophysiol 94:3637–3642. doi:10.1152/jn.00686.2005 pmid:16014787
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Brunel N (2000) Dynamics of sparsely connected networks of excitatory and inhibitory spiking neurons. J Comput Neurosci 8:183–208. pmid:10809012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Brunel N, Chance FS, Fourcaud N, Abbott LF (2001) Effects of synaptic noise and filtering on the frequency response of spiking neurons. Phys Rev Lett 86:2186–2189. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.86.2186 pmid:11289886
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Buonomano DV, Maass W (2009) State-dependent computations: spatiotemporal processing in cortical networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:113–125. doi:10.1038/nrn2558 pmid:19145235
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Buzsáki G (1996) The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cereb Cortex 6:81–92 . doi:10.1093/cercor/6.2.81
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD (2002) Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron 35:773–782. pmid:12194875
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Chauvette S, Crochet S, Volgushev M, Timofeev I (2011) Properties of slow oscillation during slow-wave sleep and anesthesia in cats. J Neurosci 31:14998–15008. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2339-11.2011 pmid:22016533
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Compte A, Sanchez-Vives MV, McCormick DA, Wang XJ (2003) Cellular and network mechanisms of slow oscillatory activity (<1 Hz) and wave propagations in a cortical network model. J Neurophysiol 89:2707–2725.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Contreras D, Timofeev I, Steriade M (1996) Mechanisms of long-lasting hyperpolarizations underlying slow sleep oscillations in cat corticothalamic networks. J Physiol 494:251–264. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021488
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Cowan RL, Wilson CJ (1994) Spontaneous firing patterns and axonal projections of single corticostriatal neurons in the rat medial agranular cortex. J Neurophysiol 71:17–32. doi:10.1152/jn.1994.71.1.17 pmid:8158226
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Curto C, Sakata S, Marguet S, Itskov V, Harris KD (2009) A simple model of cortical dynamics explains variability and state dependence of sensory responses in urethane-anesthetized auditory cortex. J Neurosci 29:10600–10612. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2053-09.2009 pmid:19710313
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    de La Rocha J, Doiron B, Shea-Brown E, Josíc K, Reyes A (2007) Correlation between neural spike trains increases with firing rate. Nature 448:802–806. doi:10.1038/nature06028 pmid:17700699
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Destexhe A (2009) Self-sustained asynchronous irregular states and up–down states in thalamic, cortical and thalamocortical networks of nonlinear integrate-and-fire neurons. J Comput Neurosci 27:493–506. doi:10.1007/s10827-009-0164-4 pmid:19499317
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Destexhe A, Contreras D (2006) Neuronal computations with stochastic network states. Science 314:85–90. doi:10.1126/science.1127241 pmid:17023650
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    Diekelmann S, Born J (2010) The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:114–126. doi:10.1038/nrn2762 pmid:20046194
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Droste F, Lindner B (2017a) Exact analytical results for integrate-and-fire neurons driven by excitatory shot noise. J Comp Neurosci 43:81–91. doi:10.1007/s10827-017-0649-5 pmid:28585050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Droste F, Lindner B (2017b) Exact results for power spectrum and susceptibility of a leaky integrate-and-fire neuron with two-state noise. Phys Rev E 95:012411. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.95.012411 pmid:28208429
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Droste F (2015) Signal transmission in stochastic neuron models with non-white or non-Gaussian noise. PhD thesis, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin.
  21. ↵
    Engel TA, Steinmetz NA, Gieselmann MA, Thiele A, Moore T, Boahen K (2016) Selective modulation of cortical state during spatial attention. Science 354:1140–1144. doi:10.1126/science.aag1420 pmid:27934763
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    Ermentrout GB, Kleinfeld D (2001) Traveling electrical waves in cortex: insights from phase dynamics and speculation on a computational role. Neuron 29:33–44. pmid:11182079
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Fourcaud N, Brunel N (2002) Dynamics of the firing probability of noisy integrate-and-fire neurons. Neural Comput 14:2057–2110. doi:10.1162/089976602320264015 pmid:12184844
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Gabbiani F (1996) Coding of time-varying signals in spike trains of linear and half-wave rectifying neurons. Network Comp Neural 7:61–85. doi:10.1162/neco.1996.8.1.44
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    Gerstner W, Kistler WM, Naud R, Paninski L (2014) Neuronal dynamics: from single neurons to networks and models of cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  26. ↵
    Gilbert CD, Sigman M (2007) Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron 54:677–696. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.019 pmid:17553419
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Goard M, Dan Y (2009) Basal forebrain activation enhances cortical coding of natural scenes. Nat Neurosci 12:1444–1449. doi:10.1038/nn.2402 pmid:19801988
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Harris KD, Thiele A (2011) Cortical state and attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:509–523. doi:10.1038/nrn3084 pmid:21829219
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Harrison PM, Badel L, Wall MJ, Richardson MJE (2015) Experimentally verified parameter sets for modelling heterogeneous neocortical pyramidal-cell populations. PLoS Comput Biol 11:e1004165. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004165
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  30. ↵
    Holcman D, Tsodyks M (2006) The emergence of up and down states in cortical networks. PLoS Comput Biol 2:e23. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020023 pmid:16557293
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Hopfield J, Brody C (2001) What is a moment? Transient synchrony as a collective mechanism for spatiotemporal integration. Proc Natl Acad Sci Usa USA 98:1282–1287 . doi:10.1073/pnas.98.3.1282
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    Johnson LA, Euston DR, Tatsuno M, McNaughton BL (2010) Stored-trace reactivation in rat prefrontal cortex is correlated with down-to-up state fluctuation density. J Neurosci 30:2650–2661. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1617-09.2010 pmid:20164349
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    Koepsell K, Wang X, Hirsch JA, Sommer FT (2010) Exploring the function of neural oscillations in early sensory systems. Front Neurosci 4:53. doi:10.3389/neuro.01.010.2010 pmid:20582272
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Kumar A, Rotter S, Aertsen A (2008) Conditions for propagating synchronous spiking and asynchronous firing rates in a cortical network model. J Neurosci 28:5268–5280. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2542-07.2008 pmid:18480283
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Larkum ME, Senn W, Lüscher HR (2004) Top-down dendritic input increases the gain of layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Cereb Cortex 14:1059–1070. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh065 pmid:15115747
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Latham PE, Richmond BJ, Nelson PG, Nirenberg S (2000) Intrinsic dynamics in neuronal networks. I. Theory. J Neurophysiol 83:808–827. doi:10.1152/jn.2000.83.2.808 pmid:10669496
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Lindner B (2016) Mechanisms of information filtering in neural systems. IEEE Trans Mol Biol Multi Scale Commun 2:5–15. doi:10.1109/TMBMC.2016.2618863
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  38. ↵
    Lindner B, Schimansky-Geier L (2001) Transmission of noise coded versus additive signals through a neuronal ensemble. Phys Rev Lett 86:2934–2937. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.86.2934 pmid:11290076
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B (2012) Slow dynamics and high variability in balanced cortical networks with clustered connections. Nat Neurosci 15:1498–1505. doi:10.1038/nn.3220 pmid:23001062
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Luczak A, Barthó P, Marguet SL, Buzsáki G, Harris KD (2007) Sequential structure of neocortical spontaneous activity in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:347–352. doi:10.1073/pnas.0605643104 pmid:17185420
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Luczak A, Bartho P, Harris KD (2013) Gating of sensory input by spontaneous cortical activity. J Neurosci 33:1684–1695. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2928-12.2013 pmid:23345241
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    Maquet P (2001) The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science 294:1048–1052 . doi:10.1126/science.1062856 pmid:11691982
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Marguet SL, Harris KD (2011) State-dependent representation of amplitude-modulated noise stimuli in rat auditory cortex. J Neurosci 31:6414–6420. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5773-10.2011 pmid:21525282
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    Mochol G, Hermoso-Mendizabal A, Sakata S, Harris KD, de la Rocha J (2015) Stochastic transitions into silence cause noise correlations in cortical circuits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:3529–3534. doi:10.1073/pnas.1410509112 pmid:25739962
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    Ostojic S (2014) Two types of asynchronous activity in networks of excitatory and inhibitory spiking neurons. Nat Neurosci 17:594–600. doi:10.1038/nn.3658 pmid:24561997
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Pachitariu M, Lyamzin DR, Sahani M, Lesica NA (2015) State-dependent population coding in primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci 35:2058–2073. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3318-14.2015 pmid:25653363
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    Petersen CC, Hahn TT, Mehta M, Grinvald A, Sakmann B (2003) Interaction of sensory responses with spontaneous depolarization in layer 2/3 barrel cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:13638–13643. doi:10.1073/pnas.2235811100 pmid:14595013
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    Poulet JFA, Fernandez LMJ, Crochet S, Petersen CCH (2012) Thalamic control of cortical states. Nat Neurosci 15:370–372. doi:10.1038/nn.3035 pmid:22267163
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Rasch B, Born J (2013) About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev 93:681–766. doi:10.1152/physrev.00032.2012 pmid:23589831
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Reig R, Sanchez-Vives MV (2007) Synaptic transmission and plasticity in an active cortical network. PLoS One 2:e670. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000670 pmid:17668052
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Reig R, Zerlaut Y, Vergara R, Destexhe A, Sanchez-Vives MV (2015) Gain modulation of synaptic inputs by network state in auditory cortex in vivo. J Neurosci 35:2689–2702. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2004-14.2015 pmid:25673859
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    Renart A, de la Rocha J, Bartho P, Hollender L, Parga N, Reyes A, Harris KD (2010) The asynchronous state in cortical circuits. Science 327:587–590. doi:10.1126/science.1179850
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    Richardson MJ, Swarbrick R (2010) Firing-rate response of a neuron receiving excitatory and inhibitory synaptic shot noise. Phys Rev Lett 105:178102. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.105.178102 pmid:21231083
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Sanchez-Vives MV, McCormick DA (2000) Cellular and network mechanisms of rhythmic recurrent activity in neocortex. Nat Neurosci 3:1027–1034. doi:10.1038/79848 pmid:11017176
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Shu Y, Hasenstaub A, McCormick DA (2003) Turning on and off recurrent balanced cortical activity. Nature 423:288–293. doi:10.1038/nature01616 pmid:12748642
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Singer W (2001) Consciousness and the binding problem. Ann NY Acad Sci 929:123–146. pmid:11349422
    OpenUrlPubMed
  57. ↵
    Staresina B, Bergmann T, Bonnefond M, van der Meij R, Jensen O, Deuker L, Elger C, Axmacher N, Fell J (2015) Hierarchical nesting of slow oscillations, spindles and ripples in the human hippocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci 18:1679–1686 . doi:10.1038/nn.4119 pmid:26389842
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Steriade M (2006) Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience 137:1087–1106. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.029 pmid:16343791
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F (1993) A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci 13:3252–3265. pmid:8340806
    OpenUrlAbstract
  60. ↵
    Steriade M, Timofeev I, Grenier F (2001) Natural waking and sleep states: a view from inside neocortical neurons. J Neurophysiol 85:1969–1985. doi:10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1969 pmid:11353014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Stern EA, Kincaid AE, Wilson CJ (1997) Spontaneous subthreshold membrane potential fluctuations and action potential variability of rat corticostriatal and striatal neurons in vivo. J Neurophysiol 77:1697–1715. doi:10.1152/jn.1997.77.4.1697 pmid:9114230
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Tononi G, Cirelli C (2006) Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev 10:49–62. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Vilela RD, Lindner B (2009) Comparative study of different integrate-and-fire neurons: spontaneous activity, dynamical response, and stimulus-induced correlation. Phys Rev E 80:031909. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.80.031909
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Vogels TP, Abbott LF (2005) Signal propagation and logic gating in networks of integrate-and-fire neurons. J Neurosci 25:10786–10795. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3508-05.2005 pmid:16291952
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    Vyazovskiy VV, Harris KD (2013) Sleep and the single neuron: the role of global slow oscillations in individual cell rest. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:443–451. doi:10.1038/nrn3494 pmid:23635871
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    Watson BO, Buzsáki G (2015) Sleep, memory & brain rhythms. Daedalus 144:67–82. doi:10.1162/DAED_a_00318 pmid:26097242
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Zagha E, Casale AE, Sachdev RN, McGinley MJ, McCormick DA (2013) Motor cortex feedback influences sensory processing by modulating network state. Neuron 79:567–578. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.008 pmid:23850595
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Tatyana Sharpee, The Salk Institute for Biological Studies

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Brent Doiron

The manuscript shows how brief epochs of high firing rates during up-down dynamics can offer improved coding over the low firing rates during asynchronous firing. Please discuss the contribution of brain rhythms other than slow waves that contribute to information transfer (spindles, hippocampal sharp wave, ripples). Please include references to papers on the role of sleep in memory consolidation.

Reviewer 1:

The authors also refer to quiet wakefulness as an example of state with UD. While occasional slow waves have been reported in awake, particularly after sleep deprivation, the quiet wakefulness is dominated by alpha (and some related) activities which are very different from SO.

Although the authors are upfront by stating that they compared two regimes (i.e., asynchronous irregular (AI) and (UD) at a fixed background rate, the motivation for having a uniform transition from AI to UD is not clear to me. Is there any evidence for switching between two states with same firing rates?

From Figure 2b it seems that firing rate is the same during Up states but the UD duty-cycle (ratio of up state to a cycle period) changes over time (probably because of exponential distributions). For such a waveform with no alteration in average firing rate, a constant ratio of up state to down state is required. Please clarify how you selected this ratio and confirm if this ratio was always the same throughout the paper. I wonder if changing this ratio affects the resulted mutual information rates.

Regardless of coherence computations, it is not conceptually clear why having pauses in background activity changes coherence (and information transfer rate) function from low-pass (high-pass) to band-pass. Adding an explanation would be helpful. Authors also concluded more information transfer was resulted by UD regime for lower background rates. Please provide a range. Does this range have overlap with UD state in the brain?

It seems that the statement of “the slower switching, the higher is the overall information rate” may not be a proper conclusion. Because, increasing an UD cycle period would probably also increase the up-state firing rate to keep the mean background rate unchanged. I suggest testing up/down duration effect in case of same up-state firing rates. Please also clarify what infinitely slow switching actually mean.

Minor comments

- Define c in equation (7). Is xn in equation (7) the one used in equation (3)?

- The exponent sign in Fourier transform formulation (in equation 8 and one before equation 10) should be negative.

- No statistical tests were used for performed comparisons (for example, comparisons in figures 2h, 4e, and 5).

Reviewer 2:

Overall, I agree with reviewer 1 that the simplifications place severe limitations on the overall impact of the work. Nonetheless, it is an important start that can be improved upon in subsequent research. I particularly like the interpretation of why “inattentive” states typically show up-down dynamics compared to low rate asynchrony, and how it also explains the higher degree of correlations during attention. A recent study that links up-down dynamics and attention that is relevant is:

Selective modulation of cortical state during spatial attention. Engel, Tatiana A and Steinmetz, Nicholas A and Gieselmann, Marc A and Thiele, Alexander and Moore, Tirin and Boahen, Kwabena, Science:354, 1140-1144, 2016.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 4 (6)
eNeuro
Vol. 4, Issue 6
November/December 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Up-Down-Like Background Spiking Can Enhance Neural Information Transmission
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Up-Down-Like Background Spiking Can Enhance Neural Information Transmission
Felix Droste, Benjamin Lindner
eNeuro 27 December 2017, 4 (6) ENEURO.0282-17.2017; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0282-17.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Up-Down-Like Background Spiking Can Enhance Neural Information Transmission
Felix Droste, Benjamin Lindner
eNeuro 27 December 2017, 4 (6) ENEURO.0282-17.2017; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0282-17.2017
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • Information Transmission
  • modelling
  • network
  • spontaneous activity
  • up-down States

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Show more Theory

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.