Abstract
Shank proteins, one of the principal scaffolds in the postsynaptic density (PSD) of the glutamatergic synapses, have been associated with autism spectrum disorders and neuropsychiatric diseases. However, it is not known whether different Shank family proteins have distinct functions in regulating synaptic transmission, and how they differ from other scaffold proteins in this aspect. Here, we investigate the role of Shanks in regulating glutamatergic synaptic transmission at rat hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses, using lentivirus-mediated knockdown and molecular replacement combined with dual whole-cell patch clamp in hippocampal slice culture. In line with previous findings regarding PSD-MAGUK scaffold manipulation, we found that loss of scaffold proteins via knockdown of Shank1 or Shank2, but not Shank3, led to a reduction of the number but not the unitary response of AMPAR-containing synapses. Only when both Shank1 and Shank2 were knocked down, were both the number and the unitary response of active synapses reduced. This reduction was accompanied by a decrease in NMDAR-mediated synaptic response, indicating more profound deficits in synaptic transmission. Molecular replacement with Shank2 and Shank3c rescued the synaptic transmission to the basal level, and the intact sterile α-motif (SAM) of Shank proteins is required for maintaining glutamatergic synaptic transmission. We also found that altered neural activity did not influence the effect of Shank1 or Shank2 knockdown on AMPAR synaptic transmission, in direct contrast to the activity dependence of the effect of PSD-95 knockdown, revealing differential interaction between activity-dependent signaling and scaffold protein families in regulating synaptic AMPAR function.
Significance Statement
Postsynaptic scaffold proteins at the glutamatergic synapses include several specific families, of which, many genes are associated with neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric disorders. The functional significance and diversity of these scaffolds remain to be elucidated. Here, we investigate how scaffold proteins, Shanks, regulate hippocampal SC-CA1 synaptic transmission. We found loss of different Shank proteins led to different degrees of deficit in AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission, with the unitary response of AMPAR-containing synapses prioritized to be maintained. Additionally, altered neural activity did not influence the effect of Shank knockdown on AMPAR synaptic transmission, in contrast to the effect of PSD-95 knockdown, indicating differential interaction between neuronal activity and scaffold proteins in regulating synaptic AMPAR function.
Introduction
The postsynaptic density (PSD) comprises scaffold proteins that interact with each other to maintain the structural stability of the postsynaptic configuration, while organizing the receptor complexes and postsynaptic signaling cascades important for activity-dependent modification of mammalian glutamatergic synapses (Scannevin and Huganir, 2000; Kim and Sheng, 2004; Kennedy et al., 2005). Shank (SH3 and multiple ankyrin repeat domains protein) proteins are multidomain structural proteins enriched in the PSD of excitatory synapses (Naisbitt et al., 1999; Rostaing et al., 2006), forming a macro-molecular complex with other PSD-enriched molecules (Tu et al., 1999; Sheng and Kim, 2000; Grabrucker et al., 2011a). It has been hypothesized that, through this multitude of molecular interactions, Shank family proteins scaffold ionotropic and metabotrophic glutamate receptors to cytoskeletal components, thereby regulating synaptic morphology and synaptic function (Scannevin and Huganir, 2000; Kim and Sheng, 2004; Kennedy et al., 2005; Frost et al., 2010; Grabrucker et al., 2011b). Supporting this hypothesis, manipulating Shank family proteins results in changes in synapse development, spine structure, PSD organization, synaptic glutamate receptor levels and synaptic transmission (Sala et al., 2001; Grabrucker et al., 2011a). Three individual genes encode Shank family proteins: Shank1, Shank2, and Shank3. Shank1 has been proposed to be a master regulator of the synaptic scaffold (Ehlers, 2003). All Shanks have been associated with neurologic diseases such as schizophrenia and autism (Durand et al., 2007; Gauthier et al., 2010; Grabrucker et al., 2011a; Leblond et al., 2014), but the severity of the phenotype seems to be gene specific. It is not known whether different Shank family members have distinct or overlapping functions, and how they differ from other scaffold proteins in regulating synaptic transmission. Understanding their individual and distinct roles in regulating synaptic transmission could provide critical insight into mechanisms of glutamatergic synaptic function under normal and pathologic conditions.
Several lines of mice have been generated to genetically ablate specific Shank genes and/or their splice isoforms. These lines of mice show an array of phenotypes including defects in basal synaptic transmission (for review, see Jiang and Ehlers, 2013). However, the resulting phenotypes were not consistent with each other. This apparent inconsistency may be due to different targeting strategies, different brain regions and developmental stages analyzed, and possible developmental and activity-dependent compensation. To circumvent complications inherent to these approaches, we sought a different approach to compare the principal contributions of each Shank family protein in a systematic manner. We used a lentivirus-mediated gene knockdown to down-regulate the expression in hippocampal CA1 neurons in organotypic slice cultures, and then tested synaptic transmission at hippocampal Schaffer Collateral-CA1 synapses. With its defined structure, the hippocampus allows manipulation of postsynaptic proteins without influencing the target proteins in the presynaptic neurons. The organotypic slice culture allows dual whole-cell patch clamping to measure evoked excitatory synaptic transmission in adjacent infected and uninfected neurons stimulated by the same set of axonal afferents. Furthermore, the organotypic slice culture permits the use of chronic pharmacological treatment to study the interactions between neuronal activity and our molecular manipulations.
Materials and Methods
Virus preparation and infection
All lentiviral constructs were modified from the original lentiviral transfer vector FUGW (Lois et al., 2002), and its variant FHUG+W with an additional RNAi expression cassette driven by an H1 promoter (Schlüter et al., 2006). Lentiviral constructs were modified to target mRNA sequences of Shank1 (GGGTTGAAGAAGTTCCTTGAA), Shank2 (GGGCACAGGATGAACATAGAA), Shank3 (shShank3, CCCTCTTTGTGGATGTGCAAA, shShank3 alternative GGCCAGGAATGTTGCATGAAT in the 3’-UTR), or a common sequence between Shank1 and Shank3 mRNA (GACAAGGGGCTGGACCCCAAT). Constructs also contained ubiquitin promoter-driven eGFP or tdTomato (tdT), which allowed identification of infected cells. Superinfection with both eGFP and tdT viruses allowed multiple combinations of Shank knockdowns to be performed. Shank2 cDNA with silent mutations in the shShank2 targeting site (ggCcaTCgCatgaaTatCgaG) was fused to the C-terminus of eGFP in respective lentiviral vectors to construct replacement vectors. Shank3c cDNA was cloned in the similar fashion with no silent mutation introduced, as shShank13 targets the Shank3 sequence that is not present Shank3c isoform. To produce the lentiviruses, the transfer vectors and the HIV-1 packaging vectors (pRSV/REV, pMDLg/pRRE, and the VSV-G envelope glycoprotein vector (Dull et al., 1998) were cotransfected into HEK293T fibroblasts (ATCC, RRID: CVCL_0063) using the FUGENE6 transfection reagent (Promega). Supernatants of culture media were collected 60 hours after transfection, and then centrifuged at 50,000 × g to concentrate the viral particles.
Dissociated cortical neuron cultures
Dissociated cortical cultures were prepared from P1 Sprague Dawley rat pups of either sex. The cortical hemispheres were dissected out and digested with papain for 20 min at 37°C, according to the protocol followed by (Schlüter et al., 2006). To infect cortical cultures, 1.5 μl of concentrated viral aliquot were dispensed into 2 ml of culture media per well of a 12-well plate, at 7 days in vitro (DIV7) and collected after DIV17. Cells were washed with ice-cold PBS, and lysed with homogenization buffer (4 mM HEPES, pH 7.4, 0.32 M sucrose, 2 mM EGTA, and protease inhibitors). The homogenate was centrifuged at 800 × g for 10 min at 4°C, after which the supernatant was centrifuged again at 10,000 × g for 15 min at 4°C. This second pellet (P2) was used for Western blot analyses.
HEK293T fibroblast cultures
HEK293T fibroblasts were cultured in DMEM supplemented with 10% FBS, and transfected with a Shank3 expressing plasmid and either GFP, shShank1, shShank2, shShank3 or shShank13 expressing vectors. The cell lysates were collected in standard protein sample buffers 48 hours post transfection and subjected for Western blot analyses.
Hippocampal slice cultures
Hippocampi of P7 Sprague Dawley rat pups of either sex were isolated and slice cultures were prepared following a published protocol (Liu et al., 2014). When slices were treated pharmacologically, 20 μM bicuculline (Tocris) or 25 μM D-APV (Tocris) was included in the media 2 d after virus injection, and bicuculline or D-APV was present until the day of recording. To infect hippocampal slice cultures, concentrated viral solutions were injected into the CA1 pyramidal cell layer using a Nanojector (Drummond). To achieve superinfection, lentivirus particles were super-concentrated at 4-fold. Equal volume of two different lentiviruses were mixed and coinjected.
Western blotting
The following primary antibodies were used: anti-Shank1 (1:200, Abcam, catalog #ab154224), anti-Shank2 (1:100, Cell Signaling, catalog #12218), anti-Shank3 (1:400, Santa Cruz, RRID: AB_2301759), anti-PanShank (1:1000, Neuromab, RRID: AB_10674115), and anti-actin (1:3000, Sigma, RRID: AB_476697). IRDye 800CW and 680LT Secondary antibodies (Licor) were used at 1:5000 dilution for detection on an Odyssey IR laser Scanner (Licor). All anti-Shank signals were normalized to the actin signal. Data from infected neurons were compared to data from uninfected neurons within the same batch. Statistical significance was estimated with Student’s t test between infected and uninfected neuron cultures.
Electrophysiology
All experiments were performed at 29-30°C, after slices had been infected for 5-8 d. Recording conditions followed from published studies (Liu et al., 2014). For evoked EPSC (eEPSC) recordings, neurons were recorded under voltage-clamp configuration in ACSF containing: 119 mM NaCl, 26 mM NaHCO3, 10 mM glucose, 2.5 mM KCl, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 4 mM MgSO4, and 4 mM CaCl2, saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2 and supplemented with 1μM 2-Chloroadenosine, 50 μM picrotoxin. The patch pipette (4.5–7 MΩ) solution contained: 115 mM CsMeSO3, 20 mM CsCl, 10 mM HEPES, 4 mM MgCl2, 4 mM NaATP, 0.4 mM NaGTP, 10 mM sodium phosphocreatine, 5 mM QX-314, and 0.5 mM EGTA, pH 7.3. For mini EPSC recordings, ACSF was additionally supplemented with 1μM tetrodotoxin, 50 μM D-APV and 50 mM sucrose. The patch pipette solution contained: 130 mM CsMeSO3, 20 mM CsCl, 10 mM HEPES, 6 mM MgCl2, 2 mM NaATP, 0.3 mM NaGTP, 5 mM sodium phosphocreatine, 5 mM QX-314, and 5 mM EGTA, pH 7.3. For both eEPSCs and mEPSCs, data were collected using a MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), digitized at 10 kHz with the A/D converter ITC-18 computer interface (Heka Instruments). Data were acquired and analyzed on-line using custom routines written with Igor Pro software (Wavemetrics). Input and series resistances were monitored throughout the recordings. mEPSCs were analyzed off line with Mini Analysis Program (Synaptosoft) using a threshold of 6 pA.
For both eEPSC and mEPSC statistical analyses between pair-recorded uninfected and infected neurons, significance was estimated with a two-tailed, paired Student’s t test. The neurons from each pair were exposed to the same dissection, culture, and injection procedures (mEPSC and eEPSC), and the same stimulated afferent input (eEPSC), therefore paired analyses were used for these analyses to control the experimental conditions. Significance was determined at p < 0.05. When plotting eEPSCs ratios across experimental conditions, averages of ratios of infected and uninfected cell pairs were logarithmically transformed and presented as back-transformed mean ± SEM. Statistical significance from the described paired t test above was shown on top of the bar.
Results
Lentivirus-mediated knockdown of specific Shank family proteins
To determine the role of Shank proteins in maintaining synaptic transmission, we used a lentivirus-mediated shRNA knockdown to reduce Shank levels in neurons. For our knockdown experiments, we designed lentiviral constructs containing shRNAs targeting either rat Shank1 (shShank1), Shank2 (shShank2), Shank3 (shShank3), or an shRNA targeting both Shank1 and Shank3 (shShank13). We screened five to eight shRNA sequences for each of the Shank genes, and identified at least one effective shRNA construct for each of the Shank genes. The targeting regions of the effective shRNAs in selective Shank isoforms are shown in Figure 1A. The constructs also expressed a fluorescent protein such as eGFP or tdT to allow visual identification of infected cells (Fig. 1B).
To confirm the specificity and efficacy of the shRNAs, we infected dissociated cortical neuron cultures with lentiviruses containing the shRNAs. Infected cultures were analyzed using Western blotting to detect Shank1, Shank2, and Shank3 levels in the synaptoneurosome fraction (P2), using actin as a loading control (Fig. 1C). A panShank antibody was also used to assay overall Shank protein levels, with the caveat that the affinity of the PanShank antibody to different Shank isoforms was unknown. Quantification of the blots showed that infection with control viruses (labeled as GFP or tdT) containing the H1 promoter but not specific shRNAs did not reduce Shank protein levels in the synaptosomal fraction as compared to uninfected cultures, indicating viral infection alone had little effect on the expression of Shank proteins. As expected, infection with shShank1 or shShank3 viruses reduced only the levels of their respective target proteins. Similarly, infection with the shShank2 viruses effectively reduced the levels of Shank2 (Fig. 1D). Interestingly, shShank2 expression also led to small but significantly reduced levels of Shank3.
To test whether this decrease of Shank3 expression by shShank2 was due to an off-target effect, we cotransfected human embryonic kidney (HEK) cells with a full-length rat Shank3-expressing construct and various shRNA expressing constructs. Only coexpression with Shank3 targeting shRNAs (shShank3 and shShank13) reduced HEK cell expression of Shank3, but not coexpression of shShank1, shShank2 or GFP (Fig. 1E), suggesting shShank2 does not have an off-target effect on Shank3 expression. The decrease of Shank3 levels accompanying shShank2 expression is thus most likely a functional consequence of reduced Shank2 levels in the synapses.
We also designed an shRNA targeting a common sequence shared by Shank1 and Shank3. The virus expressing this shRNA (shShank13) effectively reduced the levels of Shank1 and Shank3 but not that of Shank2 (Fig. 1B). Finally, superinfecting the neurons with shShank13 and shShank2 (shShank13 + 2) significantly decreased levels of all three Shanks as expected (Fig. 1C,D). This superinfection allowed us to assay the functional consequence of decreasing most if not all Shank proteins in synaptic transmission.
Knockdown of Shank1 or Shank2 reduces AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission by reducing the number of active synapses
To determine the effect of Shank knockdown on synaptic transmission, we injected shRNA-containing lentiviruses into the CA1 region of cultured hippocampal slices and recorded excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) from CA1 pyramidal cells. We recorded simultaneously from one infected cell and a neighboring uninfected cell to directly compare their responses to the same stimulation. It has been shown that lentivirus-mediated expression of GFP or other fluorescent proteins does not influence basal EPSCs (Nakagawa et al., 2004; Elias et al., 2006; Schlüter et al., 2006). In an additional control experiment, we verified that AMPAR-mediated eEPSCs (AMPAR eEPSCs) and NMDAR-mediated eEPSCs (NMDAR eEPSCs) were not affected in cells superinfected with GFP and tdT viruses that contained the H1 promoter cassette without effective shRNAs (AMPAR eEPSCs, n = 12 pairs, control, −45.5 ± 5.7 pA; infected, −46.0 ± 15.7 pA, p = 0.97; NMDAR eEPSCs, n = 11 pairs, control, 21.0 ± 3.3 pA, infected, 20.6 ± 3.0 pA, p = 0.83, graph not shown).
The expression of shShank1 reduced AMPAR eEPSCs but not NMDAR eEPSCs (shShank1, AMPAR eEPSCs, n = 21 pairs, control, −45.2 ± 4.5 pA; infected, −22.4 ± 2.0 pA, p < 0.0001; NMDAR eEPSCs, n = 18 pairs, control, 33.7 ± 4.1 pA, infected, 35.7 ± 4.3 pA, p = 0.64; Fig. 2Aa,Ab). To determine whether the decrease in AMPAR eEPSCs is due to a decrease in the number of AMPAR-containing synapses (active synapses), or a decrease in the unitary strength of active synapses, we measured AMPAR-mediated excitatory miniature EPSCs (AMPAR mEPSCs). Our results show that knockdown of Shank1 reduced mEPSC frequency but not mEPSC amplitude (shShank1, n = 11 pairs; amplitude, control, 17.8 ± 1.6 pA; infected, 17.2 ± 1.5 pA, p = 0.16; frequency, control, 3.0 ± 0.4 Hz, infected, 2.5 ± 0.3 Hz, p < 0.05; Fig. 2Ac). The mEPSC frequency can be influenced by both the number of AMPAR-containing active synapses and the presynaptic release probability. The paired-pulse ratio (PPR), which can be used to estimate the presynaptic release probability, was not significantly different between uninfected and infected neurons (Fig. 2E). Thus, our data indicate that decreasing Shank1 levels decreases AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission through a reduction of the number of active synapses, without significantly affecting unitary synaptic strength.
Similar to shShank1, decreasing Shank2 expression levels with shShank2 also reduced AMPAR eEPSCs but not NMDAR eEPSCs (AMPAR eEPSCs, n = 32 pairs, control, −41.7 ± 3.3 pA, infected, −30.8 ± 3.5 pA, p < 0.005; NMDAR eEPSCs, n = 28 pairs, control, 36.3 ± 4.5 pA, infected, 30.3 ± 3.7 pA, p = 0.15; Fig. 2Ba,Bb), reduced mEPSC frequency but not mEPSC amplitude (n = 11 pairs, amplitude, control, 20.4 ± 1.2 pA, infected, 18.7 ± 1.1 pA, p = 0.31; frequency, control, 2.7 ± 0.5 Hz, infected, 2.1 ± 0.4 Hz, p < 0.05; Fig. 2Bc), with no difference in PPR between uninfected and infected neurons (Fig. 2E). Thus, decreasing either Shank1 or Shank2 levels decreases AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission through a reduction of the number of active synapses.
In contrast to Shank1 or Shank2 knockdown, Shank3 knockdown had no effect on either AMPAR or NMDAR eEPSCs (shShank3, n = 14 pairs; AMPAR eEPSCs, control, −33.3 ± 5.6 pA; infected, −27.1 ± 4.1 pA, p = 0.31; NMDAR eEPSCs, control, 38.2 ± 5.2 pA, infected, 30.6 ± 5.6 pA, p = 0.28; Fig. 2Ca,Cb) and also did not affect mEPSCs (shShank3, n = 10 pairs; amplitude, control, 22.4 ± 1.6 pA; infected, 22.7 ± 1.1 pA, p = 0.33; frequency, control, 2.2 ± 0.4 Hz, infected, 2.0 ± 0.2 Hz, p = 0.88; Fig. 2Cc). An alternative shRNA to Shank3 with similar knockdown efficiency was also used and produced similar results (shShank3_2, n = 12 pairs; AMPAR eEPSCs, control, −38.1 ± 6.8 pA; infected, −32.4 ± 5.4 pA, p = 0.38; NMDAR eEPSCs, control, 37.7 ± 11.01 pA, infected, 44.2 ± 12.5 pA, p = 0.69). Furthermore, simultaneous knockdown of Shank1 and Shank3 (shShank13) produced results similar to those of shShank1 (shShank13, n = 17 pairs; AMPAR eEPSCs, control, −48.5 ± 4.5 pA; infected, −28.4 ± 5.9 pA, p < 0.01; NMDAR eEPSCs, control, 25.8 ± 5.5 pA, infected, 24.2 ± 4.4 pA, p = 0.63; mini AMPAR EPSCs, n = 10 pairs; amplitude, control, 16.7 ± 1.2 pA; infected, 14.7 ± 1.5 pA, p = 0.13; frequency, control, 1.2 ± 0.2 Hz, infected, 0.9 ± 0.2 Hz, p < 0.05; Fig. 2D). These results suggest that reducing Shank3 levels has little effect on synaptic transmission at Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapses in our experimental conditions.
Simultaneous knockdown of Shank1 and Shank2 further decreases synaptic transmission with decreased unitary active synapse response and NMDAR-mediated response
To further investigate the role of Shanks in synaptic transmission, we knocked down all three Shanks using a superinfection of shShank13 and shShank2. shShank13 was expressed in a construct with eGFP, while shShank2 was expressed with the red fluorescent protein, tdT. When all three Shanks were knocked down, we saw a decrease in both AMPAR and NMDAR eEPSCs (shShank13 + 2, n = 10 pairs; AMPAR eEPSCs, control, −41.2 ± 5.9 pA; infected, −11.2 ± 2.5 pA, p < 0.01; NMDAR eEPSCs, control, 31.8 ± 8.9 pA, infected, 11.7 ± 3.0 pA, p < 0.01; Fig. 3Aa,Ab). Furthermore, both mEPSC frequency and amplitude were decreased (shShank13 + 2, n = 10 pairs; amplitude, control, 21.0 ± 2.4 pA; infected, 15.7 ± 1.2 pA, p < 0.05; frequency, control, 1.9 ± 0.2 Hz, infected, 1.1 ± 0.2 Hz, p < 0.01; Fig. 3Ac). These results show that knockdown of all three Shanks leads to a loss of both the number and the unitary strength of active synapses, and that this severe loss of Shank scaffolding is sufficient to lead to a loss of NMDAR-mediated response.
Since Shank3 knockdown did not affect synaptic transmission when Shank1 and Shank2 were intact, we asked whether the effect of shShank13 + 2 on synaptic transmission was primarily due to the loss of Shank1 and Shank2. We achieved a double knockdown by expressing shShank1 in a construct with eGFP while expressing shShank2 with tdT, which allow us to identify double knockdown cells as both green and red. When both Shank1 and Shank2 were knocked down, both AMPAR and NMDAR eEPSCs were still decreased (shShank1 + 2, AMPAR eEPSCs, n = 13 pairs, control, −84.6 ± 15.3 pA; infected, −24.9 ± 3.6 pA, p < 0.01; NMDAR eEPSCs, n = 10 pairs, control, 38.0 ± 9.4 pA, infected, 24.0 ± 4.6 pA, p < 0.05; Fig. 3B), similar to shShank13 + 2. PPR was not affected by the superinfection (Fig. 2E). Collectively, these results indicate that Shank1 and Shank2 are the two principal Shank family scaffold proteins maintaining the synaptic transmission in hippocampal CA1 neurons in our experimental conditions (Fig. 3C).
Altered neuronal activity does not influence the reduction of AMPAR eEPSCs caused by Shank1 and Shank2 knockdown
It has been shown previously, in hippocampal slice culture, that knockdown of the prominent scaffold protein PSD-95 produced similar effects to knockdown of Shank1 or Shank2. In particular, AMPAR eEPSCs were decreased but NMDAR eEPSCs were unaffected (Schlüter et al., 2006), and mEPSC frequency but not amplitude was decreased (Liu et al., 2014). Treating the hippocampal slice culture with bicuculline could rescue the decrease of AMPAR eEPSCs caused by knockdown of PSD-95 (Schlüter et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2014). Because bicuculline increases excitatory drive among neurons by blocking inhibitory synaptic transmission, these results indicate that the decrease of AMPAR mediated synaptic transmission caused by PSD-95 knockdown can be rescued in an activity-dependent manner.
To test whether the decrease in AMPAR-mediated transmission caused by Shank knockdown is similarly regulated by neuronal activity, we treated hippocampal slices with pharmacological reagents for several days before electrophysiological recordings. In slices treated with bicuculline and infected with viruses containing either shShank1 or shShank2, AMPAR eEPSC amplitudes in infected neurons were still decreased compared to those in the neighboring uninfected neurons (shShank1, n = 17 pairs, control, −41.8 ± 5.1 pA; infected, −29.6 ± 3.7 pA, p < 0.05; shShank2, n = 22 pairs, control, −32.0 ± 3.6 pA; infected, −25.0 ± 2.8 pA, p < 0.05; Fig. 4A,B). Activity-dependent rescue of AMPAR eEPSCs following PSD-95 knockdown was still observed in parallel sister cultures (data not shown). These results indicate that elevating excitatory neuronal activity in slice culture does not influence the reduction of AMPAR eEPSCs caused by acute knockdown of Shank1 and Shank2 (Fig. 4C), unlike the activity-dependent effects of PSD-95 knockdown on AMPAR eEPSCs. These results indicate a functional divergence between PSD-95 and Shank family proteins in response to activity-dependent signaling pathways.
The C-terminal SAM domain is critical for the effect of Shank2 on synaptic transmission
To further investigate the molecular mechanism underlying Shank-dependent regulation of synaptic transmission, we used a lentiviral molecular replacement vector (Schlüter et al., 2006) to overexpress Shank2 with simultaneous expression of shShank2, and examined the effects of Shank2 replacement on Shank levels and synaptic transmission. To allow for expression of recombinant Shank2 in the same cells expressing shShank2, the shShank2 target sequence in the recombinant Shank2 was silently mutated. The prototypic isoform of Shank2 is smaller compared to other isoforms of Shanks (Fig. 1A,C), which allowing to fit the coding region fused to GFP in the lentiviral molecular replacement vector.
We further used the molecular replacement system to examine the functional role of certain Shank protein domains. In particular, previous studies have shown that the C-terminal sterile α-motif (SAM) domain is critical for multimerization of Shank proteins (Naisbitt et al., 1999), and for synaptic localization of Shank2 and Shank3 (Boeckers et al., 2005). We therefore generated a Shank2 mutant lacking the SAM domain (ΔSAM; Fig. 5A) to test whether the SAM domain is important for the synaptic effects of Shank2.
In infected cortical cultures, both molecular replacement viruses efficiently silenced endogenous Shank2 while expressing recombinant GFP-tagged proteins at much higher levels comparable to the endogenous Shank2 levels in GFP-only expressing cultures (Fig. 5B). We also examined the levels of Shank3 in the synaptoneurosomal fraction from cultures infected with the replacement viruses. Replacing endogenous Shank2 with the wild-type Shank2 rescued the decrease of Shank3 levels seen with shShank2 expression (Figs. 1C,D, 5B ). This observation (Fig. 5B) indicates that the decrease of Shank3 with shShank2 expression was due to insufficient Shank2 levels.
Next, we tested the effect of Shank2 and ΔSAM replacement of endogenous Shank2 on synaptic transmission. Replacement with a wild-type Shank2 rescued AMPAR eEPSCs to the control level (shShank2 to Shank2 replacement, n = 14 pairs, control, −37.8 ± 3.2 pA; infected, −34.8 ± 3.8 pA, p = 0.42; Fig. 5C). AMPAR mEPSC frequency was rescued with no changes in mEPSC amplitude (shShank2 to Shank2 replacement, n = 8 pairs; amplitude, control, 18.8 ± 1.6 pA; infected, 14.7 ± 1.1 pA, p = 0.11; frequency, control, 1.8± 0.3 Hz, infected, 1.5 ± 0.3 Hz, p = 0.20). Despite our observation that exogenous Shank2 was expressed at a much higher level than the endogenous Shank2 level, AMPAR eEPSCs were rescued only to the control levels, not higher. This result indicates that this isoform of Shank2 is sufficient for maintaining basal synaptic AMPAR levels, but other factors are required for further enhancing the strength of AMPAR-mediated synaptic responses.
In contrast, replacement with ΔSAM did not rescue the decrease in AMPAR eEPSCs caused by shShank2 (Shank2 to ΔSAM replacement, n = 17 pairs; AMPAR eEPSCs, control, −62.7 ± 8.0 pA; infected, −46.5 ± 6.0 pA, p = 0.04; Fig. 5D). This result indicates that the SAM domain is important for mediating the effect of Shank2 on synaptic response, presumably via multimerization of Shank proteins (Naisbitt et al., 1999; Boeckers et al., 2005). Maintaining synaptic Shank2 levels with intact SAM domains in CA1 neurons is thus important for proper glutamatergic synaptic transmission and maintaining Shank levels in the synaptic compartment at the analyzed developmental stage.
Short isoform of Shank3 rescues synaptic deficit caused by knocking down Shank1 or Shank2
Although shShank3 had no significant effect on the excitatory synaptic transmission in hippocampal slice cultures, it is likely that Shank3 can sufficiently support excitatory synaptic transmission. We overexpressed a Shank3 isoform Shank3c (Fig. 6A), similar to the prototypic Shank2 isoform, in the background of shShank2, and shShank13, and examined the effects of Shank3 replacement on Shank levels and synaptic transmission. As expected, in infected cortical cultures, both molecular replacement viruses efficiently silenced perspective endogenous Shank targets. GFP-tagged Shank3c proteins were expressed at much higher levels comparable to the endogenous Shank3 levels in GFP-only expressing cultures (Fig. 6B,C).
Next, we tested the effect of Shank3c replacement of endogenous Shank1 and Shank3 (shShank13) or Shank2 (shShank2) on synaptic transmission. Replacement Shank1 and Shank3 with Shank3c rescued AMPAR eEPSCs to the control level (shShank13 to Shank3 replacement, n = 6 pairs, control, −143.1 ± 23.7 pA; infected, −134.4 ± 13.5 pA, p = 0.73; Fig. 6D). In addition, replacement Shank2 with Shank3c rescued AMPAR eEPSCs to the control level (shShank2 to Shank3 replacement, n = 12 pairs, control, −116.1 ± 13.1 pA; infected, −108.4 ± 12.8 pA, p = 0.41; Fig. 6E). Similar the Shank2 replacement experiments, despite that exogenous Shank3 was expressed at a high level, AMPAR eEPSCs were rescued only to the control levels, not higher. These results indicate that Shank protein levels are required for maintaining basal synaptic AMPAR levels. The short isoforms of Shanks are sufficient for maintaining the basal synaptic function, but are not the rate limiting factor for further enhancing the strength of AMPAR-mediated synaptic responses.
Discussion
In this study, we explored the role of the Shank family PSD scaffold proteins in regulating synaptic transmission at hippocampal Schaffer Collateral-CA1 synapses in the organotypic slice culture preparation. Our work lends functional support to the role of Shanks as critical proteins in the PSD scaffold (Naisbitt et al., 1999; Ehlers, 2003; Romorini et al., 2004; Rostaing et al., 2006). It has been shown that PSD-95 and Shank proteins are assembled together via SAPAP family proteins (Romorini et al., 2004), and that this tri-partner interaction is the core component of the PSD (Chen et al., 2008). Our studies show that the effect of knocking down Shank1 or Shank2 on synaptic transmission is similar to the effect of knocking down the PSD-MAGUK family proteins, with significant impact on the number of AMPAR-containing synapses, rather than the quantal size (Béïque et al., 2006; Elias et al., 2006; Ehrlich et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2014; Levy et al., 2015). These parallel observations point toward a general mechanism: when scaffold components are limited, neurons prioritize to maintain unitary synaptic strength of remaining active synapses at the expense of the number of active synapses (Levy et al., 2015). This preferential maintenance of synaptic strength in a subpopulation of active synapses suggests that a selection process may be at play. It remains unknown whether the signaling cascade including L-type calcium channels, CaM kinase activity and the GriA2 AMPAR subunit, involved in the synapse consolidation seen with PSD-MAGUK manipulation (Levy et al., 2015) is also at play with Shank manipulation.
Double knockdown of Shank1 and Shank2 and triple knockdown of all Shanks led to new phenotypes in synaptic transmission, including decreased unitary synaptic AMPAR mediated response measured by mini amplitudes, in addition to a profound decrease in numbers of active synapses, and also decreased NMDAR eEPSC responses, suggesting an essential role of Shank proteins for maintenance of glutamatergic synaptic transmission. Knocking down Shank3 had little effect on synaptic transmission in our experimental paradigm. This finding agrees with results from some Shank3 mutant lines tested in the hippocampus of juvenile animals (Peça et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2011), at a similar developmental stage to our preparation. It is possible that the lack of effect of Shank3 is due to different expression levels of Shank proteins at hippocampus and/or a potential dominant effect of Shank1 and Shank2 on regulating synaptic transmission at this developmental stage in hippocampal slice cultures. At striatal synapses (Peça et al., 2011) or adult hippocampal synapses (Yang et al., 2012), Shank3 may play a more important role in regulating synaptic AMPAR function.
Further studies need to be done to determine how the factors including gene-dosage, different knockdown methods (Levy et al., 2015), different developmental stage and different brain region may influence the effect of manipulation of Shank proteins on excitatory synaptic transmission. Different knockdown methods and the relative amount of endogenous proteins in different brain regions and at different developmental stage may influence the protein depletion rate and efficiency, which can potentially influence the effect on synaptic transmission.
Although we observed a synaptic phenotype at basal neural activity levels with manipulation of Shank family proteins similar to manipulations of PSD-MAGUK family proteins (Figs. 2, 4; Elias et al., 2006; Schlüter et al., 2006), Shank proteins are functionally distinct from other scaffold proteins in terms of activity-dependent regulation of synaptic transmission. In particular, the decreased AMPAR eEPSCs resulting from Shank knockdown could not be rescued with increased excitatory neuronal activity, unlike activity-dependent rescue of PSD-95 knockdown (Schlüter et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2014). Blocking NMDAR activity with D-APV also did not influence the effect of shShank2 on synaptic transmission (data not shown), indicating these alterations in neuronal activity do not play a significant role in Shank2-dependent regulation of AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission. It is possible that Shank family proteins serve as the structural core of the scaffold, and the lack of Shank proteins cannot be compensated by activity-dependent AMPAR trafficking and interaction with PSD-MAGUKs. Alternatively, bicuculline- and D-APV-induced signaling events are specific for PSD-MAGUK family proteins, while Shank family proteins are targeted via other signaling cascades.
Shank2 and Shank3c were sufficient to rescue the synaptic deficit caused by decreasing Shank proteins, whereas Shank2ΔSAM, a mutant that was previously shown to perturb synaptic localization and functions of Shank2 (Naisbitt et al., 1999; Boeckers et al., 2005), was not sufficient. Together, these results suggest SAM domain-mediated interactions may play an important role in stabilizing synaptic scaffolds and exerting the effects of Shank2 on synaptic transmission.
In conclusion, we have shown the importance of Shank proteins in regulating synaptic transmission, demonstrating the functional divergence of Shank family members from each other and from PSD-MAGUK scaffold proteins in the hippocampal SC-CA1 synapse.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank Stacey Meyers and Dr. Henny Haensgen for their excellent technical support; all members of the Xu lab for their guidance during our research; and Dr. Elly Nedivi, Dr. Robert C. Makenka, Dr. Shan Wei, and Xu lab members for helpful comments
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant MH080310 (to W.X.), the Simons Center for the Social Brain at MIT, and the Phelan-McDermid Syndrome Foundation.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.