Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleOpinion, Cognition and Behavior

An Emerging Field of Primate Social Neurophysiology: Current Developments

Steve W. C. Chang
eNeuro 29 September 2017, 4 (5) ENEURO.0295-17.2017; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0295-17.2017
Steve W. C. Chang
1Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
2Department of Neuroscience, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510
3Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Steve W. C. Chang
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Recently, there has been increased interest in investigating neurophysiological mechanisms underlying social interactions using a nonhuman primate model system. Several studies in this subfield, known as primate social neurophysiology, have begun to provide novel insights into how single neurons encode socially-relevant variables. This opinion piece intends to provide insight into the state of this field. In doing so, it discusses some common principles learned from primate social neurophysiology experiments.

  • agency
  • allocentric representation
  • egocentric representation
  • nonhuman primates
  • social neurophysiology

Significance Statement

Recently, there has been increased interest in investigating neurophysiological mechanisms underlying social interactions using a nonhuman primate model system. Several studies in this subfield, known as primate social neurophysiology, have begun to provide novel insights into how single neurons encode socially-relevant variables. This opinion piece intends to provide insight into the state of this field. In doing so, it discusses some common principles learned from primate social neurophysiology experiments.

Introduction

Neurophysiologists are interested in discovering the neural codes that are used by the brain to represent variables from the sensory environments, and to plan and execute motor actions following wide-ranging cognitive operations. In the past decade or so, there has been an increasing trend in neurophysiological research to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms of social interaction. This is particularly true of nonhuman primate research, where exploiting the rich social behavioral repertoires of nonhuman primates has allowed researchers to focus on assessing how neurons from various brain regions signal information used for or as a consequence of social behavior. This research endeavor is often referred to as social neurophysiology. This opinion, by way of examining selected empirical results in rhesus macaques, discusses some common principles in social neurophysiology gained by neuronal recording experiments involving nonhuman primate social interactions. Specifically, this opinion narrates the knowledge gained so far indicating that social variables are represented in several parts of the brain involved in motivation and affective processing by way of agent-specific reference frames, contextual gain modulations, and, in some cases, privileged information processing, and mirroring.

Nonhuman and human primates share a vast array of social characteristics that are not found in other species. Macaques live in large, hierarchical groups, displaying extensive cohabitation and reciprocity (Mitani et al., 2012), and strategically acquire complex social information from facial expressions and the eyes (Emery et al., 1997). Not surprisingly, there are many similarities in the underlying neurobiology of social cognition between humans and macaques. As an example, in both humans (Mitchell et al., 2006; Burke et al., 2010; Apps and Ramnani, 2014; Sul et al., 2015) and macaques (Rudebeck, 2006; Yoshida et al., 2011; Chang et al., 2013b), the brain regions in the medial prefrontal and frontal cortices mediate social learning and prosocial behavior. As another example, the amygdala is particularly engaged when acquiring social information from facial expression as well as eye contact and gaze following in humans (Kawashima et al., 1999; Adolphs et al., 2005) as well as in macaques (Brothers et al., 1990; Gothard et al., 2007; Mosher et al., 2014). Furthermore, the value associated with particular social information is signaled by the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in both humans (Hare et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2010) and in macaques (Watson and Platt, 2012). Overall, although monkeys and humans are undoubtedly different in many ways, studying social cognition in monkeys provide unique opportunities to investigate rudimentary neurophysiological mechanisms underlying human social behavior.

An increasingly acceptable view is that social processes in the brain are enabled by a variety of repurposed mechanisms that initially evolved to solve basic survival and reproductive needs (Chang et al., 2013a; Parkinson and Wheatley, 2015). Social events are closely associated with motivation and emotion. Not surprisingly, motivational and affective circuits in the primate brain are reliably engaged in computing social behavior (Behrens et al., 2009), sharing common fundamental operations (Joiner et al., 2017). Some of these brain regions include the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the OFC, the striatum, and the amygdala, all of which will be discussed later for their involvement in social interactions based on selected experiments in primate social neurophysiology.

Agent-specific reference frames

Compared to more traditional neurophysiological research, social neurophysiology experiments frequently present novel problems in understanding neuronal encoding schemes across self and others. Debatably, the most interesting aspect of social neurophysiology research is in discovering how neurons involved in affective, motivational, sensory and motor processes encode information with respect to oneself and/or a conspecific. There is a parallel to this endeavor in the history of systems neuroscience research. Egocentric and allocentric representations in the brain have been recognized and studied in depth in the visuomotor and the spatial memory systems (Andersen et al., 1997; Burgess, 2006). Encoding variables in an egocentric frame of reference refers to cases when neuronal activity is tightly coupled to (i.e., systematically covaries with) one’s own body (e.g., body part position in space or on some location on the body). In contrast, an allocentric encoding refers to instances when neuronal activity is referenced to environmental location (e.g., world-centered) or objects (i.e., object-centered) that are outside one’s body. In the visuomotor system, a spatially-tuned neuron may encode a stimulus relative to one’s retina in an eye-centered or retinocentric frame of reference (Andersen et al., 1993), which would be classified as a type of egocentric representation. A different spatially-tuned neuron, in either the same or different brain area, may encode the position of the same stimulus relative to its environment but independent of one’s body, which would be classified as an allocentric representation. For example, in a paradigm involving body-under-head and body-plus-head rotations occurring with respect to a saccade target, visuomotor neurons in the monkey area 7a of the posterior parietal cortex modulate their responses according to the position of the head relative to the world (i.e., world-referenced, allocentric signal), but not the position of the head relative to the body (i.e., body-referenced, egocentric signal; Snyder et al., 1998). Similar egocentric and allocentric codes have been found for spatial memory (Feigenbaum and Rolls, 1991; Fyhn et al., 2004). For instance, place fields of the rat medial entorhinal cortical neurons near the postrhinal cortex display view-point-independent spatial information (i.e., allocentric; Fyhn et al., 2004). As in these examples from visuomotor processing and spatial memory, variables involved in social processing may also be encoded using distinct frames of reference (in this case, across agency) and that socially-relevant signals may undergo transformations to be represented in an egocentric or an allocentric manner (Chang, 2013). A need for such transformation processes is likely when social variables encoded in either an egocentric (e.g., relative to oneself) or allocentric (e.g., relative to a social partner) frames of reference need to be integrated to influence specific types of self- or other-regarding decisions.

A pioneering effort to understand how neurons represent self and other began when primate neurophysiologists examined how parietal neurons signal the location of food items differentially as a function of whether the location is shared by a conspecific (Fujii et al., 2007). Specifically, when two adjacent monkeys were not interacting for the food item, arm-motion related parietal neurons primarily signaled one’s own action. In contrast, when the food item was located on a shared, interactive space for the two monkeys, these neurons encoded complex patterns with respect to the actions of both self and other. More recently, a study under the framework of reward-guided decision-making examined the presence of agent-specific reference frames in three different structures in the prefrontal and frontal cortices. Specifically, this study compared the encoding of juice reward outcomes when a subject monkey made a decision to the delivery the reward only to himself, to a recipient monkey, or to neither. In OFC and the sulcus of ACC (ACCs), most neurons signal the reward outcome with respect to self. When the subject monkey makes decisions impacting another monkey, the firing rates of the majority of OFC neurons only encoded the reward outcome for the subject but not when the recipient monkey or neither was rewarded, whereas the firing rates of the majority of ACCs neurons only signaled when the subject monkey’s reward was omitted (i.e., when the reward went to the recipient or neither; Chang et al., 2013b). However, a group of neurons in the gyrus part of the ACC (ACCg) only encoded the reward outcome when the recipient monkey received the juice reward (allocentric reward coding; Chang et al., 2013b). Furthermore, in an experiment where a subject monkey was required to monitor another monkey’s action and outcome, a group of neurons in the medial frontal cortex (MFC) exclusively encoded the action of the other monkey (allocentric action encoding; Yoshida et al., 2011). In the same task, a large proportion of these MFC neurons specifically encoded the erroneous choices made by the social partner (Yoshida et al., 2012), suggesting that MFC harbors dedicated neurons for monitoring actions and errors by others. Similarly, when a monkey subject was taking turns with a human partner to constantly choose a new target located opposite from the target chosen by the human partner on the previous trial, many neurons in the lateral prefrontal cortex selectively encoded the target position of the monkey subject (egocentric encoding) or that of the human partner (allocentric encoding; Falcone et al., 2016). A further example of egocentric and allocentric encoding of social variables comes from an experiment where a monkey subject viewed a reward-predicting stimulus and touched it to realize the outcome concerning self and a conspecific. In this study, many neurons in the striatum preferentially signaled the reward outcome of self (egocentric reward encoding), regardless of whether the resulting action was performed by self or another monkey, whereas a smaller group of neurons encoded just the action of another monkey without encoding the reward outcome (allocentric action encoding; Báez-Mendoza et al., 2013). Furthermore, there are specializations with these striatal neurons for encoding erroneous performance. Specifically, although some striatal neurons signaled one’s own performance errors only, other striatal neurons signaled conspecific’s errors either exclusively or comparably to one’s own error (Báez-Mendoza and Schultz, 2016). Therefore, it is evident that both egocentric as well as allocentric encoding of social variables, which are anchored to the events of another individual, are present in the primate brain and are engaged during social interaction (Fig. 1A, three schematized neurons with different reference frames).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic illustrations of the main neuronal principles discovered from primate social neurophysiology research. A, Illustrations of three different neurons encoding task events in referenced to self (self-referenced), another individual (other-referenced), or both (common-referenced). B, An illustration of a neuron whose activity is gain-modulated by different social contexts within a given reference frame. C, An illustration of a neuron whose activity is specifically gated by particular social information, such as face and eyes. D, An illustration of mirroring in a neuron with comparable firing rate profiles for experienced and observed actions and/or outcomes. Refer to the main texts for the empirical sources for these encoding schemes during social interactions.

Gain modulations by social contexts

In addition to the concept of agent-referenced signals, social information could be added to existing encoding schemes by way of gain changes in the signals, independent of reference frames used. In visuomotor processing, when the information about current eye positions (i.e., the location of the eyes relative to the head or orbital eye position) are added by way of multiplicative gain to a target position signal encoded in an eye-centered frame of reference, the signal now embodies new information about the target location with respect to the head, effectively creating a new representation (Andersen et al., 1985). Several findings in primate social neurophysiology support the idea that the information about a particular social variable may be added by either turning up or down the signal gain. Reward value gain-modulates the activity of predominantly egocentric OFC and ACCs neurons as well as mixed egocentric and allocentric ACCg neurons (Chang et al., 2013b). Furthermore, in an experiment where a subject monkey is required to lift a lever to deliver a reward to itself and to one of two conspecifics, the value tuning of individual OFC neurons are systematically gain-modulated as a function of the social hierarchical position of the conspecific which also receives reward (Azzi et al., 2012). Additionally, a recent study asked monkeys to play a prisoner’s dilemma (PD) task to examine neural signature of strategically cooperative decisions. In this monkey PD task, players could choose one of the two options (“cooperate” or “defect”) when the juice payoffs depend critically on the choices made by both monkeys, thereby encouraging strategic decisions taking into account the prediction of what the other player would do to maximize one’s reward. During this task, ACC neurons scale their activity to differentiate strategically predicted decisions made by a social partner from the current decision made by the subject monkey (Haroush and Williams, 2015). Finally, it has been shown that the differences between own and other’s rewards (inequity) are represented in the striatal neurons using gain modulations, equity (own reward = other’s reward), advantageous inequity (own reward > other’s reward), and disadvantageous inequity (other’s reward > own reward) are signaled via scaled firing rates (Báez-Mendoza et al., 2016). Taken together, in the prefrontal and frontal regions, a gain applied to either egocentric or allocentric signals may be used to combine social variables within existing representations (Fig. 1B, schematized neuron whose activity is gain-modulated by social context).

As alluded to by some examples above, motivational factors can exert a rather dramatic influence on firing characteristics of neurons recruited during social interaction. Motivational factors in social settings may differ as a function of impacting individuals with high versus low social status as well as making a self-generated versus observed outcome influencing other individuals. In the aforementioned experiment where a subject monkey is required to lift a lever to deliver a reward to itself and to one of two conspecifics, the reward value-dependent modulations in OFC neurons positively co-scaled with social context, defined by whether the reward was shared with a specific partner over another (Azzi et al., 2012). Moreover, when viewing the conspecific images with different values, more OFC neurons are modulated by social information conveyed in these images over juice magnitude (Watson and Platt, 2012). These findings suggest that the code for valuation in OFC neurons, during social settings, is strongly coupled to socially motivating factors. Furthermore, neurons in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) show correlated value tuning functions across self and other when an actor monkey makes social decisions about reward allocation across self and other, but this relationship is no longer present when the outcome is passively cued (Chang et al., 2015). These results suggest that the encoding schemes of BLA neurons markedly differ when motivation, or active decision-making, is involved.

Privileged processing for specific social information

Finally, some social variables are highly salient over others to the primate brain, possibly having privileged access to the brain. As expected from the ecology of primate social behavior, the neural underpinnings of primate social behavior are tightly linked to facial expressions and social gaze interactions. Like in humans, faces convey special social information to monkeys. Recently, there has been a great amount of progress in how the primate brain computes facial information from both neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies in macaques. Face patches, first discovered in the temporal cortex, consist almost entirely of face-selective neurons (Tsao et al., 2006). A group of face patches form hierarchical social information processing streams, and are critical for face perception (Moeller et al., 2017; Sadagopan et al., 2017) as well as recognizing individual identity and familiarity (Chang and Tsao, 2017; Landi and Freiwald, 2017). Moreover, the activity patterns of face patch neurons are sensitive to contexts in which social interactions may take place (McMahon et al., 2015), and neurons within the face patches show diverse yet systematic interaction patterns with other brain regions (Park et al., 2017), revealing how face-related information is conveyed across the brain to guide social interactions. Taken together, face patches powerfully demonstrate the presence of privileged processing units for the most fundamental social information in primates that are sensitive to social settings. In addition, like humans, monkeys react to various facial expressions and adjust their future behaviors and follow gaze of a conspecific or make eye contacts to gain social information (Smuts et al., 1987; Ekman, 2015). Neuronal recording studies while monkeys view conspecifics’ facial expressions or the eyes have provided neural mechanistic insights into emotional processing and social gaze behavior. Recordings from the primate amygdala revealed that these neurons either increase or decrease their firing rates to encode either the facial expression or the identity of a conspecific, or some specific combination of both variables (Gothard et al., 2007), reflecting a signature of an integration process across identity and emotional expression. The amygdala neurons also embody a specialized group of neurons that are exclusively responsive to looking at the eyes of another individual or making direct eye contacts with others (Mosher et al., 2014). Furthermore, neurons in the amygdala have been shown to be involved in signaling either the production or monitoring of one’s own facial expression in reaction to viewing the face of a conspecific (Livneh et al., 2012). Signaling of facial expressions by the amygdala neurons is mediated by tactile neurons in the primate amygdala – these neurons show similar firing characteristics between self-produced facial expressions and those induced by tactile stimulation to the face (Mosher et al., 2016). Taken together, these findings suggest that certain, highly salient social information may recruit a specialized group of neurons that are specifically tuned to such ethologically-relevant information (Fig. 1C, schematized neuron with gated responses to particular social information).

Mirroring signals

First discovered in the ventral premotor cortex (PMv or F5) of monkeys (Gallese et al., 1996), “mirroring” refers to when neuronal firing rates display similar characteristic patterns between one’s own movement and observing other’s movement. A series of studies that followed, in both humans and monkeys, have demonstrated the presence of mirroring by several brain regions in connection with social cognition (Gallese et al., 2004; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). Such mirrored representations may serve unique functions in understanding other’s actions from the perspective of oneself. Mirroring, however, is not limited to the motor domain. In the social reward allocation task described earlier, a group of ACCg neurons signaled the reward outcome of self and other in an indistinguishable manner (Chang et al., 2013b) and the majority of BLA neurons showed correlated value tuning functions across self and other (Chang et al., 2015). Furthermore, as mentioned above, some striatal neurons encode performance errors regardless of whether the error was generated by self or other (Báez-Mendoza and Schultz, 2016). Therefore, mirror-type signals have been found across multiple functional domains, including movement, decision-making, and performance monitoring, and seems to be a common neural mechanism for encoding information in an agent-independent manner (Fig. 1D, schematized neuron whose activity mirrors experienced and observed actions/outcomes). How such agent-independent and aforementioned agent-specific neural signals interact to guide social behavior remains unknown.

Concluding remarks

In summary, recent experiments in primate social neurophysiology suggest that social computations are conducted in the primate brain using agent-dependent reference frames, gain-modulated encoding of social and nonsocial variables, as well as the brain regions involved in motivation and affective processing and, in some cases, a specialized group of neurons tuned to highly salient social information. At this time, the field of primate social neurophysiology is quite young and often exploratory in nature given the lack of rich literature to guide testable hypotheses. There remain several core empirical questions for the field to answer within the next decade. For instance, it still remains unclear whether neurons distinguish between social variables and nonsocial variables sharing similar complexity. This is a particularly challenging question to satisfactorily address since the boundary between what we might call “social” and “nonsocial” processes, or “agency,” may not be categorically configured in the neural circuits, but rather lie on a continuum as a function of, for example, processing complexity and predictability. Furthermore, it would be particularly informative to know how and where in the brain distinct representations relating to self and other originate, merge or diverge during the social cognitive processing. The field of primate social neurophysiology may be able to provide new knowledge into how the human brain computes information about self and other, and makes decisions impacting fellow human beings. Advances in this endeavor may one day provide insight on how to improve the lives of individuals suffering from social dysfunction.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements: The author thanks Amrita Nair for thoughtful comments on this manuscript.

Footnotes

  • The author declares no competing financial interests.

  • This work was supported by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation Grant FG-2015-66028 and National Institute of Mental Health Grants R01MH110750, R21MH107853, and R00MH099093.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR (2005) A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature 433:68–72. doi:10.1038/nature03086 pmid:15635411
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Andersen RA, Essick GK, Siegel RM (1985) Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science 230:456–458. pmid:4048942
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Li CS, Stricanne B (1993) Coordinate transformations in the representation of spatial information. Curr Opin Neurobiol 3:171–176. pmid:8513228
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Bradley DC, Xing J (1997) Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Annu Rev Neurosci 20:303–330. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.303 pmid:9056716
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Apps MAJ, Ramnani N (2014) The anterior cingulate gyrus signals the net value of others’ rewards. J Neurosci 34:6190–6200. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2701-13.2014 pmid:24790190
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Azzi JCB, Sirigu A, Duhamel J-R (2012) Modulation of value representation by social context in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:2126–2131. doi:10.1073/pnas.1111715109 pmid:22308343
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Báez-Mendoza R, Schultz W (2016) Performance error-related activity in monkey striatum during social interactions. Sci Rep 6:37199. doi:10.1038/srep37199 pmid:27849004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Báez-Mendoza R, Harris CJ, Schultz W (2013) Activity of striatal neurons reflects social action and own reward. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:16634–16639. doi:10.1073/pnas.1211342110 pmid:24062436
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Báez-Mendoza R, van Coeverden CR, Schultz W (2016) A neuronal reward inequity signal in primate striatum. J Neurophysiol 115:68–79. doi:10.1152/jn.00321.2015 pmid:26378202
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Behrens TEJ, Hunt LT, Rushworth MFS (2009) The computation of social behavior. Science 324:1160–1164. doi:10.1126/science.1169694 pmid:19478175
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Brothers L, Ring B, Kling A (1990) Response of neurons in the macaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behav Brain Res 41:199–213. pmid:2288672
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Burgess N (2006) Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends Cogn Sci 10:551–557. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.005 pmid:17071127
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Burke CJ, Tobler PN, Baddeley M, Schultz W (2010) Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:14431–14436. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003111107 pmid:20660717
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Chang L, Tsao DY (2017) The code for facial identity in the primate brain. Cell 169:1013–1028.e14. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.011 pmid:28575666
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Chang SWC (2013) Coordinate transformation approach to social interactions. Front Neurosci 7:147. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00147
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Chang SWC, Brent LJN, Adams GK, Klein JT, Pearson JM, Watson KK, Platt ML (2013a) Neuroethology of primate social behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110 [Suppl 2 ]:10387–10394. doi:10.1073/pnas.1301213110 pmid:23754410
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    Chang SWC, Gariépy J-F, Platt ML (2013b) Neuronal reference frames for social decisions in primate frontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 16:243–250. doi:10.1038/nn.3287 pmid:23263442
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Chang SWC, Fagan NA, Toda K, Utevsky AV, Pearson JM, Platt ML (2015) Neural mechanisms of social decision-making in the primate amygdala. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:16012–16017. doi:10.1073/pnas.1514761112 pmid:26668400
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Ekman P (2015) Darwin and facial expression: a century of research in review. Cambridge: Malor Books.
  20. ↵
    Emery NJ, Lorincz EN, Perrett DI, Oram MW, Baker CI (1997) Gaze following and joint attention in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J Comp Psychol 111:286–293.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Falcone R, Brunamonti E, Ferraina S, Genovesio A (2016) Neural encoding of self and another agent’s goal in the primate prefrontal cortex: human–monkey interactions. Cereb Cortex 26:4613–4622. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv224 pmid:26464474
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Feigenbaum JD, Rolls ET (1991) Allocentric and egocentric spatial information processing in the hippocampal formation of the behaving primate. Psychobiology 19:21–40.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    Fujii N, Hihara S, Iriki A (2007) Dynamic social adaptation of motion-related neurons in primate parietal cortex. PLoS One 2:e397. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000397 pmid:17460764
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Fyhn M, Molden S, Witter MP, Moser EI, Moser MB (2004) Spatial representation in the entorhinal cortex. Science 305:1258–1264. doi:10.1126/science.1099901 pmid:15333832
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain J Neurol 119:593–609. doi:10.1093/brain/119.2.593
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004) A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 8:396–403. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.002 pmid:15350240
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Gothard KM, Battaglia FP, Erickson CA, Spitler KM, Amaral DG (2007) Neural responses to facial expression and face identity in the monkey amygdala. J Neurophysiol 97:1671–1683. doi:10.1152/jn.00714.2006 pmid:17093126
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Hare TA, Camerer CF, Knoepfle DT, Rangel A (2010) Value computations in ventral medial prefrontal cortex during charitable decision making incorporate input from regions involved in social cognition. J Neurosci 30:583–590. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-09.2010
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    Haroush K, Williams ZM (2015) Neuronal prediction of opponent’s behavior during cooperative social interchange in primates. Cell 160:1233–1245. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2015.01.045 pmid:25728667
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Joiner J, Piva M, Turrin C, Chang SWC (2017) Social learning through prediction error in the brain. NPJ Sci Learn 2:8. doi:10.1038/s41539-017-0009-2
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    Kawashima R, Sugiura M, Kato T, Nakamura A, Hatano K, Ito K, Fukuda H, Kojima S, Nakamura K (1999) The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring. A PET study. Brain 122:779–783. doi:10.1093/brain/122.4.779
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Landi SM, Freiwald WA (2017) Two areas for familiar face recognition in the primate brain. Science 357:591–595. doi:10.1126/science.aan1139 pmid:28798130
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    Livneh U, Resnik J, Shohat Y, Paz R (2012) Self-monitoring of social facial expressions in the primate amygdala and cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:18956–18961. doi:10.1073/pnas.1207662109 pmid:23112157
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    McMahon DBT, Russ BE, Elnaiem HD, Kurnikova AI, Leopold DA (2015) Single-unit activity during natural vision: diversity, consistency, and spatial sensitivity among AF face patch neurons. J Neurosci 35:5537–5548. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3825-14.2015
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit RA, Silk JB (2012) The evolution of primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  36. ↵
    Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR (2006) Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 50:655–663. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040 pmid:16701214
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Moeller S, Crapse T, Chang L, Tsao DY (2017) The effect of face patch microstimulation on perception of faces and objects. Nat Neurosci 20:743–752. doi:10.1038/nn.4527 pmid:28288127
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Mosher CP, Zimmerman PE, Gothard KM (2014) Neurons in the monkey amygdala detect eye contact during naturalistic social interactions. Curr Biol 24:2459–2464. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.063 pmid:25283782
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Mosher CP, Zimmerman PE, Fuglevand AJ, Gothard KM (2016) Tactile stimulation of the face and the production of facial expressions activate neurons in the primate amygdala. eNeuro 3. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0182-16.2016
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    Parkinson C, Wheatley T (2015) The repurposed social brain. Trends Cogn Sci 19:133–141. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.003 pmid:25732617
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Park SH, Russ BE, McMahon DBT, Koyano KW, Berman RA, Leopold DA (2017) Functional subpopulations of neurons in a macaque face patch revealed by single-unit fMRI mapping. Neuron 95:971–981.e5. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.014
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. ↵
    Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) THE MIRROR-NEURON SYSTEM. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:169–192. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230 pmid:15217330
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Rudebeck PH (2006) A role for the macaque anterior cingulate gyrus in social valuation. Science 313:1310–1312. doi:10.1126/science.1128197 pmid:16946075
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    Sadagopan S, Zarco W, Freiwald WA (2017) A causal relationship between face-patch activity and face-detection behavior. Elife 6.
  45. ↵
    Smith DV, Hayden BY, Truong T-K, Song AW, Platt ML, Huettel SA (2010) Distinct value signals in anterior and posterior ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 30:2490–2495. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3319-09.2010 pmid:20164333
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW (1987) Primate societies, Ed 1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  47. ↵
    Snyder LH, Grieve KL, Brotchie P, Andersen RA (1998) Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex. Nature 394:887–891. doi:10.1038/29777 pmid:9732870
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Sul S, Tobler PN, Hein G, Leiberg S, Jung D, Fehr E, Kim H (2015) Spatial gradient in value representation along the medial prefrontal cortex reflects individual differences in prosociality. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:7851–7856. doi:10.1073/pnas.1423895112 pmid:26056280
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    Tsao DY, Freiwald WA, Tootell RB, Livingstone MS (2006) A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science 311(5761):670–674. doi:10.1126/science.1119983 pmid:16456083
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    Watson KK, Platt ML (2012) Social signals in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Curr Biol 22:2268–2273. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.016 pmid:23122847
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Yoshida K, Saito N, Iriki A, Isoda M (2011) Representation of others’ action by neurons in monkey medial frontal cortex. Curr Biol 21:249–253. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.004 pmid:21256015
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Yoshida K, Saito N, Iriki A, Isoda M (2012) Social error monitoring in macaque frontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 15:1307–1312. doi:10.1038/nn.3180 pmid:22864610
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 4 (5)
eNeuro
Vol. 4, Issue 5
September/October 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
An Emerging Field of Primate Social Neurophysiology: Current Developments
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
An Emerging Field of Primate Social Neurophysiology: Current Developments
Steve W. C. Chang
eNeuro 29 September 2017, 4 (5) ENEURO.0295-17.2017; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0295-17.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
An Emerging Field of Primate Social Neurophysiology: Current Developments
Steve W. C. Chang
eNeuro 29 September 2017, 4 (5) ENEURO.0295-17.2017; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0295-17.2017
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Agent-specific reference frames
    • Gain modulations by social contexts
    • Privileged processing for specific social information
    • Mirroring signals
    • Concluding remarks
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • agency
  • Allocentric Representation
  • Egocentric Representation
  • nonhuman primates
  • Social Neurophysiology

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Opinion

  • Novel Insights into the Social Functions of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Infancy
  • Estrogen Receptor Alpha–Expressing Neurons in Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis and Hypothalamus Encoding Aggression and Mating
  • Electrical Stimulation for Stem Cell-Based Neural Repair: Zapping the Field to Action
Show more Opinion

Cognition and Behavior

  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
  • Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Encodes the Trace Period during Appetitive Pavlovian Conditioning
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Opinion
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.