Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleNew Research, Cognition and Behavior

Proactive Control: Neural Oscillatory Correlates of Conflict Anticipation and Response Slowing

Andrew Chang, Jaime S. Ide, Hsin-Hung Li, Chien-Chung Chen and Chiang-Shan R. Li
eNeuro 16 May 2017, 4 (3) ENEURO.0061-17.2017; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0061-17.2017
Andrew Chang
1Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan 10617
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Andrew Chang
Jaime S. Ide
2Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hsin-Hung Li
1Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan 10617
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hsin-Hung Li
Chien-Chung Chen
1Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan 10617
3Center for Neurobiology and Cognitive Science, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan 10617
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Chiang-Shan R. Li
2Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
4Department of Neuroscience, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
5Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520
6 Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, Beijing 100096, China
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Chiang-Shan R. Li
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Proactive control allows us to anticipate environmental changes and adjust behavioral strategy. In the laboratory, investigators have used a number of different behavioral paradigms, including the stop-signal task (SST), to examine the neural processes of proactive control. Previous functional MRI studies of the SST have demonstrated regional responses to conflict anticipation—the likelihood of a stop signal or P(stop) as estimated by a Bayesian model—and reaction time (RT) slowing and how these responses are interrelated. Here, in an electrophysiological study, we investigated the time–frequency domain substrates of proactive control. The results showed that conflict anticipation as indexed by P(stop) was positively correlated with the power in low-theta band (3–5 Hz) in the fixation (trial onset)-locked interval, and go-RT was negatively correlated with the power in delta-theta band (2–8 Hz) in the go-locked interval. Stimulus prediction error was positively correlated with the power in the low-beta band (12–22 Hz) in the stop-locked interval. Further, the power of the P(stop) and go-RT clusters was negatively correlated, providing a mechanism relating conflict anticipation to RT slowing in the SST. Source reconstruction with beamformer localized these time–frequency activities close to brain regions as revealed by functional MRI in earlier work. These are the novel results to show oscillatory electrophysiological substrates in support of trial-by-trial behavioral adjustment for proactive control.

  • Bayesian model
  • Electroencephalogram (EEG)
  • neural oscillation
  • proactive control
  • stop-signal task

Significance Statement

Proactive control is central to adaptive behavior. Many functional MRI studies have dissected the neural basis of conflict processing and behavioral adjustment, but evidence from electrophysiology is fragmentary. Here, by combining EEG and a stop-signal task, we demonstrated distinct frequency domain substrates of conflict anticipation, RT slowing, and stimulus prediction error. In particular, neural activities of conflict anticipation preceded RT slowing, and the power of these activities were correlated, in support of proactive control of behavior. Further, beamformer analysis localized the sources of these activities as revealed by functional MRI. These new findings complement the literature by specifying the electrophysiological correlates of trial-by-trial response control within a single paradigm.

Introduction

The ability to make plans and proactively adjust our behavior is critical to survival. Proactive control has been studied with many different behavioral paradigms in which participants are cued to upcoming events (Vink et al. 2005; Brass and Haggard, 2007, Chikazoe et al. 2009; Kühn et al. 2009; Jahfari et al. 2010; Horga et al. 2011; Leunissen et al. 2016). In the stop-signal task (SST), for instance, frequent motor responses are to be interrupted occasionally by a stop signal, and increased stop signal probability bolsters proactive control, as shown in slower response time and delayed motor cortical activity (Jahfari et al. 2010). Participants typically demonstrate slower reaction time (RT) after a stop signal, or postsignal slowing (PSS), in the SST. As investigated extensively in behavioral studies, PSS does not reflect the surprise effects of stop signal but rather a global, contextual modulation of signal expectancy on motor response (Morein-Zamir et al. 2007; Bissett and Logan, 2011). In diffusion models of SST, response thresholds increased when stop signals were expected to occur, and participants made response-strategy adjustments on a trial-by-trial basis, suggesting proactive adjustment to changing environments (Verbruggen and Logan, 2009b). On the other hand, the neural mechanisms underlying trial-by-trial behavioral adjustment have been examined only recently under a formal computational framework.

In a previous functional MRI (fMRI) study of the SST, authors applied a dynamic Bayesian model to estimate trial-by-trial probability of the stop signal—P(stop)—and showed that higher P(stop) is associated with prolonged go trial RT, indicating proactive control of motor response (Hu et al. 2015a). The gist of the study was to identify and distinguish regional brain activations specific to signal anticipation and response slowing as a result of the anticipation. In modeling fMRI signals at trial and target onsets, they showed that the anterior presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA) responds to increased P(stop) in correlation with activations of the posterior pre-SMA and bilateral anterior insula during prolonged RT. These findings associate conflict anticipation with its motor consequence.

Many studies identified time-domain event-related potentials (ERPs) in association with proactive control, including processes related to switch preparation or anticipatory attention (Karayanidis and Jamadar, 2014; Angelini et al. 2016; Di Russo et al. 2016; Langford et al. 2016). Neural oscillations represent frequency-domain electrophysiological excitabilities of neuronal populations, with oscillations in different frequencies reflecting dynamic information exchange between distinct functional networks (Fries, 2005). Electroencephalogram (EEG) studies have shown that neural oscillations measured above the medial-frontal cortex are associated with cognitive control (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Cohen, 2014a). In Simon tasks, the power in theta band (∼3–8 Hz) increased and positively predicted RT in high conflict trials (Pastötter et al. 2010, 2013; Cohen and Cavanagh, 2011; Nigbur et al. 2011; Cohen and Donner, 2013; Cohen and Ridderinkhof, 2013; Clayton et al. 2015). Proactive and reactive controls were each associated with theta oscillations originating from distinct cortical networks during task switching (Cooper et al. 2015). In contrast, delta band (∼1–3 Hz) power was associated with shorter RT during error trials in the Simon task (Cohen and van Gaal, 2014). Proactive versus reactive control modulated the power of delta oscillations differently in an SST (Lavallee et al. 2014). Alpha (∼8–13 Hz) power, localized in the superior frontal cortex, was associated with attentional control to inhibit irrelevant sensory inputs in a flanker task (Suzuki and Shinoda, 2015). In a magnetoencephalography study of eye movement control, alpha to low beta bands (∼10–18 Hz) increased in power in the frontal eye field during suppression of a prepotent saccade compared with generation of an automatic saccade, an effect that positively predicted performance accuracy (Hwang et al. 2016). Together, these studies characterized oscillatory correlates of various forms of proactive control, but the frequency domain processes of conflict anticipation and response control remain to be dissociated and related to behavioral performance.

To fill this gap of research, we used a dynamic Bayesian model (Yu and Cohen, 2009) to quantify the extent of conflict anticipation—belief of an upcoming stop signal—trial by trial, on the basis of trial history. To investigate the neural oscillatory basis of proactive control, we identified the EEG time–frequency correlates of conflict anticipation and RT slowing in trial-by-trial analyses and used beamforming methods to localize the sources of these correlates. Specifically, we hypothesized that the time–frequency correlates of conflict anticipation should precede in time and correlate in power with those of RT slowing.

Methods

Participants

Eighteen healthy adults (9 males, 22.6 ± 1.3 years of age), who were all students and naive to the purposes of the experiment, participated in the study. All provided written consent and were financially compensated for their participation, in accordance with the guideline of Helsinki Declaration and a protocol approved by the Research Ethics Committee of National Taiwan University.

Behavioral task

We used a simple RT task of the stop signal paradigm (Chang et al., 2014; Chang et al., 2015; Li et al., 2008; Verbruggen and Logan, 2009a). There were two trial types, “go” and “stop,” randomly intermixed in presentation with a ratio of ∼3:1. The intertrial interval was 2 s. A small white dot (fixation) appeared at the center of a black screen to engage attention at the beginning of every trial. After an interval ranging randomly from 1 to 3 s (the foreperiod), the dot turned into a circle (∼2° of visual angle), which served as a go signal. The participants were instructed to quickly press a button at go-signal onset but not before. The circle vanished either at button press or 1 s after go-signal onset, whichever came first, and the trial terminated. A premature button press before go-signal onset also terminated the trial. In the stop trial, an additional X, the stop signal, replaced the go signal. The participants were instructed to withhold button press on seeing the stop signal. The trial terminated at the button press or 1 s after stop-signal onset. The duration between go- and stop-signal onsets, or the stop-signal delay (SSD), was determined by a staircase procedure. The one-up-one-down procedure (Levitt, 1971) started with an SSD of 200 ms, and increased and decreased by 64 ms each after a successful and failed stop trial. By increasing and decreasing each SSD after a stop success and error, the staircase procedure allows participants to succeed in approximately half of the stop trials.

The task was divided into four sessions, each with 100 trials and lasting no longer than 8 min, with a short break in between sessions. There were ∼5 min of practice on the task before the experiments to ensure that participants understood the task. Participants were instructed to “respond to the go signal quickly while watching out for the stop signal, which might appear occasionally.”

Bayesian modeling of the sequential effect

We used a dynamic Bayesian model (Yu and Cohen, 2009) to estimate the belief of an upcoming stop signal on each trial, P(stop), based on preceding stimulus history. In the model, previous trial information is encoded in the prior distribution. In every trial, the posterior distribution is carried over to the next trial as a prior. Although the stop and go trials were randomized in presentation, participants learned from local trial structure and updated their belief of P(stop) and adjusted RT on a trial-by-trial basis. P(stop) reflects participants’ estimation of the trial-by-trial likelihood of the stop signal on the basis of the Bayesian model, and it is computed independent of the RT. A sequential effect is defined as the linear correlation between P(stop) and RT for all go trials (Ide et al. 2015).

In the model, participants have the knowledge that each trial has a probability rk for being a stop trial and 1−rk for being a go trial. Furthermore, the probability rk has a chance α to be the same as rk–1 and 1 – α being resampled from a prior distribution π(rk).

With these generative assumptions, participants use Bayesian inference to update their prior belief of seeing a stop signal on trial k, Embedded Image based on the posterior of the last trial Embedded Image given the last trial’s true category (sk = 1 for stop trial, sk = 0 for go trial), where sk = {s1, …, sk} is shorthand for all trials 1 through k. Specifically, given that the posterior distribution was Embedded Image on trial k − 1, the prior distribution of stop signal in trial k is given byEmbedded Image where the prior distribution π(rk) is a beta distribution with prior mean pm and shape parameter sc, which were reparameterized from the beta(a,b) distribution such that pm = a/(a + b) and sc = (a + b), and the posterior distribution is computed from the prior distribution and the outcome according to Bayes’ rule:Embedded Image

The Bayesian estimate of the probability of trial k being a stop trial—which we colloquially call P(stop)—given the predictive distribution Embedded Image is expressed byEmbedded Image

We approximated the mean of the predictive distribution Embedded Image by the maximum a posteriori (MAP) estimate Embedded Image . The proposition that the predictive distribution is a mixture of the previous posterior distributions and a generic prior distribution is essentially equivalent to using a causal, exponential, linear filter to estimate the current rate of stop trials (Yu and Cohen, 2009). In summary, for each participant, given a sequence of observed go/stop trials and the three model parameters {α, pm, sc}, we estimated P(stop) for each trial. Generally speaking, α quantifies the weight given by the participant to the previous trials (the magnitude of influence from previous to current trial), pm is the mean of the fixed belief of stop signal, and sc reflects how skewed the distribution is around the mean. It is worth noting that the Markov modeling approach (transition between the static and dynamic states) is assumed to be of the first order.

To obtain the best fit parameters for sequential effect in each individual, we grid-searched for the parameters that maximized the Pearson correlation between go-RT and P(stop) using Matlab. The search space of model parameters were set to the following ranges: α = [0.02, 0.04, …, 0.98], pm = [0.01, 0.03, …, 0.49], and fixed sc = 10. For the stop trials, we also quantified the prediction error (PE), or Bayesian surprise, as |1 – P(stop)| (Ide et al. 2013).

Electroencephalography acquisition and data preprocessing

The EEG was collected with a whole-head, 256-channel geodesic EEG system with HydroCell Sensor Nets (Electrical Geodesics). This system provides uniform spatial sampling (∼2 cm, sensor to sensor), covering the entire scalp surface and extending 120° in all directions from the vertex reference electrode. The EEG was amplified at a gain of 1000 and recorded with a vertex physical reference. Signals were digitized at 500 Hz with a 16-bit analog-to-digital converter, which allowed an amplitude resolution of 0.076 μV. The computer administering the task sent a digital trigger to the recording system at the onset of fixation and go signal of every trial.

The data were preprocessed by the following procedures offline before statistical analysis with the Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA) software package. First, the raw data were filtered by a 0.7- to 100-Hz bandpass (FIR) filter. Second, independent component analysis (ICA) decomposition using an extended infomax algorithm (Lee et al. 1999) was performed on the continuous EEG recording of each session for each participant, and the channel-time EEG data were projected into the component-time EEG data in the ICA space, with PCA dimension reduction excluding the independent components with <0.75% of variance. ICA is a useful technique for artifact removal on EEG recordings by spatially separating independent artifact sources mixed with the brain activities of interest in the surface EEG channels. Third, the components reflecting artifact (identified by visual inspection), including eye blinking, eye movement, electrocardiogram, and 60-Hz powerline noise, were excluded. Fourth, the data of the remaining components were projected back as ICA artifact-removed 256-channel EEG data. Fifth, to identify any remaining artifacts that were not removed by ICA, the ICA artifact-removed data were epoched from –500 to 1000 ms, each time-locked to the onset time of fixation signal or go signal. In the final step, the epochs with voltage change exceeding 150 μV at any channel were identified as containing artifact and excluded for further analyses. As a result of the preprocessing, 85.9 ± 15.0% of the trials were included for further analyses.

Time–frequency decomposition

We examined EEG activities across time and frequencies, which may reflect the activities of different neuronal populations in association with distinct cognitive functions. The Morlet wavelet transformation was computed for each time point continuous on the ICA artifact-removed 256-channel unfiltered EEG data with 59 logarithmically spaced frequency bins between 2 and 50 Hz. The wavelet was designed such that the half-maximum width was equal to 1 period of the lowest frequency with the width equal to 4 periods of the highest frequency, linearly interpolated for each frequency bin in between. Thus, EEG data were transformed into power at each time–frequency bin. Subsequently, we segmented the time–frequency data from –500 to 1000 ms into epochs, time-locked to the onset of fixation signal, go signal, or stop signal, and the power of each epoch was baseline corrected to the mean power of the –300- to –100-ms period for each frequency bin. We performed the time–frequency decomposition before epoching the data, circumventing the issue of edge artifact as can occur in time–frequency decomposition on short EEG epochs. There was a buffer zone of at least 4 s at the beginning and the end of each continuous EEG recording.

Correlating time–frequency power with sequential effect

We performed a Spearman rank correlation on the power for each time–frequency bin with P(stop), go-RT, or PE on the fixation (trial onset)-locked, go-locked, and stop-locked interval across trials for each participant. To examine within-subject correlations on P(stop) and EEG across all participants, we used the group-averaged parameters {α, pm, sc} of individual Bayesian model that maximized the sequential effect for each participant, according to previous model-based studies (Camerer and Ho, 1999; O’Doherty et al. 2004; Ide et al. 2015). Further, we focused on the surface channel FCz (standard channel montage from BESA), located at midfrontal areas, because previous EEG studies of conflict processing reported activities mainly from the medial frontal cortex (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Cohen, 2014a) and fMRI studies described activations to conflict anticipation, RT slowing, and prediction error in distinct areas of the medial prefrontal cortex (Hu et al. 2015a).

To control for type I error of multiple comparisons, we performed a nonparametric cluster-based permutation test (Cohen, 2014b; Maris and Oostenveld, 2007) on the 2D time–frequency maps in two loops. In the first loop, within each participant, we randomly permuted trial mapping between EEG data and behavioral responses for 1000 times and computed EEG–behavior correlation coefficient for each permutation as a null distribution for each time–frequency bin. We computed the z-value of the observed correlation coefficient relative to the null distribution for each time–frequency bin—a z-map—to represent the EEG–behavior correlation for each participant (Cohen, 2014b).

In the second loop, we tested the random effect hypothesis of specific time–frequency bins showing consistent EEG–behavior correlation across participants. First, we used a two-tailed one-sample t test to examine whether any time–frequency bins on the z-map were significantly different from 0 across participants. Second, we grouped adjacent time–frequency bins reaching a threshold of p < 0.02, 0.005, or 0.001 into single clusters and summed the t-value within each cluster into a cluster-level statistic. Third, we built a permutation null distribution with 2000 iterations from the observed z-maps, representing zero correlation across participants. Multiple comparisons were controlled by extracting the summed t-value of the largest suprathreshold cluster of each iteration into null distribution, and the final p-value (pperm) was computed by ranking the clustered t-value in the real data relative to the null distribution (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007), where the 2000 iterations of permutation divided the pperm with a step size of 1/2000, ranging from 0 to 1. The observed statistical power was estimated by repeating the permutation test 200 times and then taking the percentage of obtaining pperm < 0.05 out of the 200 tests.

Beamformer source reconstruction

We used beamformer to estimate the source location of the time–frequency activities of interest. As a signal-processing technique to discriminate between signals arriving from a location of interest and signals arriving from other locations, beamformer is used to spatially filter scalp-recorded EEG data to estimate the source power for a specific location in the brain. Specifically, by considering signals generated from one specific voxel while attenuating signals from other voxels, the distribution of source power of the 3D brain is estimated by repeatedly constructing beamformer at each voxel (Green and McDonald, 2009). We used the multiple source beamformer algorithms in BESA (Gross et al. 2001) to estimate the source of the time–frequency cluster at the single-trial level for non–phase-locked time–frequency activities. For each voxel, the source power in the time–frequency cluster of interest P is normalized by the source power in a reference time–frequency cluster Pref as q:Embedded Image For a specific voxel, the magnitude of positive q represents the magnitude of P ≥ Pref, whereas the magnitude of negative q represents the magnitude of P < Pref.

We estimated a q-map for each individual cluster for each participant. To reconstruct the sources of each time–frequency cluster associated with the sequential effect, we median-split the trials based on P(stop) or go-RT for each participant. We did not reconstruct the source of PE owing to an insufficient number of stop trials available for analysis (n ≤ 100). We assigned beamformer power source of the time–frequency cluster, each in the higher and lower half of the behavioral index, as P and Pref, respectively. That is, the voxels with largest positive and largest negative q values are likely the source of the time–frequency cluster correlates each of P(stop) and go-RT, respectively.

We used the four-shell ellipsoidal model implemented in BESA toolbox to solve the inverse problem. Although our head model was not MRI-based and represented a simplistic approximation to the realistic brain, empirical simulations showed that the four-shell head model localizes the source with ∼1 cm of averaged errors (Cohen et al. 1990; Cuffin et al. 1991), which is reasonably accurate (Slotnick, 2004). The locations of all 256 channels were provided by BESA for the 256-channel HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net.

To perform source localization at the group level, we coregistered and normalized individual q-maps with affine transformation to reduce misalignment among the images, and performed one-sample t tests. These analyses were performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, UK).

Results

Behavioral performance and sequential effect

The go success rate was 98.4 ± 1.9% (mean ± SD), and go-RT was 405 ± 64 ms across participants. The stop success rate was 48.7 ± 2.2%, suggesting the success of the staircase procedure in tracking participants’ performance and eliciting errors in approximately half of the stop trials. The RT of stop-error trials was 360 ± 52 ms, which was significantly shorter than go-RT (t(17) = 10.81, p < 0.001a). The critical SSD was computed by a maximal likelihood procedure on the sequence of all staircase-generated SSDs for each participant, and the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) was computed by subtracting the critical SSD from the median go-RT for each participant, based on the race model. The critical SSD was 170 ± 71 ms, and SSRT was 216 ± 25 ms. These behavioral outcome were within the range reported in earlier studies (e.g., Logan et al. 1997; Li et al. 2008; Verbruggen and Logan, 2008; Ide et al. 2013; Hu et al. 2015a; Schevernels et al. 2015; Langford et al. 2016).

We fitted a dynamic Bayesian model for the trial sequence of each participant (Fig. 1A), and the best-fitted parameter values, with sc fixed at 10, were α = 0.78 ± 0.14 (mean ± SD) and pm = 0.14 ± 0.16 across participants. The Pearson correlation coefficient between P(stop) and go-RT was 0.29 ± 0.08 (range, 0.14–0.43; Fig. 1B), with p < 0.001 for 16 participants and p = 0.005 and 0.016 for the other 2 participants. At the group level, we first estimated the P(stop) for each participant, took the average of go-RT and stop error (SE) rate of the trials at each P(stop) bin (width = 0.01) within each participant, and then averaged the mean go-RT or mean SE rate across participant on each bin. We truncated the extreme P(stop) bins in which very few participants (n ≤ 2) were represented, and the resulting P(stop) bins were each in the range of [0.08, 0.37] and [0.09, 0.27] for correlation with grand-averaged go-RT and SE rate. Pearson correlation showed a positive correlation between P(stop) and the grand-averaged go-RT (r(29) = 0.89, 95% CI = [0.77, 0.94], p < 10−10b; Fig. 1C) and a negative correlation between P(stop) and grand-averaged SE rate (r(18) = –0.93, 95% CI = [–0.82, –0.97], p < 10−8c; Fig. 1D). Together, the results showed that the dynamic Bayesian model successfully captured the sequential effect for each participant as well as for the group.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Bayesian model prediction of behavioral performance in the stop-signal task. A, Example sequence of trials. The upper panel shows the sequence of go (blue dots) or stop (red dots) trials and how Bayesian belief about encountering a stop trial [P(stop), black line] increases and decreases, respectively, after each stop and go trial. The lower panel shows the sequence of go-RT in the upper panel. Overall, go-RT tended to be prolonged with a higher P(stop). B, Correlation between P(stop) and RT across all go success trials, with each regression line representing an individual participant. C, Positive correlation between go-RT and P(stop) collapsed over all participants. The plot in the upper panel shows the mean ± SE, the histogram in the lower panel shows the distribution of P(stop), and both were binned at intervals of 0.01. D, Negative correlation between stop error (SE) rate and P(stop), with the same format as in C.

Neural time–frequency power correlates

To examine for which frequency bands and at what time interval the time–frequency activities were related to sequential effects, we correlated the time–frequency power at FCz channel in midfrontal region with P(stop) and go-RT each in fixation-locked and go-locked epochs across trials within each participant. As described in detail earlier, we evaluated the statistical significance of the correlations across participants in random effects analysis on the basis of a cluster-based nonparametric permutation test.

In the fixation-locked epoch, permutation test on the 0-500 ms range showed that P(stop) was positively correlated with the power in the cluster at 3-5 Hz (low-theta band) and 0-200 ms interval (pperm = 0.034d, cluster threshold at p < 0.02), with the strongest correlation at ∼4–5 Hz and 100–150 ms (pperm = 0.036e, cluster threshold at p < 0.005; Fig. 2A). In contrast, the go-RT was not correlated with power in this time–frequency range (Fig. 2B).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Trial-by-trial oscillatory power correlates of P(stop), go-RT, and PE at channel FCz. A, Correlation between fixation-locked (onset at 0 ms) power and P(stop) at each time–frequency bin. The color represents the z-value of Spearman correlation, and the black contours represent statistically significant time–frequency clusters in the nonparametric cluster-based permutation test across participants, with clustering threshold at p < 0.02, 0.005, and 0.001 levels (see Materials and Methods for details). It showed a positive correlation in the intervals 3–5 Hz and 0–200 ms. B, No clusters showed a significant correlation between go-locked power and P(stop). C, No clusters showed a significant correlation between fixation-locked power and go-RT. D, Correlation between go-locked power and go-RT, with the permutation test showing a negative correlation in the intervals 2–8 Hz and 200–700 ms. E, The coefficient of trial-by-trial Pearson correlation between the mean power of the clusters identified in A and D of individual participants. Wilcoxon signed rank test showed that the correlation coefficients across participants were significantly below zero. F, G, Correlation between stop-locked power and PE, with the permutation test showing a positive correlation in the intervals 12–22 Hz and 300–400 ms. The topographies of each correlational cluster (p < 0.005) are shown in the bottom panel, where H, I, J, and K each show the cluster of A, D, F, and G, all with the strongest correlations at the midfrontal region. We performed the same analyses at Pz channel (Fig. 2-1).

Figure 2-1

Trial-by-trial oscillatory power correlates of P(stop), go-RT, and PE at channel Pz. The format is the same as with Fig. 2. The color represents the z-value of Spearman correlation, and the black contours represent statistically significant time–frequency clusters in the nonparametric cluster-based permutation test across participants, with clustering threshold at p < 0.02, 0.005, and 0.001 levels (see Materials and Methods for details). A, B, Time–frequency power was not correlated with P(stop) in fixation-locked or go-locked epoch. C, Time–frequency power was not correlated with go-RT in fixation-locked epoch. D, Time–frequency power was negatively correlated with go-RT in the intervals 2–8 Hz and ∼100–500 ms in go-locked epoch. E, F, Time–frequency power was not correlated with PE in stop-locked epoch.. Download Figure 2-1, TIF file.

In the go-locked epoch, permutation test showed that P(stop) was not correlated with the power over the 0-800 ms range (Fig. 2C). In contrast, go-RT was negatively correlated with power at 2–8 Hz (across delta and theta bands) in the 200-700 ms interval (pperm = 0.005f, cluster threshold at p < 0.02), with the strongest correlation at 2.5–4.5 Hz and 300–600 ms (pperm = 0.002g, cluster threshold at p < 0.001; Fig. 2D).

Given that P(stop) and go-RT were correlated as sequential effects (Fig. 1B, C), we further tested whether the power of the time–frequency clusters each associated with P(stop) and go-RT was correlated across trials within each participant. For each participant, we correlated the mean power of P(Stop) and go-RT clusters (with cluster threshold at p < 0.005 and 0.001, respectively) in a Pearson regression. Because the distribution of the correlation coefficients violated normality assumption (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: p < 0.001), we used a two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test to examine the group result. The test showed that the correlation coefficients were significantly below 0 (i.e., negative correlation) across all participants (exact p = 0.024h, 95% CI = [–0.047, –0.002]; Fig. 2E).

We also correlated the power at FCz channel with PE in stop-locked epoch across trials. In the stop-locked epoch, the nonparametric permutation test in the 0-500 ms range showed that PE was positively correlated with the power in the cluster at 12–22 Hz (low-beta band) and 300–400 ms (pperm = 0.021i, cluster threshold at p < 0.02), with the strongest correlation at ∼13–18 Hz and 320–380 ms (pperm = 0.004j, cluster threshold at p < 0.001; Fig. 2F). We further considered the possibility that this correlation was confounded with stop trial performance, given that P(stop) was associated with SE rate (Fig. 1D). Thus, we regressed out the mean power of stop-success and stop-error trial and performed the correlation of the residual power with PE across all stop trials. The result remained that the PE was positively correlated with the power in the cluster at 12–22 Hz and 300–400 ms (pperm = 0.020k, cluster threshold at p < 0.001), with the strongest correlation at ∼13–19 Hz and 310–400 ms (pperm = 0.001l; Fig. 2G).

We performed additional analyses on the time–frequency power at Pz channel in parietal-midline region (Fig. 2-1), following the same procedures on FCz. The P(stop) was not correlated with time–frequency power in either fixation-locked or go-locked epoch (Fig. 2-1A, B). The go-RT was not correlated with time–frequency power in fixation-locked epoch (Fig. 2-1C). However, the go-RT was negatively correlated with the power in the cluster at 2–8 Hz (across delta and theta bands) and ∼100–500 ms (pperm < 0.001m,n,o, cluster threshold at p < 0.02, 0.005, or 0.001; Fig. 2-1D). This finding was consistent with the finding at FCz (Fig. 2D). The PE was not correlated with time–frequency power in stop-locked epoch (Fig. 2-1E, F), whether the mean power of stop-success and stop-error trial was regressed out or not.

In sum, conflict anticipation as indexed by P(stop) was positively correlated with the power in low-theta band (3–5 Hz) over a fixation-locked interval, go-RT was negatively correlated with the power in delta-theta band (2–8 Hz) over a go-locked interval, and PE was positively correlated with the power in the low-beta band (12–22 Hz) over a stop-locked interval. These correlations were located on the midfrontal area of the scalp (Fig. 2H–K). Together, the results showed that P(stop), go-RT, and PE can be distinguished by time–frequency representations within a trial. Moreover, the time–frequency cluster of P(stop) preceded that of go-RT in time, and the powers of P(stop) and go-RT clusters were negatively correlated, suggesting a potential causal link between the neural activities of P(stop) and go-RT.

Beamforming source reconstruction

Regions reconstructed and localized for the EEG correlates of conflict anticipation and RT slowing are shown in Fig. 3A and B and summarized in Table 1. At p < 0.001 (uncorrected) and cluster size >2000 mm3, the time–frequency cluster for P(stop) was localized to the right supramarginal gyrus (SMG) and anterior pre-SMA. These two clusters are significant at p < 0.05, corrected for familywise error of multiple comparisons, with small volume correction for the regions of interest as shown in fMRI (Hu et al. 2015a). At the same voxel threshold, the time–frequency cluster for go-RT could not be localized to a significant source. We thus used a more liberal voxel threshold to examine the results. At p < 0.005 (uncorrected) and cluster size >20,000 mm3, the time–frequency cluster for go-RT was localized to the right precentral and postcentral gyri and middle/inferior frontal gyri. We were unable to reconstruct the source of the power in the low-beta band (12–22 Hz) in association with PE, because there were much fewer (<100) stop trials (in comparison to 350 ± 46 and 257 ± 58 trials that survived artifact rejection and were available for source construction each for conflict anticipation and RT slowing). ⇓

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Source reconstruction and localization. EEG correlates of conflict anticipation (p < 0.001, uncorrected) were localized to the right SMG and the anterior pre-SMA. B, EEG correlates of RT slowing (p < 0.005, uncorrected) were localized to the middle and inferior frontal gyrus (MFG/IFG) and precentral and postcentral gyrus (PC).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Source reconstruction for the EEG correlates of conflict anticipation (p < 0.001, uncorrected; cluster size >2000 mm3) and RT slowing (p < 0.005, uncorrected; cluster size >20,000 mm3)

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Statistical table

Discussion

We identified time–frequency neural activities associating trial-by-trial conflict anticipation to RT slowing in a stop-signal task. First, we showed that conflict anticipation, quantified by the trial-by-trial likelihood of a stop signal from a dynamic Bayesian model (Yu and Cohen, 2009), was positively correlated with RT slowing across trials, in support of proactive behavioral control. The Bayesian model provides a unique framework to estimate individuals’ expectations from observations and describe how individuals adjust behavior according to these expectations. In EEG, we showed that (1) conflict anticipation was positively correlated with the power at low theta band (3–5 Hz) during the foreperiod but not after go-signal onset; (2) go-trial RT was negatively correlated with the power at delta-theta band (2–8 Hz) after go-signal onset but not during the foreperiod; (3) the power of these two time–frequency clusters was negatively correlated across trials, mirroring the behavioral finding of sequential effect; and (4) stimulus prediction error was positively correlated with the power in the low-beta band (12–22 Hz), after the onset of stop signal. Further, beamforming source reconstruction localized the EEG correlates of conflict anticipation to the supramarginal gyrus and anterior pre-SMA, consistent with previous fMRI studies (Hu et al. 2015a). It is worth mentioning that despite the strong correlation between P(stop) and RT, we were able to identify distinct neural correlates of these behavioral measures by modeling P(stop)- and RT-related activities each at trial and go-signal onsets. This is made possible by the temporal resolution of EEG signals even when the trial and go-signal onsets occurred within 1–3 s. Thus, these findings distinguished the time–frequency electrophysiological correlates of conflict anticipation, response control, and prediction error and characterized the trial-by-trial neural oscillatory dynamics of proactive control of motor response.

The current results showed that theta power reflects the magnitude of both conflict anticipation and RT slowing, and these two activities were correlated in the same trial, broadly in accord with a role of theta oscillations in cognitive control (Cavanagh et al. 2009; Cohen and Ridderinkhof, 2013; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Cohen, 2014a, 2016; Chmielewski et al. 2016; van Noordt et al. 2016a, b; Wang et al. 2016). Previous EEG studies of Simon tasks showed that theta power was more positively correlated with RT on high-conflict than on low-conflict trials (Cohen and Cavanagh, 2011; Cohen and Donner, 2013); however, it remains unclear whether theta power primarily reflects conflict processing or behavioral response as a result of the conflict or both. Here, we dissociated the effects of conflict anticipation and response control and showed that theta power correlated with these two processes in opposite directions, and it is possible that the previous findings were mainly driven by conflict anticipation. This new finding suggests that different cortical networks process conflict anticipation and response control, in communication via theta oscillation. These results are also consistent with earlier fMRI findings that distinct prefrontal and frontal cortical structures support conflict anticipation and RT slowing, and activities during conflict anticipation Granger caused activities during RT slowing (Hu et al. 2015a, b; Manza et al. 2016).

A substantial body of work supports a role of theta oscillation in proactive control. In the current findings, the onset time of theta power responses of both conflict anticipation and RT slowing started as early as ∼0–200 ms after stimulus onset, favoring the explanation that these responses reflect preparation for the upcoming event rather than exogenously elicited reactions. An EEG study of conflict tasks showed that RT was better predicted by ongoing endogenous (non–phase-locked) than exogenous (phase-locked) theta power, supporting a top-down, proactive rather than bottom-up, reactive process (Cohen and Donner, 2013). In an anti-saccade task, medial frontal theta power was increased during response preparation on correct but not error trials (van Noordt et al. 2016b). With transcranial alternating current stimulation in the theta frequency applied to midfrontal scalp region, participants slowed down in response during low-conflict trials and as a result exhibited less conflict effect in a Simon task (van Driel et al. 2015). In another EEG study of proactive control, theta oscillation reflected information gathering for proactive control across oddball, go/no-go, and task-switching paradigms (Cooper et al. 2016). More broadly, studies have associated theta oscillations to various top-down cognitive processes to ready attention for task switching (Min and Park, 2010; Daitch et al. 2013; Phillips et al. 2014), maintain working memory (Scheeringa et al. 2009), and encode and retrieve episodic memories (Nyhus and Curran, 2010).

The current findings showed that delta oscillations, in addition to theta oscillations, are correlated with RT slowing. Earlier studies showed delta oscillation in the motor process of cognitive control, in accord with the current findings. For example, delta and theta power followed the motor response during error-related reactive control and in negative association with RT in trial-by-trial correlation (Cohen and van Gaal, 2014). Another intracranial EEG study showed that the phase of delta-theta (2–5 Hz) oscillation modulated high-gamma power (>70 Hz), and a stronger coupling predicted shorter RT in spatial target detection (Szczepanski et al. 2014; Voytek et al. 2015). A recent study of oddball, go/no-go, and switch tasks demonstrated sensitivity of frontal delta and theta power to sensorimotor control (Cooper et al. 2016). In a modified stop-signal paradigm, which manipulated proactive/reactive control (with informative/neutral preparatory cue) in conjunction with selectivity of stopping behavior (unimanual vs. bimanual response), both factors interactively modulated delta power on stop trials (Lavallee et al. 2014). However, it is unclear whether the change of delta power reflects the difference of bottom-up response to stopping cue or top-down conflict anticipation. The current trial-by-trial analyses dissociated these two factors and showed that the delta power was associated with RT but not with conflict anticipation, favoring the explanation that delta power reflects response control rather than signal anticipation.

It has been shown that the theta and delta oscillations were the major frequency-domain features of time-domain N2 and P3 ERP components, respectively (Cavanagh et al. 2012; Huster et al. 2013; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Harper et al. 2014; Cavanagh and Shackman, 2015). Evidence is available from response inhibition tasks (e.g. SST, go/no-go) that theta/N2 and delta/P3 each reflects conflict monitoring and motor inhibition (Huster et al. 2013). In particular, the amplitude of anterior N2, generated in medial-frontal cortex, was modulated by the predictability of stimulus (Heinze et al. 1990; Woods et al. 1992), consistent with our finding that theta power reflects conflict anticipation. P3 is associated with response inhibition (Kok et al. 2004; Kenemans, 2015), and the peak latency of P3 is positively correlated with RT (Conroy and Polich, 2007; Polich, 2007). Here, we showed that stronger delta power was associated with shorter RT, and it is possible that the change in delta power is reflected as shift in P3 peak latency in the time domain. However, it is important to note that, as a temporally averaged phase-locked signal, ERP waveform does not distinguish oscillation in various frequency bands, and the non–phase-locked neural oscillations on each trial may cancel out in averaged ERP waveform. The trial-by-trial analysis employed in the current study did not lend itself to revealing potential phase resetting responses, and future study is needed to directly examine this issue.

Beamforming analyses localized the time–frequency correlates of conflict anticipation to the pre-SMA and right SMG and those of RT slowing to right prefrontal and somatomotor cortex, in correspondence to earlier fMRI findings (Hu et al. 2015a). The preSMA is implicated in numerous studies for volitional control of behavior (Rushworth et al. 2004; Kennard et al. 2005; Jaffard et al. 2008; McDowell et al. 2008; Wolpe et al. 2013) and other functions, such as performance monitoring, required for cognitive control, as demonstrated extensively in unit recordings from behaving monkeys (Stuphorn et al. 2000; Ito et al. 2003; Stuphorn and Schall, 2006). Both right-hemispheric prefrontal structures (Li et al. 2008; Cai et al. 2014) and ipsilateral somatomotor cortex (Meyer et al. 1998) are known to be involved in the control of movement initiation. On the other hand, the beamforming results were obtained with an uncorrected statistical threshold. Although the pre-SMA and right SMG were in the locations shown by an earlier fMRI study (Hu et al. 2015a) and significant with small volume correction, the clusters identified for RT slowing were not exactly the same. Thus, while a liberal threshold may be needed to accommodate noisy trial-by-trial EEG responses and a lack of individual MRI-based head model for source reconstruction (Green and McDonald, 2009), these results would require replication.

We also showed that stimulus prediction error was encoded by low-beta band power. This finding is consistent with an earlier report that beta oscillations (∼13–30 Hz) react to unexpected, bottom-up factors (Arnal and Giraud, 2012; Engel and Fries, 2010). In sensory systems, low-beta oscillations (∼13–20 Hz) respond to prediction error, as would occur during an unexpected or oddball stimulus (Arnal et al. 2011; Chang et al. 2016; Fujioka et al. 2009; Haenschel et al. 2000; Kopell et al. 2011). Interestingly, the time–frequency interval of the low-beta response appeared to be similar for auditory prediction error (Chang et al. 2016), potentially suggesting similar neural process across sensory modalities. Another study showed that beta oscillatory synchronization in the frontostriatal network was increased in response to a novel stimulus to support adaptation of ongoing behavior (Wessel et al. 2016). In other studies, the power of high-beta band (∼20–30 Hz) increased to unexpected positive rewards (Cohen et al. 2007; Marco-Pallares et al. 2008) and alpha to beta (∼8–25 Hz) power was negatively correlated with positive prediction error (Cavanagh, 2015). More work is needed to investigate whether low-beta oscillations elicited by stimulus prediction error can be localized to the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, as shown by fMRI (Ide et al. 2013; Silvetti et al. 2014; Hu et al. 2015a; Manza et al. 2016). Likewise, many studies have shown that alpha oscillations are associated with top-down processes, including sustained alertness, selective attention, and stimulus-driven adaptive control (Sadaghiani and Kleinschmidt, 2016). Given their broad role in cognitive processes, it remains to be seen how alpha oscillations may partake in proactive control in the stop-signal task.

An important consideration is whether or how P(stop) as computed by the Bayesian model relates linearly to the construct of conflict anticipation. For instance, an extreme value of P(stop) (e.g., 0.9) would suggest full anticipation of a stop signal and readiness to stop, thus involving little conflict. On the other hand, P(stop) as estimated by the Bayesian model ranged from 0 to ∼0.45 (see Fig. 2 of Ide et al. 2013 and Fig. 1 of Hu et al. 2015a) with a peak at 0.25—the probability of the stop signal dictated by the experiment. In the current experiment, P(stop) estimates centered around a lower range. Thus, it appears that even at an upper value of, say, 0.4 of P(stop), participants predominantly expect to respond to go signal. If “conflict” reflects the conflicting processes between responding and withholding a response, P(stop) would seem to monotonically reflect the extent of conflict anticipation within this estimated range of values. It is not clear why the P(stop) in the current study spanned a lower range, compared with earlier studies. Of the task parameters, the only difference is a shorter foreperiod in the current EEG (1–3 s) compared to earlier fMRI (1–5 s) studies. This observation suggests the need of more work to investigate the influence of task variables on model outcomes of SST performance.

Another issue pertains to the potential differences in the influence of stop success and error trials on proactive control. A previous study of a saccade countermanding task showed post-success but not post-error slowing in macaque monkeys (Emeric et al. 2007). Although inconsistent with earlier work of choice RT tasks, this finding suggests that monkeys may perform to optimize their reward (water intake) by executing fast saccades most of the time and only slowing down after successfully cancelled (rewarded) trials, in favor of positive versus negative reinforcement. Stop success and error trials may involve distinct psychological processes and contribute to proactive control via distinct mechanisms according to experimental contexts (Li et al. 2008; Chang et al. 2014; unpublished observations). New models would be needed to capture the potential differences in the influence of success and error trials and, more broadly, how positive and negative reward contingencies may modulate cognitive control differently.

In conclusion, the current study provides evidence for distinct time–frequency neural mechanisms of conflict anticipation and behavioral slowing in the stop-signal task. Conflict anticipation was reflected in the power in low-theta band (3–5 Hz) during the foreperiod, and behavioral slowing was reflected in the power in delta-theta band (2–8 Hz) during motor response. The magnitude of power in these time–frequency clusters was significantly correlated on a trial-by-trial basis. These findings substantiate a neural oscillatory mechanism associating conflict anticipation to behavioral adjustment.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: Andrew Chang is now at the Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster University. Hsin-Hung Li is now at the Department of Psychology, New York University. We will share all of our experiment codes on request, including those to run the behavioral paradigm, models of behavioral data, and EEG data analyses.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This study was supported by Taiwan MOST (Taiwan) 103-2410-H-002-076-MY3 (CCC); NIH grants DA023248, DA026990 (CSRL), and AA021449 (CSRL); and NSF grant BCS1309260 (CSRL). The funding agencies were otherwise not involved in data collection and analysis or in the decision in publishing the results.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Angelini M, Calbi M, Ferrari A, Sbriscia-Fioretti B, Franca M, Gallese V, Umiltà MA (2016) Proactive control strategies for overt and covert go/nogo tasks: an electrical neuroimaging study. PLoS One 11:e0152188. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0152188 pmid:27010832
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Arnal LH, Wyart V, Giraud AL (2011) Transitions in neural oscillations reflect prediction errors generated in audiovisual speech. Nat Neurosci 14:797–801. doi:10.1038/nn.2810
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Arnal LH, Giraud AL (2012) Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends Cogn Sci 16:390–398. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.003 pmid:22682813
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Bissett PG, Logan GD (2011) Balancing cognitive demands: control adjustments in the stop-signal paradigm. J Exp Psychol Learn 37:392–404. doi:10.1037/a0021800
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Brass M, Haggard P (2007) To do or not to do: the neural signature of self-control. J Neurosci 27:9141–9145. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0924-07.2007 pmid:17715350
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Cai W, Ryali S, Chen T, Li CSR, Menon V (2014) Dissociable roles of right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula in inhibitory control: evidence from intrinsic and task-related functional parcellation, connectivity, and response profile analyses across multiple datasets. J Neurosci 34:14652–14667. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3048-14.2014 pmid:25355218
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Camerer C, Ho TH (1999) Experience-weighted attraction learning in normal form games. Econometrica 67:827–874. doi:10.1111/1468-0262.00054
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Cavanagh JF, Cohen MX, Allen JJ (2009) Prelude to and resolution of an error: EEG phase synchrony reveals cognitive control dynamics during action monitoring. J Neurosci 29:98–105. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4137-08.2009 pmid:19129388
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ (2014) Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci 18:414–421. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012 pmid:24835663
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Cavanagh JF, Shackman AJ (2015) Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence. J Physiology-Paris 109:3–15. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Cavanagh JF, Zambrano-Vazquez L, Allen JJ (2012) Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. Psychophysiology 49:220–238. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01293.x pmid:22091878
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Cavanagh JF (2015) Cortical delta activity reflects reward prediction error and related behavioral adjustments, but at different times. NeuroImage 110:205–216. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.007
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    Chang A, Bosnyak DJ, Trainor LJ (2016) Unpredicted pitch modulates beta oscillatory power during rhythmic entrainment to a tone sequence. Front Psychol 7:327. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00327 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00327 pmid:27014138
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Chang A, Chen CC, Li HH, Li CSR (2014) Event-related potentials for post-error and post-conflict slowing. PLoS ONE 9:e99909. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099909 10.1371/journal.pone.0099909 pmid:24932780
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Chang A, Chen CC, Li HH, Li CSR (2015) Perigenual anterior cingulate event-related potential precedes stop signal errors. NeuroImage 111:179–185. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.017
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    Chikazoe J, Jimura K, Hirose S, Yamashita KI, Miyashita Y, Konishi S (2009) Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop-signal task. J Neurosci 29:15870–15877. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3645-09.2009
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    Chmielewski WX, Mückschel M, Dippel G, Beste C (2016) Concurrent information affects response inhibition processes via the modulation of theta oscillations in cognitive control networks. Brain Struct Func 221:3949–3961. doi:10.1007/s00429-015-1137-1 pmid:26543023
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Clayton MS, Yeung N, Kadosh RC (2015) The roles of cortical oscillations in sustained attention. Trends Cogn Sci 19:188–195. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Cohen D, Cuffin BN, Yunokuchi K, Maniewski R, Purcell C, Cosgrove GR, Ives J, Kennedy JG, Schomer DL (1990) MEG versus EEG localization test using implanted sources in the human brain. Ann Neurol 28:811–817. doi:10.1002/ana.410280613
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Cohen MX (2014a) A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling. Trends Neurosci 37:480–490. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099909
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Cohen MX (2014b) Non-parametric permutation testing. In: Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
  22. ↵
    Cohen MX (2016) Midfrontal theta tracks action monitoring over multiple interactive time scales. NeuroImage 141:262–272. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.054 pmid:27475291
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Cohen MX, Cavanagh JF (2011) Single-trial regression elucidates the role of prefrontal theta oscillations in response conflict. Frontier Psychol 2:30. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00030
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Cohen MX, Donner TH (2013) Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. J Neurophysiol 110:2752–2763. doi:10.1152/jn.00479.2013 pmid:24068756
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Cohen MX, Elger CE, Ranganath C (2007) Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. NeuroImage 35:968–978. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.056 pmid:17257860
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Cohen MX, Ridderinkhof KR (2013) EEG source reconstruction reveals frontal-parietal dynamics of spatial conflict processing. PloS One 8:e57293. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057293 pmid:23451201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Cohen MX, van Gaal S (2014) Subthreshold muscle twitches dissociate oscillatory neural signatures of conflicts from errors. NeuroImage 86:503–513. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.033 pmid:24185026
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Conroy MA, Polich J (2007) Normative variation of P3a and P3b from a large sample: gender, topography, and response time. J Psychophysiol 21:22–32. doi:10.1027/0269-8803.21.1.22
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. ↵
    Cooper PS, Darriba Á, Karayanidis F, Barceló F (2016) Contextually sensitive power changes across multiple frequency bands underpin cognitive control. NeuroImage 132:499–511. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.010 pmid:26975557
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Cooper PS, Wong AS, Fulham WR, Thienel R, Mansfield E, Michie PT, Karayanidis F (2015) Theta frontoparietal connectivity associated with proactive and reactive cognitive control processes. NeuroImage 108:354–363. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.028 pmid:25528657
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Cuffin BN, Cohen D, Yunokuchi K, Maniewski R, Purcell C, Cosgrove GR, Ives J, Kennedy J, Schomer D (1991) Tests of EEG localization accuracy using implanted sources in the human brain. Ann Neurol 29:132–138. doi:10.1002/ana.410290204
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Daitch AL, Sharma M, Roland JL, Astafiev SV, Bundy DT, Gaona CM, Snyder AZ, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2013) Frequency-specific mechanism links human brain networks for spatial attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:19585–19590. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307947110 pmid:24218604
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    Di Russo F, Lucci G, Sulpizio V, Berchicci M, Spinelli D, Pitzalis S, Galati G (2016) Spatiotemporal brain mapping during preparation, perception, and action. NeuroImage 126:1–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.036 pmid:26608247
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Emeric EE, Brown JW, Boucher L, Carpenter RHS, Hanes DP, Harris R, Logan GD, Mashru RN, Paré M, Pouget P, Stuphorn V, Taylor TL, Schall JD (2007) Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys. Vision Res 47:35–49. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.08.032
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Engel AK, Fries P (2010) Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:156–165. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015 pmid:20359884
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Fries P (2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 9:474–480. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011 pmid:16150631
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Fujioka T, Trainor LJ, Large EW, Ross B (2009) Beta and gamma rhythms in human auditory cortex during musical beat processing. Ann NY Acad Sci 1169:89–92. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04779.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Green JJ, McDonald JJ (2009) A practical guide to beamformer source reconstruction for EEG. In T. C. Handy (Ed.), Brain Signal Analysis: Advances in Neuroelectric and Neuromagnetic Methods. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099909
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Gross J, Kujala J, Hämäläinen M, Timmermann L, Schnitzler A, Salmelin R (2001) Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: studying neural interactions in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:694–699. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.694
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    Haenschel C, Baldeweg T, Croft RJ, Whittington M, Gruzelier J (2000) Gamma and beta frequency oscillations in response to novel auditory stimuli: a comparison of human electroencephalogram (EEG) data with in vitro models. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:7645–7650. doi:10.1073/pnas.120162397
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Harper J, Malone SM, Bernat EM (2014) Theta and delta band activity explain N2 and P3 ERP component activity in a go/no-go task. Clin Neurophysiol 125:124–132. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2013.06.025
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. ↵
    Heinze HJ, Luck SJ, Mangun GR, Hillyard SA (1990) Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. I. Evidence for early selection. Electroen Clin Neuro 75:511–527. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(90)90138-A
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Horga G, Maia TV, Wang P, Wang Z, Marsh R, Peterson BS (2011) Adaptation to conflict via context-driven anticipatory signals in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 31:16208–16216. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2783-11.2011 pmid:22072672
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    Hu S, Ide JS, Zhang S, Li CSR (2015a) Anticipating conflict: neural correlates of a Bayesian belief and its motor consequence. NeuroImage 119:286–295. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099909
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Hu S, Ide JS, Zhang S, Sinha R, Li CSR (2015b) Conflict anticipation in alcohol dependence—a model-based fMRI study of stop signal task. NeuroImage-Clin 8:39–50. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.03.008 pmid:26106526
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Huster RJ, Enriquez-Geppert S, Lavallee CF, Falkenstein M, Herrmann CS (2013) Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions. Int J Psychophysiol 87:217–233. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.001 pmid:22906815
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Hwang K, Ghuman AS, Manoach DS, Jones SR, Luna B (2016) Frontal preparatory neural oscillations associated with cognitive control: a developmental study comparing young adults and adolescents. NeuroImage 136:139–148. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.017 pmid:27173759
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Ide JS, Hu S, Zhang S, Yu AJ, Li CSR (2015) Impaired Bayesian learning for cognitive control in cocaine dependence. Drug Alcohol Depen 151:220–227. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.03.021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Ide JS, Shenoy P, Yu AJ, Li CSR (2013) Bayesian prediction and evaluation in the anterior cingulate cortex. J Neurosci 33:2039–2047. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2201-12.2013 pmid:23365241
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    Ito S, Stuphorn V, Brown JW, Schall JD (2003) Performance monitoring by the anterior cingulate cortex during saccade countermanding. Science 302:120–122. doi:10.1126/science.1087847 pmid:14526085
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    Jaffard M, Longcamp M, Velay JL, Anton JL, Roth M, Nazarian B, Boulinguez P (2008) Proactive inhibitory control of movement assessed by event-related fMRI. NeuroImage 42:1196–1206. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.041 pmid:18588986
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Jahfari S, Stinear CM, Claffey M, Verbruggen F, Aron AR (2010) Responding with restraint: what are the neurocognitive mechanisms? J Cogn Neurosci 22:1479–1492. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21307 pmid:19583473
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Karayanidis F, Jamadar SD (2014) Event-related potentials reveal multiple components of proactive and reactive control in task-switching. Task Switching and Cognitive Control. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp. 200–236.
  54. ↵
    Kenemans JL (2015) Specific proactive and generic reactive inhibition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 56:115–126. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.011 pmid:26116545
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Kennard C, Mannan SK, Nachev P, Parton A, Mort DJ, Rees G, Hodgson TL, Husain M (2005) Cognitive processes in saccade generation. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1039:176–183. doi:10.1196/annals.1325.017 pmid:15826972
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Kok A, Ramautar JR, De Ruiter MB, Band GP, Ridderinkhof KR (2004) ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. Psychophysiol 41:9–20. doi:10.1046/j.1469-8986.2003.00127.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Kopell N, Whittington MA, Kramer MA (2011) Neuronal assembly dynamics in the beta1 frequency range permits short-term memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:3779–3784. doi:10.1073/pnas.1019676108 pmid:21321198
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    Kühn S, Haggard P, Brass M (2009) Intentional inhibition: How the “veto-area” exerts control. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2834–2843. doi:10.1002/hbm.20711 pmid:19072994
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Langford ZD, Krebs RM, Talsma D, Woldorff MG, Boehler CN (2016) Strategic down‐regulation of attentional resources as a mechanism of proactive response inhibition. Eur J Neurosci 44:2095–2103. doi:10.1111/ejn.13303 pmid:27306544
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    Lavallee CF, Meemken MT, Herrmann CS, Huster RJ (2014) When holding your horses meets the deer in the headlights: time-frequency characteristics of global and selective stopping under conditions of proactive and reactive control. Front Hum Neurosci 8:994. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00994 pmid:25540615
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Lee TW, Girolami M, Sejnowski TJ (1999) Independent component analysis using an extended infomax algorithm for mixed subgaussian and supergaussian sources. Neural Comput 11:417–441. pmid:9950738
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Leunissen I, Coxon JP, Swinnen SP (2016) A proactive task set influences how response inhibition is implemented in the basal ganglia. Hum Brain Mapp 37:4706–4717. doi:10.1002/hbm.23338 pmid:27489078
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Levitt H (1971) Transformed up‐down methods in psychoacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 49:467–477. doi:10.1121/1.1912375
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Li CSR, Huang C, Yan P, Paliwal P, Constable RT, Sinha R (2008) Neural correlates of post-error slowing during a stop signal task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Cogn Neurosci 20:1021–1029. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20071
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Logan GD, Schachar RJ, Tannock R (1997) Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol Sci 8:60–64. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00545.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  66. ↵
    Manza P, Hu S, Ide JS, Farr OM, Zhang S, Leung HC, Li CSR (2016) The effects of methylphenidate on cerebral responses to conflict anticipation and unsigned prediction error in a stop-signal task. J Psychopharmacol 30:283–293. doi:10.1177/0269881115625102
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Marco-Pallares J, Cucurell D, Cunillera T, García R, Andrés-Pueyo A, Münte TF, Rodríguez-Fornells A (2008) Human oscillatory activity associated to reward processing in a gambling task. Neuropsychologia 46:241–248. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.016
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    Maris E, Oostenveld R (2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. J Neurosci Meth 164:177–190. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 pmid:17517438
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    McDowell JE, Dyckman KA, Austin BP, Clementz BA (2008) Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: evidence from studies of humans. Brain Cogn 68:255–270. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.016 pmid:18835656
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    Meyer BU, Röricht S, Woiciechowsky C (1998) Topography of fibers in the human corpus callosum mediating interhemispheric inhibition between the motor cortices. Ann Neurol 43:360–369. doi:10.1002/ana.410430314 pmid:9506553
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    Min BK, Park HJ (2010) Task-related modulation of anterior theta and posterior alpha EEG reflects top-down preparation. BMC Neurosci 11:79. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-11-79 pmid:20584297
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    Morein-Zamir S, Chua R, Franks I, Nagelkerke P, Kingstone A (2007) Predictability influences stopping and response control. J Exp Psychol Human 33:149–162. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.149
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    Nigbur R, Ivanova G, Stürmer B (2011) Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference. Clin Neurophysiol 122:2185–2194. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.03.030 pmid:21550845
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    Nyhus E, Curran T (2010) Functional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:1023–1035. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.014 pmid:20060015
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    O’Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, Friston K, Dolan RJ (2004) Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304:452–454.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  76. ↵
    Pastötter B, Dreisbach G, Bäuml KHT (2013) Dynamic adjustments of cognitive control: oscillatory correlates of the conflict adaptation effect. J Cogn Neurosci 25:2167–2178. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00474
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    Pastötter B, Hanslmayr S, Bäuml KHT (2010) Conflict processing in the anterior cingulate cortex constrains response priming. NeuroImage 50:1599–1605. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.095
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    Phillips JM, Vinck M, Everling S, Womelsdorf T (2014) A long-range fronto-parietal 5- to 10-Hz network predicts “top-down” controlled guidance in a task-switch paradigm. Cereb Cortex 24:1996–2008. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 118:2128–2148. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019 pmid:17573239
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    Rushworth MFS, Walton ME, Kennerley SW, Bannerman DM (2004) Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 8:410–417. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    Sadaghiani S, Kleinschmidt A (2016) Brain networks and α-oscillations: structural and functional foundations of cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci 20:805–817. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.004 pmid:27707588
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    Scheeringa R, Petersson KM, Oostenveld R, Norris DG, Hagoort P, Bastiaansen MC (2009) Trial-by-trial coupling between EEG and BOLD identifies networks related to alpha and theta EEG power increases during working memory maintenance. NeuroImage 44:1224–1238. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.041 pmid:18840533
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    Schevernels H, Bombeke K, Van der Borght L, Hopf JM, Krebs RM, Boehler CN (2015) Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of proactive and reactive control in a rewarded stop-signal task. NeuroImage 121:115–125. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.023
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  84. ↵
    Silvetti M, Castellar EN, Roger C, Verguts T (2014) Reward expectation and prediction error in human medial frontal cortex: an EEG study. NeuroImage 84:376–382. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.058
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    Slotnick SD (2004). Source localization of ERP generators. In T.C. Todd (Eds). Event-Related Potentials: A Methods Handbook. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 149–166.
  86. ↵
    Stuphorn V, Schall JD (2006) Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field. Nat Neurosci 9:925–931. doi:10.1038/nn1714 pmid:16732274
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    Stuphorn V, Taylor TL, Schall JD (2000) Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye field. Nature 408:857–860. doi:10.1038/35048576 pmid:11130724
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    Suzuki K, Shinoda H (2015) Transition from reactive control to proactive control across conflict adaptation: An sLORETA study. Brain Cogn 100:7–14. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.09.001 pmid:26432378
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    Szczepanski SM, Crone NE, Kuperman RA, Auguste KI, Parvizi J, Knight RT (2014) Dynamic changes in phase-amplitude coupling facilitate spatial attention control in fronto-parietal cortex. PLoS Biol 12:e1001936. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001936 pmid:25157678
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    van Driel J, Sligte IG, Linders J, Elport D, Cohen MX (2015) Frequency band-specific electrical brain stimulation modulates cognitive control processes. PloS One 10:e0138984. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0138984 pmid:26405801
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    van Noordt SJ, Campopiano A, Segalowitz SJ (2016a) A functional classification of medial frontal negativity ERPs: theta oscillations and single subject effects. Psychophysiology 53:1317–1334.
    OpenUrl
  92. ↵
    van Noordt SJ, Desjardins JA, Gogo CE, Tekok-Kilic A, Segalowitz SJ (2016b) Cognitive control in the eye of the beholder: electrocortical theta and alpha modulation during response preparation in a cued saccade task. NeuroImage pii:S1053-8119(16)30533-X.
  93. ↵
    Verbruggen F, Logan GD (2008) Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. J Exp Psychol Gen 137:649–672. doi:10.1037/a0013170 pmid:18999358
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    Verbruggen F, Logan GD (2009a) Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33:647–661. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099909
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    Verbruggen F, Logan GD (2009b) Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. J Exp Psychol Human 35:835–854. doi:10.1037/a0012726 pmid:19485695
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    Vink M, Kahn RS, Raemaekers M, van den Heuvel M, Boersma M, Ramsey NF (2005) Function of striatum beyond inhibition and execution of motor responses. Hum Brain Mapp 25:336–344. doi:10.1002/hbm.20111 pmid:15852388
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    Voytek B, Kayser AS, Badre D, Fegen D, Chang EF, Crone NE, Parvizi J, Knight RT, D’Esposito M (2015) Oscillatory dynamics coordinating human frontal networks in support of goal maintenance. Nat Neurosci 18:1318–1324. doi:10.1038/nn.4071 pmid:26214371
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    Wang W, Viswanathan S, Lee T, Grafton ST (2016) Coupling between theta oscillations and cognitive control network during cross-modal visual and auditory attention: supramodal vs modality-specific mechanisms. PloS One 11:e0158465. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0158465 pmid:27391013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    Wessel JR, Ullsperger M, Obrig H, Villringer A, Quinque E, Schroeter ML, Bretschneider KJ, Arelin K, Roggenhofer E, Frisch S, Klein TA (2016) Neural synchrony indexes impaired motor slowing after errors and novelty following white matter damage. Neurobiol Aging 38:205–213. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.10.014 pmid:26563990
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. ↵
    Wolpe N, Moore JW, Rae CL, Rittman T, Altena E, Haggard P, Rowe JB (2013) The medial frontal-prefrontal network for altered awareness and control of action in corticobasal syndrome. Brain 137:208–220. doi:10.1093/brain/awt302 pmid:24293266
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  101. ↵
    Woods DL, Alho K, Algazi A (1992) Intermodal selective attention. I. Effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. Electroen Clin Neuro 82:341–355. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(92)90004-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  102. ↵
    Yu AJ, Cohen JD (2009) Sequential effects: superstition or rational behavior? In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, pp. 1873–1880. [CrossRef][10.1371/journal.pone.0099909]

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Bradley Postle, University of Wisconsin

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Michael Cohen, Patrick Bissett.

As you can see, you received an excellent set of reviews, with one reviewer particularly expert on EEG analyses and the other on the theoretical and empirical study of Stop tasks. Both make recommendations that will require some additional analyses and perhaps some expository modifications, but neither raises serious conceptual concerns, nor suggests the need for additional data collection.

R#1

This study makes a good contribution to the literature. It should definitely be published and I think will be well-received. But before giving the green light, I have some concerns that should be addressed.

Please insert the university in lines 85/6.

Page 9: -500 to 1000 is not a “longer epoch.” I would actually say it's on the short side for time-frequency analyses. This leads to my major concern:

Edge artifacts can last a few cycles per frequency. With only 200 ms between epoch onset and baseline onset, only the highest frequencies analyzed here are truely safe from edge artifacts. All the lower frequencies, including theta, have the risk of contamination. It's possible that the results are OK, but I would not feel comfortable with using such short time windows. There should be at least a full cycle of the lowest frequency on either side of the time window of interest, and preferably closer to three. Or maybe Besa does something to account for this? If so, the procedure must be described. If not, the authors should re-analyze the data using larger epochs (the “buffer zones” could be excluded from the final results and therefore plots and statistical evaluations).

Line 182: Wavelet convolution undeniably extracts time-frequency features from the data. Whether those features are “neural oscillations” is a whole other can of worms (this is discussed in many places, including the Cohen and Donner 2013 paper cited by the authors). I doubt the authors want to get caught up in this (rather obtuse and, at times, semantic) issue. Therefore, I recommend softening the language to speak more concretely about time-frequency patterns/results/responses, as leave the “hot” term oscillation to the introduction and Discussion sections (i.e., interpretation of the results as opposed to description of the results).

Here is my major conceptual concern: What is p(stop) really telling us? The Bayesian model is not a biophysical model, so it is telling us something conceptual/abstract. The model output correlates extraordinarily strongly with RT (~80% shared variance!). Is p(stop) just a proxy measure for RT? Apparently not, because the authors find dissociations in patterns of time-frequency correlations with p(stop) vs. goRT. I guess my “concern” here is really confusion -- how can two measures that correlate so strongly with each other correlate so differently with the EEG signal? This needs a more in-depth discussion, perhaps also with additional visualizations.

It would be interesting to see the trial series plot of p(stop). At present we see only distributions, but it would be interesting to see how p(stop) changes over trials, e.g., how much temporal variability there is.

line 283/4: what does [a/b] mean in, e.g., 10^(-8 b)?

I don't completely understand the model-fitting procedure. What was the k-order (trial history) of the model? And what was the balance between the order and the estimates of the coefficients? Maybe it would also help to make a plot of the k's for all subjects.

I have a new policy that I never recommend acceptance of a paper until analysis code is put online (personal website, github, google-code, figshare, supplemental materials, anything), with a clear description and link in the paper. Preferably the data should be posted as well, but at least the analysis and Bayesian modeling code.

R#2

In this work, the authors are interested in conflict and proactive control. To these ends, they relate behavior in the stop-signal task to electrophysiology. They show a few related results. First, they show that the probability of a stop signal was positively correlated with power in the low theta band at fixation. Second, they show that go RT was negative correlated with power in the wider delta-theta band at go stimulus onset. Third, they show that stimulus prediction error was positively correlated with power in the low-beta band at stop signal onset. Finally, they show that power of the probability of stop cluster and go-RT cluster are negatively correlated, suggesting that anticipating a stop signal is related to slower RT.

The authors explore a fundamental and importantly set of constructs, specifically, the relationship between proactive control, conflict, and response control. For this reason, I am enthusiastic about the paper. However, the following set of issues somewhat dampen my enthusiasm.

1. The authors seem to neglect a large swath of the stopping literature that has addressed sequential control adjustments. This includes (but it not limited to) the following work:

Bissett & Logan, 2011 JEP:LMC

Morein-Zamir et al., 2007 JEP:HPP

Rieger & Gauggel, 1999 British J of Psych

Verbruggen et al., 2008 JEP:HPP

Verbruggen & Logan, 2008 JEP:HPP

Verbruggen & Logan, 2008 QJEP

Verbruggen & Logan, 2009 JEP: HPP

Points 2 and 3 below also relate to this issue. Much of this previous work is focused on the same theoretical space of the current manuscript, including conflict, expectancy, and proactive vs. reactive control, and it uses the same basic paradigm (stop signal). Much of this work also presents alternative, simpler methods for computing sequential control adjustments (e.g., compare RT after a stop signal to RT before a stop signal). In order to maximize the impact of this work, it seems necessary to more thoroughly synthesize the current ideas with the existing literature.

2. The authors cite Lavallee et al., 2014 in passing, an EEG study of the time-frequency characteristic of stopping behavior. Considering the overlap in methods and goals and the cursory mention of it in the text of this manuscript, the reader is left to wonder how this current work goes beyond that previous work. I recommend the authors draw a clearer distinction between what has been done in previous work and what is new here.

3. The authors assert that “the extent of conflict adaptation --[is the] belief of an upcoming stop signal...”. However, the relationship between p(stop) and the concept of conflict remains unclear. Is it a linear, increasing function such that higher p(stop) yields higher conflict adaptation? What if the p(stop) approaches 1? Is it maximal at 50% p(stop) because there is maximal uncertainty about the upcoming behavior (i.e., go or stop)?

On a related point, others, including Schall and colleagues (e.g., Emeric et al., 2007 Vision Research; Ito et al., 2003 Science; Stuphorn & Schall, 2006 Nature Neuro; Stuphorn et al., 2000 Nature) have suggested that conflict is maximal specifically on successful stop (not failed stop or the anticipation of either stop). Can the current instantiation of conflict and conflict adaptation be synthesized with the way that Schall and colleagues discuss conflict?

4. Though there is some historical precedent for a simple go task (e.g., Lappin & Eriksen, 1966 JEP; Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984 JEP:HPP; Ollman, 1973 A&P IV), the overwhelming majority of stop-signal tasks use a choice go task. Here, the RTs (405ms) are much longer than are normally observed in a simple RT tasks, both without (Donders 1868/1969 Acta Psychologica) and with (Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984 JEP:HPP) stop signals. Therefore, it appears that subjects are not taking the instructions to respond quickly to the go signal very seriously.

In order to ensure that the combination of the somewhat unusual simple go task and the considerable go RT slowing does not affect any of the main dependent variables, I recommend some follow-up analyses/questions. Does RT slow over the course of the experiment (if so, SSRT may need to be adjusted: Verbruggen, Chambers, & Logan, 2013 Psych Science)? Is there considerable individual differences in this slowing? Is the race model violated in any subjects (i.e., are RT as long or longer on stop-failure trials than go trials?)? Did a subset of subjects wait until the RT deadline of 1s? Was there any sign that the upper tail of the RT distribution was truncated for any subjects by the 1s deadline? Did the SSD tracking algorithm “work” (i.e., stop success rate ~50%) for all subjects? It seems that the addition of analyses like the ones that I just mentioned could help the reader to ensure that the performance is not aberrant in any way.

5. Related to point #4, the authors note that many trials (24.1%) were removed in preprocessing. The authors say that this might have occurred because of long epochs. Were there more exclusion later in the epochs, which would be consistent with this idea? Also, did the trials that were excluded differ from the trials that were included in behavior? With such a large rate of exclusion, it seems necessary to ensure that these 24.1% of trials with artifacts in their EEG signal did not also include anomalous behavior that perhaps should be understood instead of thrown out.

Smaller issues:

Why use a maximum likelihood procedure to find the critical SSD? What does this buy over more standard methods like taking the mean/median SSD?

It was unclear why P(stop) in figure 1 was on average so low. It may be worth unpacking why it isn't .25 (like the overall probability of a stop trial in the experiment).

The thresholds for voxel clustering (2,000mm3 and 20,000mm3) were seemingly not justified by any means, citations or otherwise. Such a choice seems to require some form of justification.

The authors focus on a single electrode, and I understand the desire to reduce multiple comparisons. However, it would seem useful to at least investigate (in a separate, post-hoc analyses) the possibility of additional activation across other electrodes to see if any useful discoveries could be made through exploration.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 4 (3)
eNeuro
Vol. 4, Issue 3
May/June 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Proactive Control: Neural Oscillatory Correlates of Conflict Anticipation and Response Slowing
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Proactive Control: Neural Oscillatory Correlates of Conflict Anticipation and Response Slowing
Andrew Chang, Jaime S. Ide, Hsin-Hung Li, Chien-Chung Chen, Chiang-Shan R. Li
eNeuro 16 May 2017, 4 (3) ENEURO.0061-17.2017; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0061-17.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Proactive Control: Neural Oscillatory Correlates of Conflict Anticipation and Response Slowing
Andrew Chang, Jaime S. Ide, Hsin-Hung Li, Chien-Chung Chen, Chiang-Shan R. Li
eNeuro 16 May 2017, 4 (3) ENEURO.0061-17.2017; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0061-17.2017
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • Bayesian Model
  • Electroencephalogram (EEG)
  • neural oscillation
  • Proactive Control
  • Stop-Signal Task

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

New Research

  • LIQ HD (Lick Instance Quantifier Home cage Device): An open-source tool for recording undisturbed two-bottle drinking behavior in a home cage environment
  • Insulin-like growth factor-1 supplementation promotes brain maturation in preterm pigs
  • SK and Kv4 channels limit spike timing perturbations in pacemaking dopamine neurons
Show more New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Deciding while acting - Mid-movement decisions are more strongly affected by action probability than reward amount
  • Environment Enrichment Facilitates Long-Term Memory Consolidation Through Behavioral Tagging
  • Effects of Cortical FoxP1 Knockdowns on Learned Song Preference in Female Zebra Finches
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.