Abstract
Intellectual disability in Down syndrome (DS) is accompanied by altered neuro-architecture, deficient synaptic plasticity, and excitation-inhibition imbalance in critical brain regions for learning and memory. Recently, we have demonstrated beneficial effects of a combined treatment with green tea extract containing (-)-epigallocatechin-3-gallate (EGCG) and cognitive stimulation in young adult DS individuals. Although we could reproduce the cognitive-enhancing effects in mouse models, the underlying mechanisms of these beneficial effects are unknown. Here, we explored the effects of a combined therapy with environmental enrichment (EE) and EGCG in the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS at young age. Our results show that combined EE-EGCG treatment improved corticohippocampal-dependent learning and memory. Cognitive improvements were accompanied by a rescue of cornu ammonis 1 (CA1) dendritic spine density and a normalization of the proportion of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic markers in CA1 and dentate gyrus.
- (-)-epigallocatechin-3-gallate
- Down syndrome
- environmental enrichment
- excitation-inhibition imbalance
- neuroplasticity
Significance Statement
Therapeutic methods for improving intellectual disability in Down syndrome (DS) are limited, and their outcome remains unsatisfactory. Recently, we demonstrated that combined treatment with (-)-epigallocatechin-3-gallate (EGCG) and cognitive stimulation rescued cognitive deficits in DS individuals in a phase II clinical trial and also in a middle-age Ts65Dn mouse model of DS. Here, we show that environmental enrichment (EE) plus EGCG treatment improves corticohippocampal-dependent learning and memory deficits in young trisomic mice, restores cornu ammonis 1 (CA1) hippocampal dendritic spine density, and mitigates disruptions in excitatory/inhibitory synaptic puncta in CA1 and dentate gyrus. Our results suggest that therapies with the capacity to simultaneously target several abnormal processes underlying intellectual disability and use physiologic plasticity-enhancing interventions such as EE are optimal for disease-modifying interventions.
Introduction
Down syndrome (DS) is the most common genetic form of intellectual disability, with ∼10 in 10,000 and 14 in 10,000 live births in European countries (Khoshnood et al., 2011) and the United States (Parker et al., 2010), respectively. It arises from the presence of an extra copy (or major portion) of chromosome 21 (Hsa21), leading to a complex genetic imbalance (Antonarakis et al., 2004). Individuals with DS show moderate to severe cognitive impairment, with an average intellectual quotient of 40–50 (de Sola et al., 2015) and 39.4% in the mild intellectual disability range of 50–70. The neuropsychological profile is characterized by marked hippocampal-dependent deficits particularly affecting spatial learning, memory, and executive functions, among other cognitive domains (Chapman and Hesketh, 2000; Nadel, 2003; Pennington et al., 2003). These cognitive deficits are associated with distinct neuro-architectural, synaptic, and neurochemical alterations (Lott and Dierssen, 2010; Dierssen, 2012). At the cellular level, there is a reduction in dendritic number and complexity in cortical and hippocampal neurons, which affects synaptic connectivity (Becker et al., 1986). Furthermore, accumulating evidence suggest that DS pathophysiology is tightly associated with a disruption of the balance between the excitatory and inhibitory neuronal systems (Reynolds and Warner, 1988; Risser et al., 1997; Seidl et al., 2001; Bhattacharyya et al., 2009). These abnormalities are of particular importance because they are related to disruptions in neural plasticity, which is essential for cognition (Baroncelli et al., 2011).
Several research groups have shown that it is possible to partially rescue DS phenotypes using nonpharmacological strategies such as postnatal handling or cognitive training by environmental enrichment (EE) that ameliorate behavioral and brain alterations in the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS (Martínez-Cué et al., 2002; Dierssen, 2003; Begenisic et al., 2011; Chakrabarti et al., 2011; Golabek et al., 2011). It is widely accepted that EE is a cognitive enhancing intervention that promotes synaptic plasticity, adult neurogenesis, and epigenetic modifications, among other processes (Sale et al., 2014). However, despite its beneficial effects, EE is not sufficient to promote long-lasting dendritic spine remodeling in Ts65Dn mice (Dierssen, 2003) or significant developmental changes in DS children (Mahoney et al., 2004).
More recently, (-)-epigallocatechin-3-gallate (EGCG), the most abundant catechin found in green tea, with antioxidant and neuroprotective properties, has been shown to efficiently improve cognitive phenotypes in DS individuals and mouse models (De la Torre et al., 2014), ameliorate synaptic plasticity impairment in vitro (Xie et al., 2008), and restore excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) imbalance in Ts65Dn mice (Souchet et al., 2015). EGCG is a natural inhibitor of the kinase activity of Hsa21 candidate gene Dyrk1A (Bain et al., 2003), whose overexpression is sufficient to induce cognitive and neuromorphologic alterations (Altafaj et al., 2001; Martinez de Lagran et al., 2012) and that is also modulated by EE (Golabek et al., 2011; Pons-Espinal et al., 2013). Recently, we showed that combined treatment with EE and EGCG is more efficient than EE or EGCG alone to ameliorate age-associated cognitive impairment of older Ts65Dn mice (Catuara-Solarz et al., 2015), suggesting a synergistic mechanism. Furthermore, we demonstrated that combined treatment with cognitive training and EGCG is more efficient than cognitive training alone to promote cognitive enhancement as well as neurophysiological recovery in young adults with DS in a phase II clinical trial (de la Torre et al., 2016). Thus, here we explored the effects of combined EE-EGCG treatment on hippocampal cognitive, neuronal, and synaptic alterations in young adult Ts65Dn mice.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Ts65Dn (TS) and wild-type (WT) littermates were obtained through crossings of B6EiC3Sn a/A-Ts(1716)65Dn (Ts65Dn) females to B6C3F1/J males purchased from the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME; RRID:IMSR_JAX:001924). The mouse colony was bred in the Animal Facilities of the Barcelona Biomedical Research Park (PRBB, Barcelona, Spain). Mice were housed in standard or enriched conditions (see below) under a 12:12-h light:dark schedule (lights on at 8:00 a.m.) in controlled environmental conditions of humidity (60%) and temperature (22°C ± 2°C) with ad libitum access to food and water. Both the Ts65Dn and euploid mice were genotyped by quantitative PCR, in accordance with the Jackson Laboratory protocol.
Experiments were conducted using 1- to 2-month-old female mice. We used females because in EE conditions, Ts65Dn males show high levels of stress that could mask the effect of the treatment (Martínez-Cué et al., 2002). Although the estrus cycle may be slightly delayed in Ts65Dn mice, by the age of 2 months it is synchronized among all females (including Ts65Dn and euploid mice; Netzer et al., 2010). Thus, for the experiments conducted in this study, it is unlikely that variations in estrogen levels between mice could influence behavior, spine density, or E/I balance.
All animal procedures met the guidelines of European Community Directive 2010/63/EU and local guidelines (Real Decreto 53/2013) and were approved by the local ethics committee (Comité Ético de Experimentación Animal del PRBB; procedure numbers MDS-08-1060P2 and MDS-14-1611).
EE and EGCG
Ts65Dn and WT 1- to 2-month-old female mice were assigned using a simple randomization to either control conditions or a combination of EE and green tea extracts containing 45% EGCG. Mice received the treatments for 30 days based on previous studies (De la Torre et al., 2014; Catuara-Solarz et al., 2015). In the control conditions, animals were reared in conventional Plexiglas cages (20 × 12 × 12-cm height) in groups of 2–3 animals. EE housing consisted of spacious (55 × 80 × 50-cm height) cages with toys, small houses, tunnels, and platforms of different shapes, sizes, colors, and textures. Wheels were not introduced in the cages to avoid the effect of physical exercise. The arrangement was changed every 2 days to keep novelty conditions. To stimulate social interactions, 6–8 mice were housed in each cage. Green tea extract containing 45% EGCG was administered in drinking water (EGCG dosage: 0.326 mg/ml, 0.65 mg per day; 30 mg/kg per day) by preparing fresh EGCG solution every 2 days from a green tea leaf extract (Mega Green Tea Extract, Decaffeinated, Life Extension, Fort Lauderdale, FL; EGCG content 326.25 mg per capsule).
Morris water maze
The Morris water maze (MWM) was performed according to a previously described method (Catuara-Solarz et al., 2015). Briefly, mice were trained in a water maze (pool, 1.70-m diameter; platform, 12-cm diameter) during five learning sessions (four acquisition trials per session and one session per day). Twenty-four hours after the final acquisition session, mice underwent one probe/removal session (reference memory trial) in which the platform was removed, followed by one cued session. Starting the next day, three reversal sessions (four trials per session) were conducted in which the platform position was changed 180° to test cognitive flexibility as a measure of executive function. In every session, mice randomly entered the pool from four different positions and were allowed to search the platform for 60 s. The experimenter who performed all the MWM procedures was blind to mice genotype. Mice were video-tracked during the test, and their latency to reach the platform, total distance swum, time spent in periphery, and swimming speed were recorded using SMART software (Panlab, Barcelona, Spain, RRID:SCR_002852). Subsequently, data were computed with software developed by our lab (Jtracks; Arqué et al., 2008) to obtain other measurements to quantify the most efficient and direct trajectory from the location of mice to the platform, such as the Gallagher index (average distance from each mouse to the center of the platform), the Gallagher distance (accumulated distance from each mouse to the center of the platform), and the Whishaw index (percentage of path inside the optimal corridor connecting release site and goal; Whishaw and Jarrard, 1996). Mice that did not reach the platform in <30 s in the cue session were considered unsuitable for the test and were subtracted from the analysis. One overperforming mouse from the EE-EGCG–treated TS group was removed from the analysis. The estimate of the number of mice required (n = 10) was based on the expected difference between the experimental groups, deriving from previous data obtained in our laboratory (WT, n = 11; TS, n = 8; WT-EE-EGCG, n = 12; TS-EE-EGCG, n = 8).
Novel object recognition test
The novel object recognition test was performed as an adaptation from the protocols described in Leger et al. (2013). The procedure was conducted in a V-maze apparatus (wall height 27 cm, arm length 30 cm, and arm width 6 cm). First, mice were subjected to a 5-min habituation session during which they were allowed to explore the maze without any objects. The next day, mice went through a 10-min familiarization session in which two identical objects were situated at the end of each arm attached to the wall and the floor with adhesive tape. After a 60-min intertrial interval, the recognition test session was conducted consisting of a 5-min trial in which one of the objects used at familiarization was substituted by a new object. Recognition of the new object was assessed by calculating the discrimination index (DI) by the following formula DI = (novel object exploration time/total exploration time) – (familiar object exploration time/total exploration time) × 100. Exploratory behavior was defined as the mouse directing its head or sniffing toward the object at a distance of ∼1–2 cm. The estimate of the sample size was based on previous data obtained in our laboratory (WT, n = 8; TS, n = 7; WT-EE-EGCG, n = 7; TS-EE-EGCG, n = 7).
Golgi neuronal staining and dendritic spine imaging
To avoid confounding effects, these experiments were performed with mice that did not undergo behavioral assessment. Golgi staining was performed according to manufacturer instructions (SuperGolgi Kit, Bioenno Tech, Santa Ana, CA; cat. # 003010, RRID:AB_2620135). Mice were sacrificed with CO2 and perfused intracardially with 0.01 m PBS followed by chilled 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains were removed from the skull and hemispheres were immersed freshly into impregnation solution for 9 days. When impregnation was ready, tissue blocks were rinsed with distilled water and transferred into postimpregnation buffer for 4–5 days at room temperature (RT) in the dark. The solution was renewed after 1 day of immersion. After that, brains were cut with a vibratome (VT1000S; Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany) in sections of 150 μm and kept in collection and mounting buffer. Sections were mounted on adhesive microscope slides, and gentle pressure was applied with filter paper over the sections to enhance adhesion. Slides were washed in 0.01 m PBS/0.3%Triton X-100 for 20–30 min and placed in the staining solution for 20 min in a closed dark jar. After that, slides were moved to the poststaining buffer for 20 min in a dark area and washed in 0.01 m PBS/0.3% Triton X-100 for 15 min. Slides were dried in a closed jar at RT for 1 d. Finally, sections were dehydrated in 100% ethanol for 5–10 min and cleared in xylene for another 10 min. Slides were covered with coverslips using mounting medium and were kept at RT in a dark area.
Images were acquired from outer molecular layer secondary apical dendrites of granule neurons (located 40–90 μm from the neuronal soma) of the dentate gyrus (DG) and secondary apical dendrites from pyramidal neurons from the cornu ammonis 1 (CA1; located 50–100 μm from the neuronal soma). These hippocampal regions were selected because they play a critical role in the process and storage of spatial information (Tsien et al., 1996; Deng et al., 2010). We used an Olympus BX51 microscope with 100× objective and Neurolucida software (11.03.1, MBF Bioscience, Williston, VT; RRID:SCR_001775). Quantification of dendritic spine density was performed on 20-μm dendritic segment lengths using NIH ImageJ software version 1.46a and Multipoint plugin. The criterion to define dendritic spines was the identification of tridimensional protrusions emerging from the dendritic shaft that could be visualized across the z-planes. For this experiment, we used three brain slices per mouse of the dorsal hippocampus (bregma: 1.82–1.94, Paxinos and Franklin, 2012) of five to six mice per experimental group. From each brain slice, two to three dendrites from each hippocampal subregion were imaged (number of dendrites in DG: WT, 46; TS, 42; WT EE-EGCG, 48; TS EE-EGCG, 42; number of dendrites in CA1: WT, 38; TS, 32; WT EE-EGCG, 31; TS EE-EGCG, 39).
Immunohistochemical labeling of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic vesicle proteins
Synaptic modifications due to genotype and treatment were addressed by performing immunohistochemical labeling for vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (VGLUT1) and vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT). Animals were exposed to CO2 and afterward perfused intracardially with 0.01 m PBS, pH 7.5, followed by 4% PFA. Brains were removed, kept at 4°C in 4% PFA overnight, and transferred to a solution of 30% sucrose in PBS for 2 days. Series of coronal sections (40 μm) were obtained using a vibratome (VT1000S; Leica Microsystems) and stored at –20°C in a cryoprotection solution (30% ethylene glycol, 30% glycerol, 40% PBS). Free-floating brain sections were permeabilized with 0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS for 30 min at RT and incubated for 20 min with Glycine (50 mm) in PBS/0.3% Triton X-100. Slices were washed for 15 min with PBS/0.3% Triton X-100 and blocked with 5% normal goat serum (NGS)/PBS/-0.3% Triton X-100 for 1 h at RT. Sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with the primary antibodies—mouse anti-VGLUT1 monoclonal antibody (1:150; cat. no. 135-511, Synaptic Systems, Goettingen, Germany; RRID: AB_887879) and guinea pig anti-VGAT polyclonal antibody (1:200; cytoplasmic domain; Synaptic Systems; cat. no. 131004, RRID:AB_887873)—in 0.1% Triton X-100/2.5% NGS in PBS. Slices were washed with PBS/0.3% Triton X-100 and incubated with secondary antibodies—Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-mouse (cat. no. A11001, RRID:AB_2534069) and Alexa Fluor 555 anti–guinea pig (cat. no. A-21435, RRID:AB_2535856; 1:1000—in 0.1% Triton X-100/2.5% NGS in PBS for 2 h at RT, protected from light. Finally, sections were washed with PBS/0.3% Triton X-100, nuclei were stained with Hoechst (1:1000) in PBS for 10 min, and tissues were mounted on glass slides with Mowiol reagent.
Images were acquired from DG and CA1 hippocampal regions using a confocal microscope with a 63× objective (TCS SP5, Leica Microsystems) and LAS AF software. For each region, all pictures were captured with identical confocal settings for laser power, gain, and offset levels. Images were imported into ImageJ, converted into binary data, and thresholded to achieve maximum number of individual puncta without causing puncta fusion. The same threshold was applied to all the images to outline puncta number, size, and percentage of area occupied by puncta, using the “analyze particle” plugin. For this experiment, we used two brain slices of the dorsal hippocampus (bregma: 1.82–1.94, Paxinos and Franklin, 2012) of four mice per experimental group. From each brain slice, four to five images were acquired per hippocampal subregion.
Statistical analysis
Morris water maze
Single-variate analysis
Differences among experimental groups over time were tested using a single-variate analysis for selected learning-related parameters (latency to reach the platform, Gallagher index, and percentage of time spent in the periphery) using one-way repeated-measures ANOVA. To avoid the ceiling effect of mice unable to solve the task, the variable latency was considered right-censored when reaching the maximum allowed time (60 s; Vock et al., 2012) using a Tobit model implemented in the censReg package from R Foundation for Statistical Computing (RRID:SCR_001905), version 3.2.1 (Henningsen, 2011). Multiple comparisons for parametric models were used to address post hoc comparisons using the multtest R package and the glht function (Hand and Taylor, 1987; Dickhaus, 2012). To control the FDR caused by multiple post hoc comparisons, the Benjamini–Hochberg method was used (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).
Principal component analysis
We used principal component analysis (PCA) to identify the linear combinations of the original variables (latency to reach the platform, percentage of time spent in target quadrant, percentage of time spent in the periphery, Whishaw index, Gallagher index, distance traveled, and speed) that explain the maximum amount of experimental variability. More precisely, our application of the method aims primarily at two kinds of variability: variation among experimental groups for a given learning session and variation of a given group across learning sessions. For this, we implemented the same procedure as described in Catuara-Solarz et al. (2015). Briefly, a PCA for the acquisition sessions was performed on 20 observations of seven variables. Here, the observations correspond to the four experimental groups on the five learning sessions, and the variables are the experimental parameters described above. The resulting 20 × 7 data matrix contains the medians of the measured variables for each group during each session (the four trials per mouse of each learning session were averaged). As variables are measured in different units, they were scaled to unit variance to enable a combined analysis. The result of the described PCA approximates a decomposition of what is commonly called between-group variance. A third kind of variability coming from individuals within a group for a given session can also be quantified. For this, 195 supplementary points that correspond to the 39 individuals appearing five times each were projected. The R package FactoMineR was used (Lê et al., 2008). Separately, a similar PCA was done for the three reversal sessions.
Our analysis can be considered a discriminant analysis in the sense that the PCA is performed for groups and individuals are projected only after the PCA is performed. However, we use group medians instead of the commonly applied group means weighted by group sizes, leading to two differences: first, between-group variance is defined as variance between group medians; second, the overall barycenter no longer coincides with the group barycenter (our origin), and thus the total variance obtained by summing squared distances of all individuals from the origin as applied in Catuara-Solarz et al. (2015) overestimates the true variance by a small amount (i.e. by the squared distance of the barycenter from the origin). To comply with the original definition (Greenacre, 2010, chapter 11) of between-group variance when decomposing total variance, the overall barycenter instead of the origin can be used as the reference point, and weighted group means obtained from the supplementary points can be used to calculate the between-group variance instead. We found that the difference between the two approaches is on the order of a few percentage points only.
To validate the stability of the PCA, we used a jackknifing procedure: each individual is subtracted from the analysis, the resulting modified group median is used to perform a new PCA, and the angle between the new first principal component (PC1) and PC2 with respect to the original principal axes is computed. The procedure showed that both axes remain very stable, with PC1 attaining maximum angles around 1 degree, suggesting a minor influence of the small number of experimental groups on the outcome of the analysis (data not shown).
Density plots were obtained using the statdensity_2d function from the ggplot2 R package (Wickham, 2009; RRID:SCR_014601) with the parameters: n = 100, h = 4, and bins = 6. to assess statistical significance of group separation, we randomly reassigned individuals to experimental groups to perform a permutation test (Sham and Purcell, 2014) in which original numbers of individuals in each group were kept. For this, learning differences were evaluated using a t statistic involving PC1 pairwise group comparisons based on supplementary points. All pairwise comparisons were determined at each permutation. The number of randomized PCAs was 10,000. Finally, to evaluate the change in within-group variances before and after learning, we averaged squared distances of a group’s supplementary points from their barycenter using coordinates from all seven principal axes.
Novel object recognition
Differences in the discrimination index among experimental groups were tested using one-way ANOVA. Tukey multiple comparisons for parametric models were used to address post hoc comparisons using the multtest R package and the glht function (Hand and Taylor, 1987; Dickhaus, 2012). To control the FDR caused by multiple post hoc comparisons, the Benjamini–Hochberg method was used (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).
Dendritic spine density and excitatory (VGLUT1) and inhibitory (VGAT) synaptic puncta
For analysis of the differences among the experimental groups in dendritic spine densities and number, size, and percentage of area occupied by synaptic puncta of VGLUT1 and VGAT, we used linear mixed models, which included experimental group as a factor and mouse as a random effect to account for the repeated measures per mouse. The F test was used to evaluate the global hypothesis that there was no association between the response variables and the groups. Whenever this hypothesis was rejected, post hoc tests for the following contrasts of interest were applied: WT versus TS; TS versus TS EE-EGCG, and WT versus WT-EE-EGCG. The analyses were performed using R packages nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2016) and multcomp (Hothorn et al., 2008) for the fit of the linear mixed models and the multiple tests, respectively. Statistical significance was set at 0.05. The significance levels for the three contrasts of interest were adjusted to guarantee a family-wise error rate of 0.05.
Results
Effects of EE-EGCG treatment on corticohippocampal-dependent learning and memory impairment in Ts65Dn mice
To evaluate the effect of EE-EGCG treatment, we compared the behavioral performance of WT and Ts65Dn mice treated with EE-EGCG with their untreated counterparts in the MWM. During the acquisition sessions, there were statistical differences among all groups in escape latency, distance to the target [as shown by both the Gallagher index (mean distance to the platform) and the Gallagher (accumulated) distance to the platform], and thigmotactic behavior, (percentage of time spent close to the periphery) the percentage of time spent close to the periphery of the pool (Table 1).
We detected learning defects in Ts65Dn mice compared with WT (Fig. 1A) as shown by the higher escape latency across sessions (Fig. 1B; Table 1), increased Gallagher distance and index (Fig. 1A, C; Table 1), and typical increase in thigmotactic behavior (Fig. 1A, D; Table 1).
EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice showed improved learning performance during the acquisition sessions (Fig. 1A). In comparison to untreated Ts65Dn mice, EE-EGCG–treated TS mice presented significantly reduced escape latency (Fig. 1B; Table 1) and Gallagher distance and index (Fig. 1A, C; Table 1) but no statistical differences in thigmotactic behavior (Fig. 1A, D; Table 1). Conversely, EE-EGCG–treated WT mice did not show differences compared with untreated WT mice.
There were differences in swimming speed among the groups; specifically, Ts65Dn mice had lower swimming speeds than WT mice (data not shown; Table 1). However, EE-EGCG did not promote significant changes in swimming speed in Ts65Dn or WT with respect to the untreated condition (data not shown; Table 1). This suggests that the learning differences in EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice are not mediated by changes in swimming speed.
To assess reference memory, a probe trial (removal session) was performed 24 h after the final acquisition day. In this session, there were no differences among groups in the percentage of time spent in the target quadrant, probably because of the high variability of the data (data not shown). However, the latency to the first entry to the platform area and the Gallagher distance, which is a more precise performance measure (Maei et al., 2009), showed significant differences among experimental groups (Fig. 1A, E, F; Table 1). Post hoc analysis demonstrated higher values of Ts65Dn mice in the Gallagher distance and the latency to the first entry compared with WT mice (Fig. 1A, E, F; Table 1), indicating poorer reference memory. The mean difference in these parameters between EE-EGCG–treated and untreated Ts65Dn mice was fairly large; however, it did not reach statistical significance, possibly because of high variability of the data (Fig. 1A, E, F; Table 1).
In the reversal sessions, we detected statistical differences among the experimental groups in escape latency, Gallagher distance and index, and thigmotaxis (Table 1). Whereas untreated WT mice efficiently shifted their search to the new platform position (Fig. 1A), Ts65Dn mice had increased escape latency (Fig. 1G; Table 1), increased Gallagher distance and index (Fig. 1A, H; Table 1), and increased thigmotaxis across the three reversal learning sessions (Fig. 1A, I; Table 1), compared with WT mice. During reversal, there was no significant reduction in thigmotaxis, suggesting that this variable was not associated with reversal learning.
EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice showed a fairly large (although not statistically significant) reduction in the latency to reach the new platform position compared with untreated Ts65Dn mice (Fig. 1A, G; Table 1). There were no significant differences between EE-EGCG–treated and untreated Ts65Dn mice in Gallagher distance or index (Fig. 1A, H; Table 1) or thigmotaxis (Fig. 1A, I; Table 1). EE-EGCG–treated and untreated WT mice showed no significant differences in the latency to reach the new platform position, the Gallagher distance or index, or thigmotaxis. During the reversal sessions, there were no statistical differences in swimming speed among the experimental groups (not shown; Table 1).
Multidimensional analysis of learning using PCA
PCA enabled placement of the experimental groups in a low-dimensional coordinate system built from variables taken during the MWM experiment. A group’s progression along the acquisition sessions became apparent in its resulting 5-day trajectory (Fig. 2A). We obtained a PC1 that explained 84% of the between-group variance and was mainly composed of learning-related variables (i.e., escape latency, Gallagher index, percentage of time spent in periphery, Whishaw index, distance traveled, percentage of time spent in target quadrant; Figs. 2B, C). Swimming speed also contributed to PC1, but to a lesser extent. Efficient learning behaviors (short distances to target, low escape latencies, high percentages of time in the target quadrant, etc.) correspond to large values of PC1 (Fig. 2B), and thus PC1 can be interpreted as a quantification of learning. In contrast, PC2 explained 11% of between-group variance and was mainly composed of swimming speed (Figs. 2B, D). This component of speed is unrelated to learning, since PC2 is orthogonal to the learning-related PC1. It thus seems to reflect motor performance rather than determination to reach the target quickly. Swimming speed is thus decomposed in a learning-dependent component (PC1) and a learning-independent component (PC2). Learning-related variables contributed to a much lesser extent to PC2.
Because a group trajectory represents a group’s overall learning through its progression along PC1, the trajectory representation allows for effective comparisons between group performances. Untreated Ts65Dn mice showed a trajectory reaching a maximum value of PC1 that corresponds to initial PC1 values of the untreated WT trajectory, revealing poor learning. On the other hand, the EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn trajectory attained more advanced maximum values of PC1.
Additionally, we determined individual variation within the groups by mapping the position of each individual on each acquisition day on the PCA plot. As shown in the density plots of sessions 1 and 5 in Fig. 2E, there is substantial individual variation across learning sessions in all the experimental groups. In fact, overall group differences explain less than half of the total variance. Within-group variance, however, differs between experimental groups. Whereas WT mice generally show higher variability than Ts65Dn mice, the treatment roughly doubles variance for both genotypes in the first learning session. Interestingly, learning increases variability in all groups. This effect is stronger for untreated groups, so treated and untreated groups show similar variability in the final learning session. We summarize within-group variances of the first and last acquisition sessions in Table 2.
To assess the statistical significance of differences in learning, we performed a permutation test involving a t statistic based on PC1. Untreated Ts65Dn mice had significantly lower PC1 values in comparison to WT mice in the first learning session (Fig. 2F; Table 3). EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice had higher PC1 than untreated Ts65Dn mice (Fig. 2F; Table 3); at this stage, the difference could be associated with procedural learning and was not significantly different from untreated WT (Fig. 2F; Table 3). At the end of the learning period (session 5), untreated Ts65Dn mice still had significantly lower PC1 values in comparison to WT mice (Fig. 2F; Table 3). EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice exhibited higher PC1 than untreated Ts65Dn mice, although they showed significantly lower PC1 values than untreated WT mice (Fig. 2F; Table 3). On the other hand, EE-EGCG treatment did not significantly change learning outcomes of WT mice in either the first or the last session.
Similarly, group trajectories comprising three time points each were obtained for the reversal sessions (Fig. 3A). Here, PC1 explained 84% of the between-group variance and, as in the acquisition sessions, was dominated by learning-related variables (Fig. 3B, C). PC2, which explained 12% of the between-group variance, showed again a strong contribution of swimming speed. The main contribution here, however, turned out to be from thigmotaxis (Fig. 3B, D). Interestingly, these two variables fall on a line separating the groups along an efficiency gradient from strong thigmotaxis and low speed to no thigmotaxis and high speed in the order TS, TS-EE-EGCG, WT, and WT-EE-EGCG (Fig. 3A, B). Again, there was an increased within-group variability associated with the learning process in all groups (Fig. 3E; Table 2).
According to the permutation tests, untreated Ts65Dn mice showed significantly lower PC1 than untreated WT mice (Fig. 3F; Table 2). EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice had significantly higher PC1 than untreated Ts65Dn mice, although they still showed significantly lower PC1 than WT mice (Fig. 3F; Table 2). In WT mice, EE-EGCG treatment did not modify cognitive flexibility outcomes in the first or the last session.
Effects of EE-EGCG treatment on recognition deficits in Ts65Dn mice
To assess the impact of the treatment on a less stressful learning task, we conducted a novel object recognition test. The performance of this test depends on the functionality of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortices and the hippocampus (Brown and Aggleton, 2001).
In this test, Ts65Dn mice showed no deficit in their DI in comparison to WT mice, although a slight tendency to impairment was detected (p = 0.08; Fig. 4; Table 4). EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice had an increase in their DI with respect to their untreated counterparts (Fig. 4; Table 4) and scored at similar levels to WT mice (Fig. 4; Table 4). Conversely, EE-EGCG–treated WT mice showed a poorer performance than untreated WT mice (Fig. 4; Table 4).
Effects of EE-EGCG treatment on dendritic spine density in Ts65Dn hippocampus
Ts65Dn mice showed a significant reduction of dendritic spine density in the CA1 (Fig. 5A; Table 5) and DG (Fig. 5B; Table 5) hippocampal subregions. EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice did not show statistically significant differences in DG dendritic spine density compared with untreated Ts65Dn or WT mice (Fig. 5B). Conversely, EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice had increased dendritic spine density in CA1 in comparison to untreated Ts65Dn mice (Fig. 5A; Table 5), and EE-EGCG–treated WT mice showed reduced CA1 dendritic spine density in comparison to untreated WT mice (Fig. 5A; Table 5).
Effects of EE-EGCG treatment on hippocampal excitatory and inhibitory synaptic puncta in Ts65Dn mice
In DG, Ts65Dn mice showed increased VGLUT1 puncta density (data not shown; Table 6) of reduced size (data not shown; Table 6), and no differences in the number or size of VGAT puncta compared with WT mice. This resulted in an increased VGLUT1/VGAT density ratio (Fig. 6A; Table 6). Because the increase in the number of VGLUT1 puncta was compensated by a reduction in size, Ts65Dn mice showed no difference in the VGLUT1/VGAT percentage of area occupied compared with WT mice (Fig. 6B; Table 6).
In CA1, Ts65Dn mice also showed significantly increased density of VGLUT1 puncta of reduced size compared with WT mice (data not shown; Table 6). As in DG, Ts65Dn mice presented no differences in VGAT density puncta in CA1, but in this region, VGAT puncta were enlarged (data not shown; Table 6). This resulted in an increased ratio of VGLUT1/VGAT puncta density (Fig. 6C; Table 6), and a reduced VGLUT1/VGAT percentage of area occupied (Fig. 6D; Table 6).
Compared with untreated conditions, EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice exhibited a significant reduction in the density of VGLUT1 puncta in DG (data not shown; Table 6) but not in CA1. VGLUT1 puncta were significantly enlarged in DG (data not shown; Table 6) but not in CA1. There were no significant differences in VGAT puncta number or size between EE-EGCG–treated and untreated Ts65Dn mice in DG or CA1. As a result, EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice showed a reduction in the ratio of VGLUT1/VGAT density in both DG (Fig. 6A; Table 6) and CA1 (Fig. 6C; Table 6), leading to values that were similar to those of WT mice. On the other hand, EE-EGCG–treated WT mice also showed a trend toward an enlargement of VGAT puncta size in CA1 (data not shown; Table 6), leading to a decreased VGLUT1/VGAT percentage of area occupied (Fig. 6D; Table 6) in comparison to untreated WT mice.
Discussion
In the present study, combined EE-EGCG treatment significantly increased spine density in CA1, normalized excitatory and inhibitory synaptic markers in CA1 and DG, and improved performance in a corticohippocampal-dependent learning task in young Ts65Dn mice.
In line with previous studies, we detected poor learning strategies and hippocampal-dependent learning and memory performance in the MWM in young Ts65Dn mice (Escorihuela et al., 1995; Reeves et al., 1995). In Ts65Dn mice, but not WT mice, EE-EGCG treatment improved performance in the MWM, reducing escape latency and Gallagher index and distance during the learning sessions. PCA confirmed that untreated Ts65Dn showed inefficient learning progress over acquisition sessions, reaching maximum values of PC1, a global learning variable, similar to initial WT values. EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice improved on global learning measures. They reached more advanced maximum PC1 values than untreated Ts65Dn mice, suggesting a modification in learning-related behavior as previously reported in middle-age Ts65Dn mice (Catuara-Solarz et al., 2015).
Ts65Dn mice also exhibited poor reference memory, as indicated by a significantly increased Gallagher index and distance and latency to the first entry to the platform area in the probe trial. However, no genotype effects were detected in other variables, such as time spent in the target quadrant or latency to first entry to target area, probably because of the high variability of the data. Additionally, Ts65Dn mice presented a deficit in cognitive flexibility, as shown by the inefficient performance during the reversal sessions (executive function).
Single-variate analysis of different parameters between EE-EGCG–treated and untreated Ts65Dn mice did not reach statistical significance at reference memory and reversal sessions. However, a significant enhancement in cognitive flexibility was shown by multivariate analysis of the reversal sessions. In middle-age Ts65Dn mice, EE-EGCG treatment improved learning and reference memory, but not cognitive flexibility (Catuara-Solarz et al., 2015). This suggests that EE-EGCG treatment effects depend on age and cognitive domain, possibly because of differential effects on different underlying brain regions and functions at different ages. EE-EGCG–treated WT mice did not show significant differences, possibly because of a ceiling effect.
MWM is a learning paradigm based on the stressful and aversive stimuli of the water pool, which triggers increases in plasma corticosterone, leading to a motivational state in the mice to learn the spatial configuration of the cues to escape (Harrison et al., 2009). Previous studies have shown that EGCG exerts an anxiolytic effect on different behavioral anxiety tests such as the forced swimming test, elevated plus maze, passive avoidance test, and tail suspension test (Dias et al., 2012). It could thus be speculated that the potential anxiolytic effects of EGCG would contribute to the learning improvement we found in the MWM. However, a number of facts suggest that the learning improvements found in treated Ts65Dn mice are not mainly contributed by the anxiolytic effect of EGCG. Ts65Dn mice have reduced levels of anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze (Coussons-Read, 1996; Demas, 1996; Escorihuela, 1998; Shichiri, 2011), suggesting that the learning deficits shown by Ts65Dn in the MWM are not associated with anxiety. Thus a potential anxiolytic effect of EGCG would not eventually lead to significant learning improvement. Additionally, in the case of a potential MWM improvement associated with the anxiolytic effect of EGCG, we should be able to observe it in the control group that is subjected to the same anxiogenic scenario. However, in our study, the WT group did not benefit from the combined EE-EGCG treatment.
Even so, we also addressed the effects of the combined EE-EGCG treatment in a less stressful learning test, the novel object recognition test. The performance of this test depends on the functionality of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortices and the hippocampus (Brown and Aggleton, 2001; Brown et al., 2010). In this test, trisomic mice presented no significant deficit in their DI in comparison to WT mice, similar to some previous reports (Hyde and Crnic, 2002), although a slight tendency to impairment was detected that is in line with data from Fernandez et al. (2007). EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice presented an improvement in DI with respect to untreated counterparts and scored at similar levels to WT mice. On the other hand, EE-EGCG–treated WT mice showed a poorer performance than untreated WT mice, suggesting a possible deleterious effect of EGCG.
Along with learning improvement, EE-EGCG–treated mice showed significant neuromorphologic changes in the hippocampus. Consistent with previous reports in DS (Ferrer and Gullotta, 1990) and Ts65Dn mice (Belichenko et al., 2004), we observed a reduction in dendritic spine density in outer molecular layer dendrites from granule cells of the DG, and in apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons of CA1 in Ts65Dn mice. Combined treatment with EE-EGCG partially rescued the dendritic spine density deficit in CA1, but not in the DG of Ts65Dn mice. A reduction of Dyrk1A kinase activity (Bain et al., 2003; Golabek et al., 2011; Pons-Espinal et al., 2013), but also other signaling pathways that are modified by both EE and EGCG, such as increased CREB and Akt phosphorylation (Jia et al., 2013; Ramírez-Rodríguez et al., 2014; Ortiz-López et al., 2016) or increases in BDNF expression (Young et al., 1999; Li et al., 2009a, 2009b), could contribute to these neuroplasticity changes.
We also explored the effects of EE-EGCG treatment on E/I balance, using excitatory (VGLUT1) and inhibitory (VGAT) synaptic vesicle markers. In Ts65Dn DG, VGLUT1 puncta were more abundant but smaller, with no changes in the percentage of area occupied, whereas VGAT puncta showed no differences compared with WT littermates. In CA1, Ts65Dn mice showed the same phenotype, VGLUT1 puncta being more abundant and smaller; however, in this region, VGAT puncta were enlarged, leading to an increased VGLUT1/VGAT ratio but a reduction of VGLUT1/VGAT percentage of area occupied. That in both DG and CA1 VGLUT1 puncta were more abundant and smaller in Ts65Dn could affect the probability or efficiency in neurotransmitter release (Harris and Sultan, 1995; Bozdagi et al., 2000; Antonova et al., 2001; Bamji et al., 2006; Bourne et al., 2013) and could also be related to the previously reported enhanced GABAA and GABAB evoked inhibitory postsynaptic currents in DG of 3- to 4-month-old male mice (Kleschevnikov et al., 2012) and increased GABA release in the hippocampus of male and female adult mice (Begenisic et al., 2011). Consistent with our results, previous work showed no changes in density of VGAT puncta nor density of inhibitory synapses using electron microscopy in the DG of 3-month-old male mice, although apposition length of symmetric (inhibitory) synapses was larger (Belichenko et al., 2009). Additionally, Kleschevnikov et al. (2012) found no differences in GAD67 optical density in DG, with only a trend toward reduction in GAD67 in the outer molecular layer of 3- to 4-month-old male mice. A recent study using Western blot showed a reduction in the hippocampal expression of VGLUT1 and no statistical difference in VGAT (Souchet et al., 2015). Conversely, others have shown an increase in the percentage of area occupied by VGAT puncta and VGAT/gephyrin puncta in DG of 4.5- to 5.5-month-old male mice (Martinez-Cue et al., 2013). Differences in experimental methods, hippocampal subregions, age, or sex could account for these divergent results. On the other hand, very little is known about how E/I is affected across different brain regions or ages in Ts65Dn mice. Possibly, the E/I imbalance could arise from alterations in excitation, inhibition, or both and may be continuously changing as a result of synaptic plasticity, leading to region-specific dysfunction (Bartley et al., 2015).
Interestingly, EE-EGCG–treated Ts65Dn mice showed normal density and size of VGLUT1 puncta, and as a consequence, the balance of excitatory and inhibitory puncta in DG and in CA1 was also in the normal ranges. That the treatment restores the density and size of VGLUT1 puncta is consistent with the treatment effect on the density of dendritic spines. Excitatory synapses comprise a presynaptic terminal with abundant synaptic vesicles containing glutamate, in association with dendritic spine heads acting as a postsynaptic element. Thus, our results suggest that combined treatment with EE-EGCG may increase excitatory synaptic connections.
These results are also consistent with the outcome of a recent phase II clinical trial with DS individuals in which a therapy combining cognitive training and EGCG normalized neuronal network functionality, as measured by functional MRI, and cortical excitability by transcranial magnetic stimulation (de la Torre et al., 2016).
Conversely, in WT mice EE-EGCG treatment reduced spine density in CA1, but not in the DG, and led to E/I imbalance in CA1, without significant changes in the DG. This deleterious effect could be explained by an overinhibition of Dyrk1A kinase activity in WT conditions, since it has been shown that DYRK1A haploinsufficiency is associated with neuroanatomical and neuroarchitectural defects in flies, mice, and humans. Indeed, mutant flies with reduced Dyrk1A expression present reductions in the volumes of the adult optic lobes and central brain hemispheres (Tejedor et al., 1995). Brains from Dyrk1A heterozygous mice are ∼30% smaller and have reduced size and weight in specific brain regions, along with reduced neuronal density in the superior colliculus and increased neuronal numbers in brain regions such as somatosensory and motor cortices (Fotaki et al., 2002), with significantly smaller and less complex basal dendritic arbors and reduced dendritic spine densities (Benavides-Piccione et al., 2005). In the hippocampus, Dyrk1A heterozygous mice show a significant reduction in hippocampal thickness, accompanied by decreases in cell number in CA1, CA2, CA3, and DG (Arqué et al., 2008). Humans with de novo heterozygous variants of DYRK1A also have congenital microcephaly, structural brain abnormalities, and intellectual disability (Møller et al., 2008; Ji et al., 2015).
Taken together, our results suggest that combined treatment with EE and EGCG is a potent cognitive enhancing intervention for DS. We demonstrated that EE-EGCG treatment–derived cognitive improvements are associated with neuromodulatory effects at the hippocampus that normalize defects in dendritic spine density and E/I synaptic puncta ratio. Overall results suggest that combined EE-EGCG treatment has the capacity to simultaneously target several abnormal processes underlying intellectual disability in DS which would be optimal for a disease-modifying intervention in this clinical population.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments: We gratefully thank the students Carla Cuní-Lopez, Andrea Pérez, and María Alemany for their contribution in data acquisition and technical assistance.
Footnotes
Authors report no conflict of interest.
Authors Contribution: S.C.S. designed research, performed research, analyzed data, wrote the paper; J.E.-C., I.E., K.L., J.R.G. analyzed data; C.N. supervised the PCA analysis; M.D. provided financial support, designed research, wrote the paper.
This research was supported by DIUE of Generalitat de Catalunya (Grups consolidats SGR 2014/1125); Fondation Jérôme Lejeune (Paris, France); Spanish Ministry of Economy and Finance (MINECO) grants SAF2013-49129-C2-1-R, BFU2011-28575, and MTM2015-64465-C2-1-R CDTI (“Smartfoods”); the CIBER of Rare Diseases (ISCIII initiative); and EU-Era Net Neuron PCIN-2013-060. The CRG has received support as a Severo Ochoa Center of Excellence SEV- 2012-O208. S.C.-S. received a FPI doctoral Fellowship from the MINECO (SAF2010-16427).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.