Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleNew Research, Cognition and Behavior

Neural Correlates of the Time Marker for the Perception of Event Timing

Kaoru Amano, Liang Qi, Yoshikazu Terada and Shin’ya Nishida
eNeuro 31 August 2016, 3 (4) ENEURO.0144-16.2016; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0144-16.2016
Kaoru Amano
1Center for Information and Neural Networks (CiNet), National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Suita City 565-0871, Osaka, Japan
2Precursory Research for Embryonic Science and Technology (PRESTO), Japan Science and Technology Agency, Saitama 332-0012, Japan
3Graduate School of Frontier Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Kashiwa 277-8561, Chiba, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kaoru Amano
Liang Qi
3Graduate School of Frontier Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Kashiwa 277-8561, Chiba, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yoshikazu Terada
1Center for Information and Neural Networks (CiNet), National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Suita City 565-0871, Osaka, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shin’ya Nishida
4NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation, Atsugi 243-0198, Kanagawa, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure1
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Dissociation between two measures of timing perception. A, Stimulus configuration of the simple RT and SJ tasks. In both tasks, we used two types of temporal changes of the motion coherence: step (30%, 40%, and 90%) and ramp (80, 120, and 200%/s; for details, see Materials and Methods). B, Example data from the simultaneity judgment task with three levels of ramp stimuli. The horizontal axis shows the SOA between the coherent motion onset and a beep, while the vertical axis shows the percentage of simultaneous responses. The PSS was defined as the weighted average of SOA values based on the percentage of simultaneous responses. C, Averaged RTs for step and ramp stimuli. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. D, Averaged PSS for step and ramp stimuli. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. Both RT and PSS decrease with increasing stimulus amplitude, but the decrease is much larger for RT than for PSS. E, Comparison between the RT and PSS for both the step and ramp stimuli. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. The steeper slope for the ramp stimuli suggests that the amplitude of ramp stimuli has a larger effect on the PSS than the step stimuli. Please note that x- and y-axes are scaled differently. F, Correlation between the variance of RT across stimuli and that of PSS. Each dot represents the data of individual participants. A significant correlation supports the result that the same integrated signal could account for both RT and PSS variations (Fig. 3).

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    An example of MEG responses to step and ramp motion onsets and the extracted visual response time course. A, MEG response time courses averaged across trials for a typical participant. Data from 10 sensors that showed the greatest response for the 90% step stimulus at the peak latency are overlaid, and the gray region in each panel indicates the motion coherence time course. For the step stimuli, the MEG responses peaked at ∼230 ms, and the amplitude increased with motion coherence. For the ramp stimuli, the response peak was less clear and the peak latency was much longer. B, Normalized spatial pattern (topographic map) of the visual and auditory responses used in the SSP analysis, for the participant shown in A. The left image shows the peak MEG response evoked by the strongest visual stimulus (90% step), while the right image shows the largest peak MEG response averaged with respect to the timing of auditory stimuli. The auditory peak was either M100 or M200 (it was M100 for eight participants, and was M200 for three participants, including the participant shown here). C, The time course of visual responses for the 90% step stimulus, extracted by the SSP method (Tesche et al., 1995). The spatial pattern at each latency was decomposed into visual and auditory responses by calculating the best weight of the spatial pattern of visual and auditory responses (B), and the absolute value of the weight was defined as the response amplitude.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Neural correlates of RT and PSS. A, B, Schematic of the peak/level detector models (A) and the integrator model (B). C, The time course of visual responses for a typical participant. Solid and dotted lines represent the optimized threshold of the level detector model for RT and PSS, respectively (they were almost the same for this participant). Gray squares represent the latencies predicted by the peak detector model (common across the detection latencies for RT and time-marker latencies for PSS). Diamonds represent the latencies predicted by the level detector model (open and filled diamonds represent detection latencies for RT and time-marker latencies for PSS, respectively). D, The time course of integrated visual responses (τ = ∞) for the same participant as shown in A. Solid and dotted lines represent the optimized threshold of the integrator model for RT and PSS, respectively. Open and filled circles represent the detection latencies and time-marker latencies, respectively. The threshold for PSS was lower than that for RT. E, F, Relationship between RT and detection latency (E), or between PSS and time-marker latency (F), for the participant shown in A and B. Since we are interested in the model that quantitatively accounts for the RT or PSS variations, the model performance was evaluated by the MSE from the best-fitted line of unit slope, which takes into account not only poorly correlated data with a slope of 1 but also highly correlated data with a slope higher or lower than 1. G, Comparison of the threshold for the integrator model (τ = ∞) that best accounts for RT and PSS variations. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. The threshold was significantly lower for PSS than for RT, suggesting that the stimulus content information, which is necessary for manual response, was not available at the timing of the time marker, and the event timing is postdictively perceived (Nishida and Johnston, 2002).

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Comparison of models that account for the changes in RT and PSS using the MEG response. A, B, The MSE from the best-fitted line of unit slope for the scatter plot between RT/PSS (x-axis) and the latency predicted by the model (y-axis), as shown in Figure 3, E and F . The MSE was summed across participants, and the error bars indicate SDs across 1000 bootstrap samples. C, D, While MSE is a biased estimator of the prediction error, bias-corrected MSE accounts for the difference in both susceptibility to noise and the number of parameters across models. The bias-corrected MSE was summed across participants, and the error bars indicate SDs across 1000 bootstrap samples. A and C, and B and D show the results for the RT and PSS data, respectively. The SH test (Shimodaira, 1998) performed on the bias-corrected MSE showed that the peak and level detector models were significantly worse than the best model for both RT and PSS (Table 1).

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Effect of an auditory stimulus on the model latencies. A, B, Detection latency for RT (A) and time-marker latency for PSS (B) based on the full integrator model (τ = ∞), calculated separately for shorter and longer SOA trials. Similar latencies between shorter and longer SOA trials indicate that the auditory response was reasonably removed by the SSP analysis to extract the visual response time course, and had a negligible effect on the latencies predicted by the integrator model. C, Comparison of the threshold for the integrator model (τ = ∞) that best accounts for RT and PSS variations, calculated separately for shorter and longer SOA trials. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. The threshold was significantly lower for PSS than for RT for both shorter and longer SOA trials (p = 0.03 and p = 0.001, respectively), replicating the analysis that used all of the trials (Fig. 3G ).

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Subjective delay of motor response. A, Stimulus configuration of the second experiment that measured the subjective delay of simple reactions. B, Stimulus detectability, expressed in terms of d', as a function of motion coherence. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. C, Representative histograms of RT and subjective delay of motor response for each motion coherence level. D, The RT, subjective delay of RT, and PSS, as a function of motion coherence. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. E, The slope of the fitted line for the plot between coherence level and RT/subjective delay for each individual participant. The subjective delay slope, on average, was shallower than the RT slope but was significantly <0 (p < 0.001). In addition, the RT and subjective delay slopes were similar for 4 of 11 participants (red lines).

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Comparison of the percentage correct between motion direction judgments and simultaneity judgments. A, Stimulus configuration of the third experiment that measured the threshold for motion direction judgments and simultaneity judgments. B, The percentage correct for motion direction judgments and simultaneity judgments. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. C, The percentage correct for simultaneity judgments analyzed separately for the correct and incorrect trials for motion direction judgments. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. The percentage correct for simultaneity judgments for direction-incorrect trials is at the level of chance. D, Comparison of d' for motion direction judgments, simultaneity judgments (third experiment), and a simple RT (second experiment) task. Error bars indicate SEs across participants. RT task d' values were lower than motion direction judgment task d' values; however, they were close to, but never lower than, simultaneity judgment task d' values.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1:

    p Values for the SH test on the bias-corrected MSE summed across participants

    PeakLevelLeaky (τ = 100 ms)Leaky (τ = 500 ms)Leaky (τ = 1000 ms)Full (τ = ∞)
    RT0.0010.0070.0520.3040.4880.906
    PSS00.0160.1240.3380.5090.889
    • The null hypothesis is that the corresponding model is not different from the best model, while the alternative hypothesis is that the corresponding model is worse than the best model. Bold type indicates the models within a confidence set of models.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 3 (4)
eNeuro
Vol. 3, Issue 4
July/August 2016
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Neural Correlates of the Time Marker for the Perception of Event Timing
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Neural Correlates of the Time Marker for the Perception of Event Timing
Kaoru Amano, Liang Qi, Yoshikazu Terada, Shin’ya Nishida
eNeuro 31 August 2016, 3 (4) ENEURO.0144-16.2016; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0144-16.2016

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Neural Correlates of the Time Marker for the Perception of Event Timing
Kaoru Amano, Liang Qi, Yoshikazu Terada, Shin’ya Nishida
eNeuro 31 August 2016, 3 (4) ENEURO.0144-16.2016; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0144-16.2016
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Visual Abstract
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • MEG
  • motion
  • synchrony
  • time marker
  • timing perception

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

New Research

  • Hyperconnectivity of two separate long-range cholinergic systems contributes to the reorganization of the brain functional connectivity during nicotine withdrawal in male mice
  • Sepsis-induced changes in spectral segregation and kinetics of hippocampal oscillatory states in rats
  • Target-distractor competition modulates saccade trajectories in space and object-space
Show more New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Neuronal representation of a working memory-based decision strategy in the motor and prefrontal cortico-basal ganglia loops
  • Strawberry additive increases nicotine vapor sampling and systemic exposure but does not enhance Pavlovian-based nicotine reward in mice
  • Attention Without Constraint: Alpha Lateralization in Uncued Willed Attention
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.