Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleNew Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

Effects of Fluoxetine and Visual Experience on Glutamatergic and GABAergic Synaptic Proteins in Adult Rat Visual Cortex

Simon Beshara, Brett R. Beston, Joshua G. A. Pinto and Kathryn M. Murphy
eNeuro 24 December 2015, 2 (6) ENEURO.0126-15.2015; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0126-15.2015
Simon Beshara
1McMaster Integrative Neuroscience Discovery and Study (MiNDS) Program, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Brett R. Beston
1McMaster Integrative Neuroscience Discovery and Study (MiNDS) Program, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
2Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behavior, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
3Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, L5L 1C6, ON
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joshua G. A. Pinto
1McMaster Integrative Neuroscience Discovery and Study (MiNDS) Program, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
4Health Care Investment Banking, Credit Suisse AG, San Francisco, CA 94108
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kathryn M. Murphy
1McMaster Integrative Neuroscience Discovery and Study (MiNDS) Program, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
2Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behavior, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Fluoxetine has emerged as a novel treatment for persistent amblyopia because in adult animals it reinstates critical period-like ocular dominance plasticity and promotes recovery of visual acuity. Translation of these results from animal models to the clinic, however, has been challenging because of the lack of understanding of how this selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor affects glutamatergic and GABAergic synaptic mechanisms that are essential for experience-dependent plasticity. An appealing hypothesis is that fluoxetine recreates a critical period (CP)-like state by shifting synaptic mechanisms to be more juvenile. To test this we studied the effect of fluoxetine treatment in adult rats, alone or in combination with visual deprivation [monocular deprivation (MD)], on a set of highly conserved presynaptic and postsynaptic proteins (synapsin, synaptophysin, VGLUT1, VGAT, PSD-95, gephyrin, GluN1, GluA2, GluN2B, GluN2A, GABAAα1, GABAAα3). We did not find evidence that fluoxetine shifted the protein amounts or balances to a CP-like state. Instead, it drove the balances in favor of the more mature subunits (GluN2A, GABAAα1). In addition, when fluoxetine was paired with MD it created a neuroprotective-like environment by normalizing the glutamatergic gain found in adult MDs. Together, our results suggest that fluoxetine treatment creates a novel synaptic environment dominated by GluN2A- and GABAAα1-dependent plasticity.

  • adult plasticity
  • amblyopia
  • fluoxetine
  • monocular deprivation
  • receptors
  • visual cortex

Significance Statement

Patching therapy is the most common treatment for children with amblyopia. For many, the acuity recovered during patching is lost when the treatment stops leaving the child with persistent amblyopia. Fluoxetine has emerged as an interesting treatment option because it reinstates critical period-like ocular dominance plasticity and promotes acuity recovery in adult animals. It remains unclear, however, how this selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor affects visual cortex plasticity, which relies heavily on glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses. Here we report the effects of fluoxetine and visual manipulation on the visual cortex of adult rats. Surprisingly we found that fluoxetine did not reinstate a critical period-like state, but rather created a novel synaptic environment that favors mature NMDA and GABAA receptor subunits.

Introduction

Amblyopia is commonly treated with patching, but in some cases, the recovered acuity is lost when patching stops, leaving the child with persistent amblyopia (Birch, 2013). A variety of therapeutics have been proposed to treat persistent amblyopia in adolescents or young adults. Fluoxetine has emerged as a treatment option because it reinstates critical period (CP)-like ocular dominance plasticity and promotes acuity recovery in adult rats (Vetencourt et al., 2008). It is unclear, however, what effects this selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor has on visual cortex (V1) plasticity that relies heavily on maturation of glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses (Levelt and Hübener, 2012). An appealing idea is that fluoxetine shifts the synaptic environment in V1 to a CP-like state that supports heightened experience-dependent plasticity.

During the CP, experience-dependent plasticity is driven by visually evoked responses that depend upon maturation of presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms. Development of presynaptic vesicle cycling proteins (eg, synapsin, synaptophysin) and transporters (eg, VGLUT, VGAT) are necessary for reliable neurotransmitter release (Hopf et al., 2002; Conti et al., 2004; Wojcik et al., 2004, 2006) that drives strong visually evoked activity. Also, shifts in the excitation/inhibition (E/I) balance set up the physiological environment needed for heightened plasticity, triggering the CP (Hensch, 2005; Hensch and Fagiolini, 2005). That E/I balance is mediate by postsynaptic scaffolding proteins PSD-95 and gephyrin that regulate the number of excitatory and inhibitory synapses (Prange et al., 2004; Lardi-Studler et al., 2007; Keith and El-Husseini, 2008). Furthermore, the start of the CP in rat and human V1 coincides with a rapid switch from much more gephyrin to an equal balance with PSD-95 (Pinto et al., 2013, 2015).

A host of glutamatergic and GABAergic receptor mechanisms affect the threshold for CP plasticity. These include addition of AMPA receptors (AMPARs) that end the period of NMDA receptor (NMDAR)-dominated silent synapses (Huang et al., 2015) and add the fast component to EPSCs (Kleppe and Robinson, 1999). Furthermore, the addition of GluN2A-containing NMDARs (Flint et al., 1997; Stocca and Vicini, 1998) speeds up receptor kinetics (Cull-Candy et al., 2001) and affects signaling pathways, such as GluN2B activation of Ras/ERK or alpha calcium-calmodulin kinase II and mTOR pathways (Kim et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2011). The shift to GluN2A also affects functional maturation by mediating sharpening of orientation selectivity (Fagiolini et al., 2003). Finally, activation of GABAA receptors (GABAARs) triggers the start of the CP (Hensch, 2005), and GABAAα1 subunits in particular regulate patterns of activity needed for development of ocular dominance (Fagiolini et al., 2004).

Despite our understanding of the influence of fluoxetine treatment on adult plasticity, there is little evidence to identify “how” fluoxetine affects the expression profile of synaptic mechanism that are critical in the initiation of CP plasticity. To address this, we treated animals with fluoxetine and quantified a set of glutamatergic and GABAergic synaptic proteins to assess if they changed to a CP-like state. We then determined the effects of monocular deprivation (MD) alone, or in combination with fluoxetine on these synaptic proteins. Surprisingly, fluoxetine alone shifted both NMDAR and GABAAR subunits to a more mature composition. Furthermore, when fluoxetine was combined with MD, the treatment normalized the increase in glutamatergic proteins found in adult MD rats. These results show that fluoxetine treatment does not recreate a CP-like synaptic environment but instead shifts plasticity mechanisms to a new state.

Materials and Methods

Animals and surgical procedures

We studied changes in expression of 12 synaptic proteins in V1 of young adult male Long–Evans rats (P98). Rats were individually housed in Plexiglas cages with food and water ad libitum, and a 12 h light/dark cycle. Animals were randomly assigned to one of four groups: normally reared to P98 (n = 6), animals given 4 weeks of fluoxetine (from P70 to P98; 0.2 mg/ml of drinking water; n = 8), animals monocularly deprived (MDed; P91–P98; n=6), or animals that received both fluoxetine (P70–P98) and MD (P91–P98; n=8). Eyelids were closed by trimming the lid margins and suturing them together with 5-0 vicryl using aseptic surgical techniques. The surgery was done using gaseous anesthetic [isoflurane (1.5–5%) in oxygen] for induction and maintenance of anesthesia. Eyelids were inspected daily for openings. All experimental procedures were approved by the [McMaster University] Animal Research Ethics Board.

Tissue collection

Animals were euthanized (sodium pentobarbital, 150 mg/kg), and transcardially perfused with cold 0.1 m PBS (4°C; 4–5 ml/min) until circulating fluid was clear. The brain was quickly removed from the skull and immersed in cold PBS. Bilateral samples of V1 including monocular and binocular regions, quickly frozen on dry ice, and stored at −80°C.

Sample preparation

To study high-abundance vesicle cycling proteins and receptor scaffolding proteins (synapsin, synaptophysin, PSD-95, gephyrin) we prepared homogenate samples. The frozen tissue was added to cold homogenization buffer (1 ml buffer:50 mg tissue containing the following: 0.5 mm DTT, 1mm EDTA, 2 mm EGTA, 10 mm HEPES, 10 mg/L leupeptin, 100 nm microcystin, 0.1 mm PMSF, 50 mg/L soybean trypsin inhibitor) and homogenized in a glass–glass Dounce homogenizer (Kontes). The sample was then combined with 10% SDS. To study lower abundance receptor subunits (GluA2, GluN1, GluN2A, GluN2B, GABAAα1, GABAAα3) and transporters (VGLUT1, VGAT), we enriched the samples following a synaptoneurosomes protocol (Hollingsworth et al., 1985; Quinlan et al., 1999; Murphy et al., 2014). Following the homogenization step each sample was passed through a 5 μm pore hydrophilic mesh filter (Millipore), then centrifuged at 1000 × g for 10 min. Both the synaptic pellet and the whole-homogenate samples were resuspended in boiling 1% SDS. Protein concentrations for each sample was determined using the bicinchoninic acid assay guidelines (Pierce) and final concentrations were adjusted to 1mg/ml using Laemmli sample buffer. A control sample was made by combining a small amount of each of the 28 samples.

Immunoblotting

Samples (25 μg) were resolved on 4–20% SDS–PAGE gels (Precise Protein Gels, Pierce Biotechnology) and transferred to polyvinylidene difluoride (PVDF-FL) membranes (Millipore). Blots were incubated with blocking buffer (Odyssey Blocking Buffer 1:1 with PBS) for 1 h (LI-COR Biosciences), then with primary antibody overnight at 4°C using the following concentrations: GAPDH, 1:4000 (Imgenex); synapsin 1, 1:8000 (Invitrogen); synaptophysin, 1:2000 (Sigma-Aldrich); PSD-95, 1:32000 (Millipore); gephyrin, 1:2000 (Millipore); VGLUT1, 1:2000 (Synaptic Systems); VGAT, 1:2000 (Synaptic Systems); GluA2, 1:2000 (Invitrogen); GluN1, 1:8000 (Chemicon International); GluN2B, 1:2000 (Chemicon International); GluN2A, 1:2000 (PhosphoSolutions); GABAAα1 1:500 (Santa Cruz Biotechnology); GABAAα3 1:2000 (Chemicon International). The blots were washed with PBS containing 0.05% Tween (Sigma-Aldrich; PBS-T; 3 × 10 min), incubated for 1 h at room temperature with the appropriate IRDye-labeled secondary antibody, (anti-mouse, 1:8000; anti-rabbit, 1:10,000; LI-COR Biosciences), and washed in PBS-T (3 × 10 min). The blots were visualized using an Odyssey scanner (LI-COR Biosciences). The combination of IRDye secondary antibodies and Odyssey scanner provides a wide linear dynamic range so that both strong and weak bands could be accurately quantified on the same blot. Blots were stripped and reprobed with additional antibodies (Blot Restore Membrane Rejuvenation kit, Millipore).

Analyses

To analyze the bands, we scanned the blots (Odyssey infrared scanner) and quantified the bands using densitometry (LI-COR Odyssey Software v3.0; LI-COR Biosciences). Density profiles were determined by performing a subtraction of the background, integrating the pixel intensity across the area of the band, and dividing the intensity by the width of the band to control for variations in lane width. Protein loading was checked using GAPDH as a control for sample concentration and volume loaded in each well. Each band was normalized to the average for the set of blots run at the same time and the control sample on the individual blot.

To quantify the relationship between functional pairs of proteins we calculated contrast indices that are commonly used in signal processing to determine the quality of the signal. AMPAR-NMDAR index: (GluA2-GluN1)/(GluA2+GluN1). NMDAR index: (GluN2A-GluN2B)/(GluN2B+GluN2A). GABAAR Index – (GABAA α1-GABAA α3)/(GABAA α1+GABAA α3). Presynaptic E/I index: (VGLUT1-VGAT)/(VGLUT1+VGAT). Postsynaptic E/I index: (PSD-95-gephyrin)/(PSD-95+gephyrin).

To compare levels of protein expression among the groups we made histograms showing the mean and SEM for each group. All results were plotted normalized to the normal young adult group. To make statistical comparisons between groups we used bootstrapping, a modern resampling statistical method that provides robust estimates of SE and confidence intervals, that is especially useful for animal studies such as ours constrained to smaller sample sizes. We used R to simulate a normally distributed dataset with 1,00,000 points and the same mean and SD as the group being compared. To determine differences between groups, we compared the simulation dataset with average protein expression with each of the other groups. We ran a Monte Carlo simulation which randomly samples from the simulation dataset N time, where N was the number of animals in each of the other groups (N=6 or 8). This simulation was repeated 10,000 times to create the normal distribution expected for the N sample sizes. We calculated confidence intervals for the simulated distribution and compared those with the observed means for the other groups. Groups were identified as significantly different (eg, p < 0.05) when the observed average expression was either greater or <95% of the simulated distribution and thus outside its confidence interval (Table 1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Statistical table

Image manipulation

Bands are representative samples taken from different parts of the same gel or different gels. Horizontal and vertical transformations were uniformly applied to size bands appropriately for each figure. A linear adjustment layer was applied uniformly to all bands of each protein, preserving the relative intensities between groups.

Results

We verified that GAPDH was an appropriate loading control by comparing expression of it among the four groups. We found no significant differences from normal demonstrating that GAPDH is an appropriate loading control. We began by examining expression of synapsin, synaptophysin, PSD-95, and gephyrin in V1 ipsilateral to the deprived eye. MD effects are much weaker in the ipsilateral hemisphere (Sawtell et al., 2003) and we did not find any significant differences among the groups for those synaptic proteins (Fig. 1). Thus, all of the following analyses are for V1 contralateral to the deprived eye.

Fig 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 1.

Presynaptic and postsynaptic proteins in ipsilateral V1. In V1 ipsilateral to the deprived eye, there was no effect of experimental condition on the expression of synapsin (A), synaptophysin (B), PSD-95 (C), or gephyrin (D). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001).

Presynaptic changes

We analyzed how fluoxetine changed the presynaptic environment by quantifying a set of proteins involved in cycling, transport, and loading of glutamatergic and GABAergic vesicles. We compared expression of synapsin, synaptophysin, VGLUT1 and VGAT in V1 of normally reared adult rats, rats given 1 month of fluoxetine, 1 week of MD, or the combination of fluoxetine and MD. We found no differences among the groups for synapsin (n.s.; Fig. 2A) or the GABAergic transporter VGAT (n.s.; Fig. 2D) and only a modest loss of synaptophysin for the MDed animals (−13%, SEM 4.1%, p < 0.05; Fig. 2B). The glutamate transporter VGLUT1, however, had more changes. MDed animals had an increase in VGLUT1 (+25%, SEM 8.4%, p < 0.001), whereas both groups of fluoxetine treated animals had less VGLUT1 than normal (fluoxetine alone −29%, SEM 3.0%, p < 0.0001; fluoxetine+MD −13%, SEM 4.9%, p < 0.05; Fig. 2C.

Fig 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 2.

Presynaptic vesicle cycling and transporter proteins. In contralateral V1, synapsin (A) was not affected by experimental condition. For synaptophysin (B) fluoxetine alone had no effect, MD alone caused a loss of expression, but combining fluoxetine with MD prevented the MD-induced loss. For VGLUT1 (C) fluoxetine alone or with MD caused a loss of expression, but MD alone increased expression. VGAT (D) was not affected by experimental condition. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.

Postsynaptic changes

Next, we examined how fluoxetine changed the expression of a set of postsynaptic scaffolding proteins and receptor subunits for glutamatergic and GABAergic receptors. Changes among the groups were very similar for PSD-95 and gephyrin. Fluoxetine alone did not change the level of expression relative to normal animals, but MD caused loss of expression (PSD-95 −37%, SEM 5.6%, p ∼ 0.06; gephyrin −45%, SEM 4.0%, p < 0.01) and fluoxetine combined with MD increased expression (PSD-95 +46%, SEM 15%, p < 0.05; gephyrin +34% SEM 11%, p < 0.05; Fig. 3A,B).

Fig 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 3.

Postsynaptic receptor scaffolding proteins and subunits. In contralateral V1, PSD-95 (A) and gephyrin (B) had a similar pattern of changes: fluoxetine alone had no effect, MD alone caused a loss of expression, but combining fluoxetine with MD prevented the MD-induced loss and caused super-compensation above normal levels. GluN1 (C) was reduced by fluoxetine regardless of visual experience, whereas MD alone caused an increase. GluA2 (D) was unaffected by fluoxetine alone, MD caused an increase, but combing fluoxetine with MD caused a decrease. GluN2B (E) was reduced by fluoxetine regardless of visual experience, whereas MD had no effect. GluN2A (F) expression of each experimental group was not different from normal animals, but the MDed group had higher expression than either fluoxetine alone or fluoxetine combined with MD. GABAAα3 (G) was unaffected by fluoxetine alone, MD caused an increase, but combing fluoxetine with MD prevented the MD-induced increase. GABAAα1 (H) was increased by fluoxetine regardless of visual experience, while MD alone had no effect. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.

The pattern of changes for the receptor subunits was almost opposite to the scaffolding proteins. For the glutamatergic receptor subunits (GluN1, GluA2, GluN2B, GluN2A) fluoxetine alone caused losses for GluN1 and GluN2B (GluN1 −15%, SEM 4.8%, p < 0.01; GluN2B −28%, SEM 4.5%, p < 0.01) and when combined with MD caused a loss of GluA2, as well as losses for GluN1 and GluN2B (GluA2 −15%, SEM 4.2%, p < 0.0001; GluN1 −18%, SEM 4.5%, p < 0.0001; GluN2B −21%, SEM 4%, p < 0.05; Fig. 3C–F). However, MD alone caused either an increase (GluN1 +25%, SEM 8.8%, p < 0.0001; GluA2 +18%, SEM 5.9%, p < 0.05) or no significant change from normal (GluN2B, GluN2A, n.s.). Thus, MD alone caused gains for these glutamatergic subunits that were reduced when MD was combined with fluoxetine. MD also increased GABAAα3 (+18%, SEM 6.6%, p < 0.001; Fig. 3G) but did not change GABAAα1 (Fig. 3H). In contrast, GABAAα1 was increased in both fluoxetine treated groups (fluoxetine alone +24%, SEM 11%, p < 0.001; fluoxetine+MD +24%, SEM 20%, p < 0.001; Fig. 3H).

Receptor subunit balances

During development, there are a series of maturational shifts in expression of glutamatergic and GABAergic receptor subunits. One of the shifts is the change from NMDAR-dominated silent synapses to AMPAR activated synapses. We studied if fluoxetine created a CP-like state by shifting the GluA2–GluN1 balance in favor of GluN1 but found no changes from the normal adult balance (n.s; Fig. 4A). Different results were found when the GluN2A–GluN2B and GABAAα1–GABAAα3 balances were examined. During normal development there is an increase in GluN2A, shifting the balance from much more GluN2B to slightly in favor of GluN2B in young adult rats (Fig. 4B). However, all of the experimental groups changed beyond that level toward even more GluN2A (p < 0.05). There were differences, however, in what drove the changes in the GluN2A–GluN2B balance with the fluoxetine groups shift being caused by less GluN2B, whereas the MD shift was caused by more GluN2A. The GABAAα1–GABAAα3 balance revealed another dissociation among the experimental groups (Fig. 4C). Here the MD shift was caused by a 20% increase in GABAAα3 (p < 0.05), whereas the shift for the fluoxetine groups was caused by a 20% increase in GABAAα1 (fluoxetine alone, p < 0.01; fluoxetine+MD, p < 0.05; Fig. 4C). This series of subunit balances unpacks subtle effect of fluoxetine treatment showing that it does not cause a shift to a CP-like state, instead it maintains subunit balances that are like normal adults (GluA2–GluN1) or shifted to more of the mature subunits (GluN2A, GABAAα1).

Fig 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 4.

Postsynaptic receptor subunit balances. Neither fluoxetine, MD, nor fluoxetine combined with MD affected the relative abundance of GluN1-containing NMDARs and GluA2-containing AMPARs in contralateral V1 (A). Fluoxetine shifted the relative abundance of NMDAR subunits in favor of the more mature GluN2A subunit, regardless of visual experience. MD caused a shift in favor of the more immature GluN2B (B). Fluoxetine shifted the relative abundance of GABAAR subunits in favor of the more mature α1 subunit, regardless of visual experience. MD caused a shift in favor of the more α3 subunit (C). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.

E/I balances

The final analyses examined presynaptic and postsynaptic proteins that regulate the E/I balance. First, we calculated a presynaptic E/I balance using the vesicular transporters VGLUT1 and VGAT. MD caused a large shift toward VGLUT1 (p < 0.05; Fig. 5A) but when combined with fluoxetine there was no change in the presynaptic E/I balance. The same pattern was seen on the postsynaptic side, here MD also caused a large shift toward the excitatory side (more PSD-95; p < 0.05; Fig. 5B) but when MD was paired with fluoxetine there was no change from the normal adult E/I balance.

Fig 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 5.

Presynaptic and Postsynaptic E/I balance. Presynaptic Index in contralateral V1 (A): (VGLUT1−VGAT)/(VGLUT1+VGAT). Postsynaptic Index in contralateral V1 (B): (PSD-95−gephyrin)/(PSD-95+gephyrin). We found strikingly similar patters in the presynaptic and postsynaptic indexes of E/I synapses. Fluoxetine caused a slight shift toward inhibition in the presynaptic index and had no effect on the postsynaptic index. MD caused a strong shift to excitatory markers. Combining fluoxetine and MD kept the balance at normal levels. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.

Discussion

In this study, we quantified the effect of fluoxetine treatment on 12 glutamatergic and GABAergic markers linked with visual experience-dependent plasticity in V1. Fluoxetine caused a pattern of change in those markers that provides new insights into how this drug affects plasticity in adult V1. We compared normal adult rats with ones treated with either fluoxetine alone, MD, or fluoxetine paired with MD. The main findings are that fluoxetine treatment in adult rats does not shift these markers to a younger pattern but instead rebalances MD driven glutamatergic gain and promotes a novel synaptic environment.

In this study, we used Western blotting to quantify the effects of fluoxetine treatment on a collection of synaptic proteins in adult V1. A strength of this approach is that a large number of synaptic proteins were analyzed. Western blotting, however, does not provide laminar or cell-specific information that is needed to identify the neural circuits in V1 affected by fluoxetine. Future neuroanatomical studies are needed to address that question and those studies may be guided by the current results.

Fluoxetine does not recreate a younger synaptic environment

An appealing hypothesis about drug treatments, such as fluoxetine, is that they reinstate ocular dominance plasticity in adult V1 by changing the synaptic environment to a CP-like state. During the CP, there are increases in amount of proteins and shifts in balances between functional pairs of synaptic proteins. Our results do not support the idea that fluoxetine in adult rats dials back synaptic age. For example, we found that fluoxetine combined with MD caused greater expression of PSD-95 and gephyrin. These levels were higher than found during the CP (Pinto et al., 2015) and are consistent with a spike in PSD-95 that ends the CP (Huang et al., 2015). Furthermore, fluoxetine alone did not reduce expression of either scaffolding protein and only MD caused a loss of PSD-95 and gephyrin. The modest losses for VGLUT1 and receptor subunits caused by fluoxetine suggest a shift to a more immature stage, but the balances among the subunits do not support that conclusion. If a younger synaptic environment was recreated then it should favor NMDA over AMPA receptors (Wu et al., 1996), GluN2B over GluN2A (Carmignoto and Vicini, 1992; Flint et al., 1997; Stocca and Vicini, 1998) and GABAAα3 over GABAAα1 (Laurie et al., 1992). Instead, the NMDAR to AMPAR ratios were balanced for both fluoxetine groups (GluN1∼GluA2), whereas subunit balances jumped past age-matched adults toward even more of the mature subunits (GluN2A, GABAAα1). Finally, E/I balances for presynaptic and postsynaptic markers were similar to the normal adults in both fluoxetine groups. Together these findings illustrate that fluoxetine treatment did not simply recreate a CP-like synaptic environment in V1.

It is important to note that we examined synaptic proteins after 1 month of fluoxetine treatment and 1week of MD. We know from previous studies (Williams et al., 2015) that there are dynamic changes in synaptic proteins during a period of MD and it seems reasonable to propose that fluoxetine treatment may cause similarly dynamic changes. Thus, the findings here provide a snapshot of long-term effects of fluoxetine treatment. It will be important for future studies to probe other time points to understand the full landscape of synaptic changes and how transient changes caused by fluoxetine (Vetencourt et al., 2011) impact long-term plasticity in V1.

Fluoxetine triggers a novel synaptic environment in adult V1

The original study showing that fluoxetine reinstates ocular dominance plasticity also found improvement of visual function, reduced intracortical inhibition, and increased BDNF expression (Vetencourt et al., 2008). All of those changes occurred without significantly altering neuronal responsiveness or orientation selectivity in V1. Here we found normal presynaptic and postsynaptic E/I balances, and adult levels of GABAAα1 that could support normal responsiveness and orientation selectivity. A previous study of gene expression found reduced VGAT after fluoxetine treatment but no changes in other genes associated with E/I mechanisms (Tiraboschi et al., 2013). We, however, did not find that fluoxetine caused a loss of VGAT protein expression. Some forms of GABAergic plasticity involve changes in VGAT protein expression associated with the amount of neurotransmitter in vesicles (Hartman et al., 2006), and the lack of change in VGAT makes it unlikely that fluoxetine altered this type of plasticity.

A recent proteomic analysis found that fluoxetine caused alterations in cytoskeleton organization, endocytosis, molecular transport, intracellular signaling, redox cellular state, metabolism, and protein degradation (Ruiz-Perera et al., 2015). Those changes included proteins that regulate AMPAR and GABAAR, and may affect the E/I balance. Nonetheless, our quantification of synaptic proteins, along with the gene and proteomic studies, show that fluoxetine affects mechanisms that regulate experience-dependent plasticity.

The GluN2A–GluN2B and GABAAα1–GABAAα3 balances were both affected by fluoxetine and importantly the GABAA balance differentiated fluoxetine treatments from the effects of MD. The changes in these functional pairs of glutamatergic and GABAergic receptor subunits suggest that fluoxetine creates a novel synaptic environment in adult V1. An environment that is dominated by GluN2A and GABAAα1 but also has balanced levels of presynaptic and postsynaptic E/I markers. Both GluN2A and GABAAα1 subunits are described as mature components because they gradually increase during development and affect plasticity. For example, the developmental shift from GluN2B to more GluN2A speeds up receptor kinetics (Cull-Candy et al., 2001), changes cellular signaling (Kim et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2011), relieves GluN2B negative regulation of AMPARs (Hall and Ghosh, 2008), and controls metaplasticity in V1 (Philpot et al., 2007). GABAAα1 is necessary for normal development of orientation tuning (Fagiolini et al., 2004) and gamma rhythms (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009). The prevalence of GABAAα1-positive synapses on pyramidal cell bodies makes them important components in GABAergic regulation of experience-dependent plasticity (Hensch, 2005; Griffen and Maffei, 2014). The different roles of these subunits in experience-dependent plasticity suggests that fluoxetine creates a unique synaptic environment in adult V1 that can support both GluN2A-dependent metaplasticity and GABAergic regulation of ocular dominance plasticity.

How might fluoxetine trigger adult plasticity?

Reduced intracortical GABA and GABAergic transmission have been found after fluoxetine treatment (Vetencourt et al., 2008; Baroncelli et al., 2011). In contrast, we found a small increase in GABAAα1 expression and no loss of GABAAα3 or VGAT in rats treated with fluoxetine. Previous studies have shown that fluoxetine positively modulates GABAA receptors and one way is by increasing receptor sensitivity to small amounts of GABA (Robinson, 2002). The α1 subunit is one of the subtypes that confer that increased sensitivity and perhaps more GABAAα1 expression modulates GABAergic drive when the amount of neurotransmitter is reduced by fluoxetine. Interestingly, during the CP a brief exposure to vision after deprivation causes a rapid rebound potentiation in miniature IPSCs (mIPSCs) that is correlated with an increase in GABAARs (Gao et al., 2014). Perhaps the increase in GABAAα1 expression found here supports a similar potentiation of mIPSCs and because GABAAα1-containing synapses form a key part of the neural circuitry involved in ocular dominance plasticity (Hensch, 2005) fluoxetine may drive a compensatory mechanisms where sensitized GABAARs enhance adult plasticity.

We also found that fluoxetine caused changes to glutamatergic receptor subunit expression. Fluoxetine is known to inhibit NMDA receptors and may provide neuroprotective effects by regulating glutamatergic involvement in excitotoxicity (Szasz et al., 2007). We found that fluoxetine paired with MD ameliorated glutamatergic gain driven by MD alone, suggesting that one of fluoxetine's effects in adult V1 may be neuroprotective. Fluoxetine acts by inhibiting GluN2B-containing NMDARs (Kiss et al., 2012) and that may trigger increases in both BDNF and AMPARs. GluN2B-mediated signaling inhibits AMPAR trafficking and the amount of GluA2-containing AMPARs (Kim et al., 2005; Derkach et al., 2007; Hall et al., 2007; Hall and Ghosh, 2008) through unique cellular processes that include Ras/ERK, αCamKII, and mTor pathways (Kim et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2011). One way that fluoxetine could affect adult ocular dominance plasticity is if the loss of GluN2B changes the length of GluN2B-mediated Ras/ERK activation (Kim et al., 2005) thereby increasing insertion of AMPAR into synapses and supporting long-term potentiation (LTP). ERK activation is necessary for ocular dominance plasticity in developing V1 (Di Cristo et al., 2001) and fluoxetine in adult animals may enhance ERK-dependent plasticity through the loss of GluN2B.

During the CP ocular dominance plasticity reflects the depression of deprived eye responses but in adults MD leads to enhancement of open-eye responses in V1 (Sawtell et al., 2003). That adult plasticity is dependent on activation of NMDARs and may use Hebbian [LTP, long-term depression (LTD), spike time-dependent plasticity], homeostatic, or metaplasticity (synaptic modification threshold) mechanisms (for review, see Hofer et al., 2006). Visual experience-driven changes to LTP and LTD during the CP depend on GluN2A and previous studies have identified shifts in the GluN2A–GluN2B balance as the mechanism underlying an adjustable synaptic modification threshold in V1 (Philpot et al., 2007). Perhaps the shift to balanced GluN2A–GluN2B expression after fluoxetine treatment is an indication that metaplasticity plays a dominant role in fluoxetine driven adult plasticity. Interestingly, in auditory cortex fluoxetine reduces the potential for LTP (Dringenberg et al., 2014) raising the possibility that the effects of fluoxetine might not be uniform across the cortex.

Fluoxetine could also trigger events similar to those promoted by other NMDAR antagonists that cause a transient burst of glutamate, followed by BDNF release and synapse formation (Duman and Aghajanian, 2014). BDNF plays a key role in fluoxetine’s reactivation of plasticity (Castrén and Rantamäki, 2010) suggesting that a fluoxetine induced loss of GluN2B signaling may enhance BDNF and AMPAR involvement in experience-dependent plasticity in adult V1. Thus, fluoxetine appears to enhance glutamatergic and GABAergic mechanisms that support experience-dependent plasticity in adult V1.

Implications for other therapies

A variety of other methods are being explored to promote adult recovery from persistent amblyopia, such as dark rearing in animals (He et al., 2006, 2007; Montey and Quinlan, 2011; Duffy and Mitchell, 2013), manipulation of the brakes on plasticity including PirB (Bochner et al., 2014) and chondroitin sulphate proteoglycans (Pizzorusso et al., 2002; Morishita et al., 2010; Bukhari et al., 2015), environmental enrichment (Sale et al., 2007), patterned visual stimulation (Montey et al., 2013), or perceptual learning (Levi and Li, 2009; Baroncelli et al., 2011; Bonaccorsi et al., 2014; Tsirlin et al., 2015). All of these appear to reactivate a certain degree of plasticity that can support ocular dominance plasticity and even visual recovery. The cellular mechanisms typically include LTP of cortical synapses, and although some molecular changes have been identified (He et al., 2006), the full extent has yet to be explored. Do these other techniques mimic the novel pattern of fluoxetine driven glutamatergic and GABAergic changes or do they create different synaptic environments? These are important questions to answer to determine whether these adult manipulations activate one or many different forms of experience-dependent plasticity in V1.

Future studies will need to determine the long-term consequences of fluoxetine-induced changes in adult V1. It is not clear whether stopping drug treatment will allow the synaptic environment to shift back to a normal adult state. In addition, if not what effects that new synaptic environment may have on neural function in the long-term. Finally, it will be important to determine how much of these effects are driven by the increase in serotonin, as opposed to unique effects of fluoxetine. Each of these are important questions to answer that well help to understand plasticity in adult V1 and translate that knowledge into effective treatments for persistent amblyopia.

Footnotes

  • ↵1 The authors report no conflict of interest.

  • ↵3 This work was supported by NSERC Grant RGPIN-2015-06215 awarded to K.M.M.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Baroncelli L, Maffei L, Sale A (2011) New perspectives in amblyopia therapy on adults: a critical role for the excitatory/inhibitory balance. Front Cell Neurosci 5:25. doi:10.3389/fncel.2011.00025 pmid:22144947
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Birch EE (2013) Amblyopia and binocular vision. Prog Retin Eye Res 33:67–84. doi:10.1016/j.preteyeres.2012.11.001 pmid:23201436
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Bochner DN, Sapp RW, Adelson JD, Zhang S, Lee H, Djurisic M, Syken J, Dan Y, Shatz CJ (2014) Blocking PirB up-regulates spines and functional synapses to unlock visual cortical plasticity and facilitate recovery from amblyopia. Sci Transl Med 6:258ra140. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3010157 pmid:25320232
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Bonaccorsi J, Berardi N, Sale A (2014) Treatment of amblyopia in the adult: insights from a new rodent model of visual perceptual learning. Front Neural Circuits 8:82. doi:10.3389/fncir.2014.00082 pmid:25076874
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Bukhari N, Burman PN, Hussein A, Demars MP, Sadahiro M, Brady DM, Tsirka SE, Russo SJ, Morishita H (2015) Unmasking proteolytic activity for adult visual cortex plasticity by the removal of Lynx1. J Neurosci 35:12693–12702. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4315-14.2015 pmid:26377459
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Cardin JA, Carlén M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai L-H, Moore CI (2009) Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459:663–667. doi:10.1038/nature08002 pmid:19396156
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Carmignoto G, Vicini S (1992) Activity-dependent decrease in NMDA receptor responses during development of the visual cortex. Science 258:1007–1011. pmid:1279803
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    Castrén E, Rantamäki T (2010) The role of BDNF and its receptors in depression and antidepressant drug action: reactivation of developmental plasticity. Dev Neurobiol 70:289–297. doi:10.1002/dneu.20758 pmid:20186711
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Conti F, Minelli A, Melone M (2004) GABA transporters in the mammalian cerebral cortex: localization, development and pathological implications. Brain Res Rev 45:196–212. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.03.003 pmid:15210304
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Cull-Candy S, Brickley S, Farrant M (2001) NMDA receptor subunits: diversity, development and disease. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:327–335. pmid:11399431
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Derkach VA, Oh MC, Guire ES, Soderling TR (2007) Regulatory mechanisms of AMPA receptors in synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:101–113. doi:10.1038/nrn2055 pmid:17237803
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Di Cristo G, Berardi N, Cancedda L, Pizzorusso T, Putignano E, Ratto GM, Maffei L (2001) Requirement of ERK activation for visual cortical plasticity. Science 292:2337–2340. doi:10.1126/science.1059075 pmid:11423664
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    Dringenberg HC, Branfield Day LR, Choi DH (2014) Chronic Fluoxetine treatment suppresses plasticity (long-term potentiation) in the mature rodent primary auditory cortex in vivo. Neural Plast 2014:571285. doi:10.1155/2014/571285 pmid:24719772
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Duffy KR, Mitchell DE (2013) Darkness alters maturation of visual cortex and promotes fast recovery from monocular deprivation. Curr Biol 23:382–386. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.017 pmid:23416100
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Duman RS, Aghajanian GK (2014) Neurobiology of rapid acting antidepressants: role of BDNF and GSK-3β. Neuropsychopharmacol 39:233–252. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.217 pmid:24317309
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Fagiolini M, Fritschy JM, Löw K, Möhler H, Rudolph U, Hensch TK (2004) Specific GABAA circuits for visual cortical plasticity. Science 303:1681–1683. doi:10.1126/science.1091032 pmid:15017002
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    Fagiolini M, Katagiri H, Miyamoto H, Mori H, Grant SGN, Mishina M, Hensch TK (2003) Separable features of visual cortical plasticity revealed by N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor 2A signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:2854–2859. doi:10.1073/pnas.0536089100 pmid:12591944
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    Flint AC, Maisch US, Weishaupt JH, Kriegstein AR, Monyer H (1997) NR2A subunit expression shortens NMDA receptor synaptic currents in developing neocortex. J Neurosci 17:2469–2476. pmid:9065507
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Gao M, Maynard KR, Chokshi V, Song L, Jacobs C, Wang H, Tran T, Martinowich K, Lee HK (2014) Rebound potentiation of inhibition in juvenile visual cortex requires vision-induced BDNF expression. J Neurosci 34:10770–10779. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5454-13.2014 pmid:25100608
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    Griffen TC, Maffei A (2014) GABAergic synapses: their plasticity and role in sensory cortex. Front Cell Neurosci 8:91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Hall BJ, Ghosh A (2008) Regulation of AMPA receptor recruitment at developing synapses. Trends Neurosci 31:82–89. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.11.010 pmid:18201773
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Hall BJ, Ripley B, Ghosh A (2007) NR2B signaling regulates the development of synaptic AMPA receptor current. J Neurosci 27:13446–13456. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3793-07.2007 pmid:18057203
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    Hartman KN, Pal SK, Burrone J, Murthy VN (2006) Activity-dependent regulation of inhibitory synaptic transmission in hippocampal neurons. Nat Neurosci 9:642–649. doi:10.1038/nn1677 pmid:16582905
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    He H-Y, Hodos W, Quinlan EM (2006) Visual deprivation reactivates rapid ocular dominance plasticity in adult visual cortex. J Neurosci 26:2951–2955. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5554-05.2006 pmid:16540572
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    He HY, Ray B, Dennis K, Quinlan EM (2007) Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Nat Neurosci 10:1134–1136. doi:10.1038/nn1965 pmid:17694050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Hensch TK (2005) Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:877–888. doi:10.1038/nrn1787 pmid:16261181
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Hensch TK, Fagiolini M (2005) Excitatory–inhibitory balance and critical period plasticity in developing visual cortex. In: Progress in brain research: development, dynamics and pathiology of neuronal networks: from molecules to functional circuits, Vol 147, pp. 115–124. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  28. ↵
    Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M (2006) Lifelong learning: ocular dominance plasticity in mouse visual cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:451–459. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.06.007 pmid:16837188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Hollingsworth EB, McNeal ET, Burton JL, Williams RJ, Daly JW, Creveling CR (1985) Biochemical characterization of a filtered synaptoneurosome preparation from guinea pig cerebral cortex: cyclic adenosine 3′:5′-monophosphate-generating systems, receptors, and enzymes. J Neurosci 5:2240–2253. pmid:2991484
    OpenUrlAbstract
  30. ↵
    Hopf W, Waters J, Mehta S, Smith SJ (2002) Stability and plasticity of developing synapses in hippocampal neuronal cultures. J Neurosci 22:775–781. pmid:11826107
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    Huang X, Stodieck SK, Goetze B, Cui L, Wong MH, Wenzel C, Hosang L, Dong Y, Löwel S, Schlüter OM (2015) Progressive maturation of silent synapses governs the duration of a critical period. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:E3131–E3140. doi:10.1073/pnas.1506488112 pmid:26015564
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    Keith D, El-Husseini A (2008) Excitation control: balancing PSD-95 function at the synapse. Front Mol Neurosci 1:4. doi:10.3389/neuro.02.004.2008 pmid:18946537
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Kim MJ, Dunah AW, Wang YT, Sheng M (2005) Differential roles of NR2A- and NR2B-containing NMDA receptors in Ras-ERK signaling and AMPA receptor trafficking. Neuron 46:745–760. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.031 pmid:15924861
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Kiss JP, Szasz BK, Fodor L, Mike A, Lenkey N, Kurkó D, Nagy J, Vizi ES (2012) GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors as possible targets for the neuroprotective and antidepressant effects of fluoxetine. Neurochem Int 60:170–176. doi:10.1016/j.neuint.2011.12.005 pmid:22197911
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Kleppe IC, Robinson HP (1999) Determining the activation time course of synaptic AMPA receptors from openings of colocalized NMDA receptors. Biophys J 77:1418–1427. doi:10.1016/S0006-3495(99)76990-0 pmid:10465753
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Lardi-Studler B, Smolinsky B, Petitjean CM, Koenig F, Sidler C, Meier JC, Fritschy JM, Schwarz G (2007) Vertebrate-specific sequences in the gephyrin E-domain regulate cytosolic aggregation and postsynaptic clustering. J Cell Sci 120:1371–1382. doi:10.1242/jcs.003905 pmid:17374639
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Laurie DJ, Wisden W, Seeburg PH (1992) The distribution of thirteen GABAA receptor subunit mRNAs in the rat brain: III. Embryonic and postnatal development. J Neurosci 12:4151–4172. pmid:1331359
    OpenUrlAbstract
  38. ↵
    Levelt CN, Hübener M (2012) Critical-period plasticity in the visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:309–330. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113813 pmid:22462544
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Levi DM, Li RW (2009) Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: a mini-review. Vision Res 49:2535–2549. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.010 pmid:19250947
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Montey KL, Eaton NC, Quinlan EM (2013) Repetitive visual stimulation enhances recovery from severe amblyopia. Learn Mem 20:311–317. doi:10.1101/lm.030361.113 pmid:23685763
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Montey KL, Quinlan EM (2011) Recovery from chronic monocular deprivation following reactivation of thalamocortical plasticity by dark exposure. Nat Commun 2:317. doi:10.1038/ncomms1312 pmid:21587234
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Morishita H, Miwa JM, Heintz N, Hensch TK (2010) Lynx1, a cholinergic brake, limits plasticity in adult visual cortex. Science 330:1238–1240. doi:10.1126/science.1195320 pmid:21071629
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Murphy KM, Balsor J, Beshara S, Siu C, Pinto JGA (2014) A high-throughput semi-automated preparation for filtered synaptoneurosomes. J Neurosci Methods 235:35–40. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.05.036 pmid:24997341
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Philpot BD, Cho KKA, Bear MF (2007) Obligatory role of NR2A for metaplasticity in visual cortex. Neuron 53:495–502. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.027 pmid:17296552
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Pinto JGA, Jones DG, Murphy KM (2013) Comparing development of synaptic proteins in rat visual, somatosensory, and frontal cortex. Front Neural Circuits 7:97. doi:10.3389/fncir.2013.00097 pmid:23754984
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Pinto JG, Jones DG, Williams CK, Murphy KM (2015) Characterizing synaptic protein development in human visual cortex enables alignment of synaptic age with rat visual cortex. Front Neural Circuits 9:3. doi:10.3389/fncir.2015.00003
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    Pizzorusso T, Medini P, Berardi N, Chierzi S, Fawcett JW, Maffei L (2002) Reactivation of ocular dominance plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science 298:1248–1251. doi:10.1126/science.1072699 pmid:12424383
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    Prange O, Wong TP, Gerrow K, Wang YT, El-Husseini A (2004) A balance between excitatory and inhibitory synapses is controlled by PSD-95 and neuroligin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:13915–13920. doi:10.1073/pnas.0405939101 pmid:15358863
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    Quinlan EM, Olstein DH, Bear MF (1999) Bidirectional, experience-dependent regulation of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor subunit composition in the rat visual cortex during postnatal development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:12876–12880. pmid:10536016
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    Robinson RT (2002) Fluoxetine increases GABAA receptor activity through a novel modulatory site. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 304:978–984. doi:10.1124/jpet.102.044834
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  51. ↵
    Ruiz-Perera L, Muniz M, Vierci G, Bornia N, Baroncelli L, Sale A, Rossi FM (2015) Fluoxetine increases plasticity and modulates the proteomic profile in the adult mouse visual cortex. Sci Rep 5:12517. doi:10.1038/srep12517 pmid:26205348
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Sale A, Vetencourt JFM, Medini P, Cenni MC, Baroncelli L, De Pasquale R, Maffei L (2007) Environmental enrichment in adulthood promotes amblyopia recovery through a reduction of intracortical inhibition. Nat Neurosci 10:679–681. doi:10.1038/nn1899 pmid:17468749
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Sawtell NB, Frenkel MY, Philpot BD, Nakazawa K, Tonegawa S, Bear MF (2003) NMDA receptor-dependent ocular dominance plasticity in adult visual cortex. Neuron 38:977–985. pmid:12818182
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K (2009) Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature 459:698–702. doi:10.1038/nature07991 pmid:19396159
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Stocca G, Vicini S (1998) Increased contribution of NR2A subunit to synaptic NMDA receptors in developing rat cortical neurons. J Phys 507:13–24. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.013bu.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Szasz BK, Mike A, Karoly R, Gerevich Z, Illes P, Vizi ES, Kiss JP (2007) Direct inhibitory effect of fluoxetine on N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors in the central nervous system. Biol Psychiat 62:1303–1309. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.014 pmid:17659262
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Tiraboschi E, Guirado R, Greco D, Auvinen P, Maya-Vetencourt JF, Maffei L, Castrén E (2013) Gene expression patterns underlying the reinstatement of plasticity in the adult visual system. Neural Plast 2013:605079–605079. doi:10.1155/2013/605079 pmid:23936678
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Tsirlin I, Colpa L, Goltz HC, Wong AMF (2015) Behavioral training as new treatment for adult amblyopia: a meta-analysis and systematic review meta-analysis of behavioral training for amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 56:4061–4075. doi:10.1167/iovs.15-16583 pmid:26114483
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Vetencourt J, Sale A, Viegi A, Baroncelli L (2008) The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science 320:385–388. doi:10.1126/science.1150516 pmid:18420937
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    Vetencourt J, Tiraboschi E, Spolidoro M, Castrén E, Maffei L (2011) Serotonin triggers a transient epigenetic mechanism that reinstates adult visual cortex plasticity in rats. Eur J Neurosci 33:49–57. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07488.x pmid:21156002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Wang CC, Held RG, Chang S-C, Yang L, Delpire E, Ghosh A, Hall BJ (2011) A critical role for GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in cortical development and function. Neuron 72:789–805. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.023 pmid:22153375
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Williams K, Balsor JL, Beshara S, Beston BR, Jones DG, Murphy KM (2015) Experience-dependent central vision deficits: neurobiology and visual acuity. Vision Res 114:68–78. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.021 pmid:25668772
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Wojcik SM, Katsurabayashi S, Guillemin I, Friauf E, Rosenmund C, Brose N, Rhee JS (2006) A shared vesicular carrier allows synaptic corelease of GABA and glycine. Neuron 50:575–587. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.016 pmid:16701208
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Wojcik SM, Rhee JS, Herzog E, Sigler A, Jahn R, Takamori S, Brose N, Rosenmund C (2004) An essential role for vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (VGLUT1) in postnatal development and control of quantal size. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:7158–7163. doi:10.1073/pnas.0401764101 pmid:15103023
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    Wu GY, Malinow R, Cline HT (1996) Maturation of a central glutamatergic synapse. Science 274:972. pmid:8875937
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text

Synthesis

The decision was a result of the Reviewing Editor Marlene Bartos and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus was reached. A fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision is listed below. The following reviewers agreed to reveal their identity: Hans Dringenberg, Nicoletta Berardi

Both reviewers were very excited about the novelty of data presented in the manuscript. 'This manuscript describes interesting and novel findings regarding the effect of fluoxetine on V1 plasticity and synaptic functioning. There is considerable interest in using fluoxetine to promote plasticity of cortical circuits, an effect that is thought to involve up-regulation of synaptic proteins that predominate during the earlier, critical/sensitive periods of cortical maturation. Here, the authors directly test this hypothesis in a comprehensive and detailed manner. The results are surprising and challenge the notion that fluoxetine reinstates critical period-like plasticity in V1.' 'The data are clear and new. Experimental design appears appropriate. Statistical analysis is appropriate.

However, a main weakness was related to the unspecificity of homogenates. They prevent a direct link between changes on the protein level and specific cell types. Moreover, antibody labelling against the various molecules found to be affected by fluoxetine would add great significance, because it would show whether the effects found are cell-specific. The lack of physiological data was viewed as a diminishing factor. If electrophysiological data (see below) cannot be provided by the authors, than they should at least discuss the possible functional consequences (e.g. IPSP, mIPSCs) of the changes in protein expression.

Reviewer 1:

Specific comments:

1. p. 4, line 57: define "EPSC" on first use

2. p. 4, line 66: "...of the influence of fluoxetine treatment of adult plasticity..." should be "...fluoxetine treatment ON adult plasticity"

3. p. 4, lines 69-70: "To address this, we treated animals with fluoxetine AND quantified a set of glutamatergic and GABAergic synaptic proteins to (asses) ASSESS if they changed to a CP-like state. (When) WE then..."

4. p. 5, sentence "But when combined with MD, fluoxetine treatment normalized the glutamatergic shift found in adult MD rats.": Avoid using "But" at the beginning of a new sentence. Also, avoid vague language: what is a "glutamatergic shift"? You probably mean "an increase in glutamatergic strength of synapses in V1 representing the non-deprived eye"?

5. p. 6, line 83: be consistent in use of abbreviations, MD for monocular deprivation, not MDep as is used here.

6. Results: The authors collected bilateral samples of V1 tissue. However, in the Results section, it is never specified if data are for the V1 ipsilateral or contralateral to the deprived eye. I assume it is always contralateral, but this needs to be stated. Also, what happened in ipsilateral V1?

7. The authors need to clarify whether their samples included both the monocular and binocular zones of V1. In the Discussion, it should be mentioned that these zones differ in terms of synaptic inputs and plasticity mechanisms?

8. p. 11, line 191-192 and elsewhere: When reporting means for decreases in expression, use negative numbers e.g., less VGLUT1 should be -29%.

9. p. 14, first sentence: check grammar; also, first part of sentence not necessary.

10. p. 16, lines 290-291: "A gene study found reduced VGAT after fluoxetine treatment but no changes in other genes associated with E/I mechanisms (Tiraboschi et al., 2013)." Re-word this sentence; what is a "gene study"? Tiraboschi et al. examined gene expression, not genes.

11. p. 19, last sentence of top paragraph, "Thus, fluoxetine appears to set up an array of glutamatergic mechanisms in adult V1 that are known to support experience-dependent plasticity (Cooke and Bear, 2014)."

This seems very much at odds with some of the experimental results and previous discussion, specifically the failure of fluoxetine to create a CP-like synaptic environment and the increase (absolute and/or relative) in receptor subunits that indicate a more mature synaptic state. To reconcile these discrepancies, it would be important to discuss the difference (as shown in prior work) in synaptic mechanisms mediating OD plasticity in juvenile and adult animals. Given that these mechanisms differ, and that fluoxetine does not re-create CP-like synaptic environments, it seems that it may act on those mechanisms that are particularly important for OD shifts in adulthood. This possibility and its implications need to be addressed.

12. p. 25, line 456: This reference is incomplete and should be: Dringenberg, H.C., Branfield Day, L.R., and Choi, D.H. (2014) Chronic fluoxetine treatment suppresses plasticity (long-term potentiation, LTP) in the mature rodent primary auditory cortex in vivo. Neural Plasticity, vol. 2014, Article ID 571285, 9 pages, 2014. doi:10.1155/2014/571285.

13.p. 27, line 495: incomplete reference

14. p. 30, line 544: incomplete reference

Reviewer 2:

Major point: analysis of homogenates prevents from linking the changes found with specific cell populations. An immuno for the molecules found to be affected by fluoxetine would add great significance, because it would show whether the effects found are cell-specific.

The lack of physiological date (e.g., IPSP, minis,....) diminishes the relevance of the paper. At least, in the discussion, an stronger effort to link the molecular effects with functional properties should be done.

Minor points:

In the Method section it is stated that samples of both V1 cortices were taken, the ipsilateral and the contralateral to the deprived eye. In figures, it seems that only data for the contra cortex are reported, although this is never stated, not even in the captions. Could the authors clarify this?

Line 41-42: a small typo, "During the CP, experience-dependent plasticity is driven by visually evoked responses that dependent upon....."

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 2 (6)
eNeuro
Vol. 2, Issue 6
November/December 2015
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Effects of Fluoxetine and Visual Experience on Glutamatergic and GABAergic Synaptic Proteins in Adult Rat Visual Cortex
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Effects of Fluoxetine and Visual Experience on Glutamatergic and GABAergic Synaptic Proteins in Adult Rat Visual Cortex
Simon Beshara, Brett R. Beston, Joshua G. A. Pinto, Kathryn M. Murphy
eNeuro 24 December 2015, 2 (6) ENEURO.0126-15.2015; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0126-15.2015

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Effects of Fluoxetine and Visual Experience on Glutamatergic and GABAergic Synaptic Proteins in Adult Rat Visual Cortex
Simon Beshara, Brett R. Beston, Joshua G. A. Pinto, Kathryn M. Murphy
eNeuro 24 December 2015, 2 (6) ENEURO.0126-15.2015; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0126-15.2015
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • adult plasticity
  • amblyopia
  • fluoxetine
  • monocular deprivation
  • receptors
  • visual cortex

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

New Research

  • Heterozygous Dab1 null mutation disrupts neocortical and hippocampal development
  • The nasal solitary chemosensory cell signaling pathway triggers mouse avoidance behavior to inhaled nebulized irritants
  • Different control strategies drive interlimb differences in performance and adaptation during reaching movements in novel dynamics
Show more New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Different control strategies drive interlimb differences in performance and adaptation during reaching movements in novel dynamics
  • The nasal solitary chemosensory cell signaling pathway triggers mouse avoidance behavior to inhaled nebulized irritants
  • Taste-odor association learning alters the dynamics of intra-oral odor responses in the posterior piriform cortex of awake rats
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.