Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
Next
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

Experience-Dependent Neuroplasticity in the Hippocampus of Bilingual Young Adults

Federico Gallo, Toms Voits, Jason Rothman, Jubin Abutalebi, Yury Shtyrov and Andriy Myachykov
eNeuro 16 May 2025, 12 (6) ENEURO.0128-25.2025; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0128-25.2025
Federico Gallo
1UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø N-9037, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Toms Voits
2University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg 405 30, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Toms Voits
Jason Rothman
1UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø N-9037, Norway
3Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YW, United Kingdom
4Nebrija Research Center in Cognition, Madrid 28043, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jubin Abutalebi
1UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø N-9037, Norway
5Higher School of Economics, Moscow 109028, Russia
6University Vita Salute San Raffaele, Milan 20132, Italy
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yury Shtyrov
5Higher School of Economics, Moscow 109028, Russia
7Aarhus University, Aarhus 8000, Denmark
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andriy Myachykov
5Higher School of Economics, Moscow 109028, Russia
8University of Macau, Taipa, Macau 999078, China
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Models of experience-dependent neuroplasticity predict that the acquisition and extensive use of a new skill trigger a nonlinear trajectory of neurostructural modifications, where initial expansion of relevant brain areas subsequently (once the skill is acquired) gives way to volumetric renormalization. Such predictions also apply in the domain of language during learning and/or simultaneous management of two (or more) linguistic systems. In a sample of 69 young adult Russian–English bilinguals, we tested the hypothesis that individual differences in bilingual engagement nonlinearly correlate with normalized volume of the hippocampus—a key learning-related brain region particularly amenable to experience-dependent plasticity. Results revealed an inverted U-shape association between second language engagement and left hippocampal gray matter volume. The present results replicate and expand the findings from aging populations, showing a nonlinear pattern of structural hippocampal plasticity in healthy young adults. These findings support the role of bilingualism as a promoter of experience-dependent neuroplasticity.

  • bilingualism
  • expansion–renormalization
  • experience-dependent plasticity
  • hippocampus
  • MRI

Significance Statement

Bilingual experience has been associated with neurocognitive adaptations and linked to more favorable cognitive aging. The hippocampus, crucial in aging, has been previously shown to exhibit volumetric increases in response to language learning with some reports of nonlinear adaptations linked to bilingual experience. General models of neuroplasticity related to skill acquisition and bilingualism-specific models predict a morphological trajectory of volumetric expansions followed by renormalization of hippocampal volumes along the bilingual experience continuum. In this cross-sectional study we, for the first time, empirically tested this prediction in a sample of young individuals. In line with model predictions, our findings revealed an inverted U-shape relationship between second language engagement and left hippocampal volume, suggesting bilingualism as a source of experience-dependent neuroplasticity.

Introduction

External influences such as the acquisition and sustained use of new skills can reshape the human brain through “experience-dependent plasticity” (Lindenberger and Lövdén, 2019). The relationship between structural changes and learning is not linear (Wenger et al., 2017a). Wenger and colleagues (Wenger et al., 2017b) showed a trajectory of gray matter (GM) expansion in the initial stages of motor skill learning, followed by a subsequent renormalization to pretraining volumes. Importantly, this inverted U-shape dynamic emerged in the presence of continuous practice “and” improving task performance. This and other similar results led to the formulation (Lövdén et al., 2013) and refinement (Lindenberger and Lövdén, 2019) of the “exploration–selection–refinement (ESR)_model” of experience-dependent plasticity, positing a trajectory of expansion and renormalization of brain structure associated with learning. Initially, the brain explores combinations of available neuronal microcircuits (Yuste et al., 2024) that can perform the relevant task. Increased coordinated activity results in neurostructural modifications via dendritic branching/synaptogenesis and/or the generation/modification of astrocytes and microglia (Zatorre et al., 2012). Subsequently, dopamine-mediated processes contribute to the selection and stabilization of the most efficient circuits (Dolan and Dayan, 2013). Finally, any excess synaptic connections are eliminated via efficiency-based mechanisms such as synaptic pruning.

“Bi-/multilingualism” is a known source of neurocognitive adaptations (Bialystok et al., 2012), affecting both language-related and domain-general brain structure/function and associated cognitive abilities (Grundy et al., 2017). Prior to the last decade, relevant work in bilingualism heavily relied on between-group comparisons of bilinguals and monolinguals. However, the field is presently transitioning to a more nuanced and ecologically valid approach, operationalizing bilingual experience as a continuum and shifting to within-group analyses (Rothman et al., 2023). This has promoted the development of theoretical models delineating the time-course of neurocognitive modifications expected to emerge at different stages of the experience.

Drawing from the ESR model, the “dynamic restructuring model” (DRM; Pliatsikas, 2020) applies its framework to detail how the trajectory of bilingualism-induced neuroplastic changes is expected to unfold over time under sustained engagement from L2 learning to maintenance/use. The DRM predicts a trajectory of expansion and subsequent renormalization across several brain regions with increasing L2 competence. One such brain structure is the hippocampus, implicated in aging, memory, and other domains of cognition. Research confirms dual language engagement (learning or use) is linked to augmented hippocampal volume across different age groups with mixed findings regarding the laterality of the effect. Bilingualism-related increases in hippocampal volume have been reported in the right (Mårtensson et al., 2012; Bellander et al., 2016; DeLuca et al., 2019) and left hemisphere (Li et al., 2017) or bilaterally (Voits et al., 2022, 2024; Coulter et al., 2024). As some of the studies cited above show bilingualism-related hippocampal increases, without revealing any subsequent decreases with increased L2 expertise, the predictions of DRM for the hippocampus are at least partly supported by empirical data. However, the existing literature mostly employs linear modeling and thus is unable to directly test the predictions of DRM. The only study using nonlinear modeling for the hippocampus in MCI patients (Voits et al., 2024) indeed finds an inverted U-shape relationship between hippocampal volumes and L2 engagement. Thus, the underlying data may be nonlinear, but linear effects can still be observed. [It is also true that these studies vary in terms of what regressors they employ in their analyses and/or show as explanatory, e.g., age of acquisition (AoA), proficiency, etc. This alone complicates a meaningful comparison of the studies or anything that can clearly be drawn from them in their aggregate. Being that they are so few to begin with, it is not clear that when some show X and others Y or nothing at all, they are actually contradictory. The extent to which distinct factors are deterministic in specific contexts is an important question in the present discussion, yet it is a separate empirical one. Only work that is specifically designed to tease out if and, if so, why AoA over proficiency over continuous bilingual engagement measures actually have distinct or potentially clandestine overlapping coverage. Trying to address this across the existent studies is inappropriate beyond speculation precisely because the requisite control is not in place. The issue should be acknowledged a priori, and resolving it should form part of future work.]

Notwithstanding predictions by the DRM and general models of experience-dependent neuroplasticity, the hypothesized inverted U-shaped neuroplastic association between hippocampal volume and L2 engagement has not yet been investigated in bilingual young adults. The present study fills this gap using structural MRI with an atlas-based morphometry approach.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty-nine bilingual individuals (L1, Russian; L2, English; mean age, 22.81 years; SD, 3.4; 23 males) took part in the study (see Table 1 for an overview of demographic and language measures). All were right-handed, as indicated by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), and reported no psychiatric or neurological impairments. We assessed individual profiles for age, maximal educational attainment, and annual household income bands. The latter, used as a proxy of socioeconomic status, were adjusted based on the European Social Survey 2020 (ESS Round 10: European Social Survey Round 10 Data, 2020) to represent local standards. General intelligence was measured with a subset of the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices for adults (Raven, 2003). The study was approved by the local research ethics committee, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for sociodemographic and language background measures

Experimental design

Bilingual experience

Participants were presented with the Russian version of the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q; Marian et al., 2007) to evaluate their individual profiles along several dimensions of bilingual experience, including self-rated second language (L2) proficiency, L2 engagement, and AoA. To obtain an objective measure of L2 proficiency, participants also completed the online Cambridge test for adult learners (http://www.cambridgeenglish.org/test-your-english/general-english/).

MRI acquisition and preprocessing

T1-weighted images were acquired on a Philips Intera 1.5 T MRI scanner using the following parameters: TR, 25 ms; TE, 4.6 ms; flip angle, 30; FOV, 240 × 240; resolution, 1 × 1 × 1 mm; matrix, 256; TA, 5.35 min; mode, 3DFFE; and number of slices, 191. Bilateral hippocampal GMVs were extracted via a region-based morphometry routine implemented in CAT12 (Computational Anatomy Toolbox, https://neuro-jena.github.io/cat/) within the SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) software. Estimates of hippocampal volumes acquired with 1.5 T MRI scanners have been shown to be comparable with those acquired with 3 T scanners (Briellmann et al., 2001). Images were initially visually inspected to check for gross field distortions and movement artifacts, with no participants discarded as a result. The origin was then manually set to correspond to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line. Subsequently, CAT12 segmentation procedure was used to segment raw structural images into GM, white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). CAT12 outputs postsegmentation reports providing automatized image quality ratings, which were checked to confirm that all images were of sufficient quality. Images were all rated substantially above the sufficiency level. In particular, the weighted image quality rating was A for 53 participants, A− for 15 participants, and B for 1 participant. We then coregistered each image to the International Consortium for Brain Mapping European brain space template with the affine regularization routine. After coregistration, bilateral hippocampal GMVs were estimated in non-normalized native space using maximum tissue probability labels from the Neuromorphometrics Atlas (http://www.neuromorphometrics.com/) via an in-built CAT12 tool. Total intracranial volume (TIV) was computed by summing global volumes of different tissue classes—GM, WM, and CSF—in native space. Finally, individual hippocampal GMVs were normalized against TIV following the procedure presented in Jack et al. (1989), to control for individual differences in the brain size.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using Stata 17 (StataCorp, 2021). We used quadratic regressions to test whether variations in L2 proficiency, L2 AoA, and L2 engagement predicted variations in hippocampal GMVs. We evaluated which, among the sociodemographic measures we collected (i.e., fluid intelligence, years of education, sex, age, and socioeconomic status), to insert as covariates in our models. We expected fluid intelligence to affect both bilingual experience factors (BEFs) and hippocampal GMVs. Alternatively, we expected other variables, namely, sex, socioeconomic status, years of education, and age, to only influence hippocampal volumes and not BEFs. In particular, we expected these variables’ effect to be mediated by TIV, as there was no specific a priori motivation to expect them to influence hippocampal GMV particularly as opposed to whole-brain GMVs. The causal diagram illustrating our line of reasoning is visually represented in Figure 1. Since we had already adjusted our individual hippocampal volumes for TIV, and since these sociodemographic variables were not expected to influence our predictors of interest, i.e., BEFs, they were not included as covariates in the regression models to avoid noise inflation. Thus, our two full models—one with the left hippocampus GMV and the other with the right hippocampus GMV as the dependent variables—included L2 proficiency, L2 AoA, L2 engagement, and Raven's Matrices score as independent variables. To examine potential curvilinear relationships between BEFs and hippocampal GMVs, based on existing theoretical models (Pliatsikas, 2020), we interacted each BEF—specifically, L2 proficiency, L2 AoA, and L2 engagement—with itself in the regression models. This approach is used conventionally to allow the model to evaluate both the linear and quadratic contributions of each predictor simultaneously. The two quadratic regression models were tested via the regress function in Stata.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A causal diagram illustrating the reasons behind covariate structure choice for statistical models.

After model estimation, we ran a LASSO model selection procedure using a cross-validation selection method to identify the most parsimonious models, via Stata's lasso linear command.

Results

For the right hippocampus, the full model (R2 = 0.156; R2adjusted = 0.059) revealed no significant effect of BEFs, with Raven's Matrices score as the only significant predictor.

For the left hippocampus, the full model (R2 = 0.154; R2adjusted = 0.057) revealed a significant effect of both the linear (β = 0.016; p = 0.035) and the curvilinear (β = −0.0003; p = 0.019) L2 engagement predictors, together with Raven's Matrices score (β = −0.148; p = 0.007). The curvilinear term for L2 engagement had a more significant contribution than the linear one, suggesting a curvilinear relationship between L2 engagement and GMV of the left hippocampus.

For the right hippocampus, the LASSO procedure individuated as the best-fitting model (R2 = 0.083; R2adjusted = 0.07) one including Raven's Matrices score as the sole predictor. For the left hippocampus, the best-fitting model (R2 = 0.139; R2adjusted = 0.01) included Raven's Matrices Score and L2 engagement (linear, β = 0.014; p = 0.045; curvilinear, β = −0.0003; p = 0.025) as predictors. This confirmed the results of the full model analyses. After model estimation, we estimated the marginal effect of L2 engagement on left hippocampal GMV via Stata's margins command. The effect plot revealed that the relationship followed an inverted U-shape (Fig. 2).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

A plot of the curvilinear relationship between daily L2 engagement and left hippocampal GMV (top panel). The left hippocampus shown on the bottom panel on an MNI template for illustrative purposes.

Discussion

Herein, we examined the association between individual differences in bilingual experience and variations in GMVs of the hippocampus. We hypothesized that this relationship would follow an inverted U-shaped pattern, consistent with the DRM and broader frameworks of experience-dependent neuroplasticity. Analyses confirmed our hypothesis: we observed an inverted U-shaped relationship between L2 engagement and GMV of the left hippocampus. This finding is in accord with theoretical models on the lifelong trajectory of bilingualism-related neuroplasticity. While we have introduced the DRM framework (Pliatsikas, 2020) above, it is worth unpacking it more explicitly at this juncture. Consider that the DRM predicts differential neural adaptations based on the time-course of bilingual experience and the relative variations in cognitive effort imposed by language learning and ensuing control demands. In other words, the DRM is predicated on the idea that the brain adapts continuously in response to bilingualism over time. The model proposes three stages—initial exposure (language learning), consolidation (development), and peak efficiency (maintenance after acquisition over time). Specifically for the hippocampus, the DRM predicts initial GMV increases followed by eventual renormalization (return to baseline) during the consolidation stage with potentially further reductions of GMV for those individuals who have reached peak efficiency. Crucially, however, the DRM also predicts measurably maintained or enhanced efficiency during the latter two stages, despite a structure that is renormalized or even reduced. As such, the hippocampus would be expected to increase in volume at initial stages of L2 acquisition in order to cope with cognitive demands associated with the novel task of language control. This novel cognitive demand would induce the hippocampus to undergo structural changes via the formation of new synaptic connections. Subsequently, increasing bilingual experience would lead to an increase of hippocampal functional efficiency. This increased functional efficiency, in turn, renders the previously accumulated “extra” structural resources no longer necessary for optimized language control. As consolidation of the learning process sets in, the surplus connections would thus be eliminated via synaptic pruning. This process would result in the hippocampal structural substrate to return to prebilingualism levels in gross volumetric terms (or even reduce), while its enhanced synaptic connectivity has been reorganized toward higher efficiency. Admittedly, the best way to capture this process would be in a longitudinal design. Nevertheless, the U-shaped pattern across our cross-sectional approach by relative exposure maps well onto these three hypothesized stages. It also replicates the pattern found in older bilingual populations with MCI (Voits et al., 2024) as well as data demonstrating similar volumetric trajectories in other cortical and subcortical regions of the language control/executive network (Marin-Marin et al., 2022; Gallo et al., 2023; Korenar et al., 2023; Yee et al., 2024). As such, we interpret this pattern as consistent with the DRM's predictions, highlighting what is at its core a nonlinear trajectory of economy-driven, experiential-based adaptation.

While we had not made any specific predictions regarding the lateralization of any effect, it must be noted that we only found a relationship between bilingual experience and the GMV of the left hippocampus. Indeed, previous literature in the neurocognition of bilingualism field has reported mixed findings regarding lateralization: bilingual experience has been reported to affect, similar to the present finding, the left hippocampus (Li et al., 2017) but also the right hippocampus (Mårtensson et al., 2012; Bellander et al., 2016) as well as both (DeLuca et al., 2019; Voits et al., 2022, 2024). Despite this, we would submit that the present lateralized pattern is not surprising. It is well known that hippocampal function is lateralized in healthy individuals, with the left hippocampus being dominant for linguistic cognitive performance (Nemati et al., 2023) and verbal memory (Ezzati et al., 2016). The left medial temporal lobe and the left hippocampus also seem to have a critical role in determining the hemispheric lateralization of language in general (Liégeois et al., 2004). With this in mind, it is not surprising that in the present sample of young healthy adults, the left hippocampus is specifically implicated. Indeed, in at least some of previous studies indicating either a bilateral or right lateralized hippocampus effect, it is not clear if this pattern is produced by dual language experience alone. For example, the very design of the Bellander et al. (2016) study involved high-intensity lexical learning. Accordingly, their findings might not reflect an effect of language experience per se but rather incipient, intense learning more generally. In the Mårtensson et al. (2012) study, the participants were simultaneous interpreters during rigorous army-based training which involved extensive lexicon memorizing routines; such an engagement in dual language use is clearly atypical in several ways, both in terms of an incomparable level of its intensity to everyday bilingual experiences (as in our study) and in how the brain is being taxed more generally. It is not clear that bilingual switching under such a scenario uses the same mechanisms, in part or entirely, as more mundane, yet typical bilingualism in more real-world contexts. As such, the studies mentioned above potentially capture elements taking place beyond (or in parallel to) the exponents of dual language engagement shared by the participants in the current study. Interestingly, in line with our results, Abutalebi et al. (2007) reported functional adaptation of the left hippocampus in bilinguals during literacy training. During an fMRI experiment, bilingual participants, who were only literate in their L2, learned to read words in their native language, which was reflected by significant activity of the left hippocampus and left caudate but only at the initial stages. At more proficient stages of the literacy acquisition process, these bilinguals would cease to rely on their left hippocampus.

As a final point, it is worth keeping in mind that substantial evidence points to bilingualism being a factor that contributes to better-than-expected longevity in the later years. Thus, bilingual experience is hypothesized to contribute to greater “reserve” (Perani and Abutalebi, 2015; Bialystok, 2021; Gallo et al., 2022)—a concept devised to account for the high variability observed in individual trajectories of cognitive aging. Reserve can be broadly defined as the individual capacity to resist adverse consequences of cognitive aging (Stern, 2009; Barulli and Stern, 2013; Stern et al., 2020). As such, it originates from cognitively challenging life experiences which reinforce one's neural and cognitive resources. In other words, reserve can be seen as the result of experience-dependent plasticity. Although the effects of reserve are mainly observable in senescence, its accrual is thought to take place over the course of the lifespan, starting from youth (Tucker and Stern, 2011). Early-life experiences appear to play a crucial role in shaping health and aging outcomes later in life (de Rooij, 2022), and higher general cognitive ability in youth is associated with greater reserve in older age, indicating that reserve-related compensatory mechanisms may depend, at least in part, on early-life experiences (Panico et al., 2023; Schwarz et al., 2024). In line with these findings, Lupien et al. (2007) have reported a relationship between early hippocampal volume and neurocognitive outcomes in late life stages. In this vein, our results provide an indication on the potential origin of the bilingualism-induced reserve effects extensively observed in the literature. Indeed, recall that both the DRM and the ESR predict, as a result of the inverted U-shaped neuroplastic trajectory, an increase in the efficiency of available neural resources related with the relevant cognitive ability. This augmented efficiency has been put forward as one of the mechanisms underlying the protective effects exerted by reserve during the aging process (Barulli and Stern, 2013). Further supporting this interpretation is the observation that the present results parallel previous findings of a curvilinear relationship between bilingual experience and hippocampal volumes in older adults with MCI (Voits et al., 2024).

In summary, the present neuroimaging findings add to the body of evidence showing that cognitive demands associated with dual language acquisition and control lead to neurostructural adaptations in the brain, documented here for the hippocampus—a brain structure critical for a vast array of cognitive functions. The present study underscores the role of bilingualism as a powerful promoter of experience-dependent neuroplasticity.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • The article was prepared in the framework of a research grant funded by the Ministry of Science and Higher Education of the Russian Federation (Grant ID, 075-15-2022-325). F.G. has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon Europe research and innovation program under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie Grant Agreement Number 101106069. J.A. and J.R. acknowledge funding by the Trond Mohn Foundation, Grant Number TMS2023UiT01.

  • ↵*F.G. and T.V. contributed equally to this work.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abutalebi J,
    2. Keim R,
    3. Brambati SM,
    4. Tettamanti M,
    5. Cappa SF,
    6. De Bleser R,
    7. Perani D
    (2007) Late acquisition of literacy in a native language. Hum Brain Mapp 28:19–33. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20240 pmid:16639742
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Barulli D,
    2. Stern Y
    (2013) Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn Sci 17:502–509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012 pmid:24018144
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bellander M,
    2. Berggren R,
    3. Mårtensson J,
    4. Brehmer Y,
    5. Wenger E,
    6. Li T-Q,
    7. Bodammer NC,
    8. Shing Y-L,
    9. Werkle-Bergner M,
    10. Lövdén M
    (2016) Behavioral correlates of changes in hippocampal gray matter structure during acquisition of foreign vocabulary. Neuroimage 131:205–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bialystok E,
    2. Craik FIM,
    3. Luk G
    (2012) Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends Cogn Sci 16:240–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001 pmid:22464592
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bialystok E
    (2021) Bilingualism: pathway to cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn Sci 25:355–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.003 pmid:33771449
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Briellmann RS,
    2. Syngeniotis A,
    3. Jackson GD
    (2001) Comparison of hippocampal volumetry at 1.5 Tesla and at 3 Tesla. Epilepsia 42:1021–1024. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1528-1157.2001.0420081021.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Coulter K,
    2. Phillips NA
    (2024) Bilinguals show evidence of brain maintenance in Alzheimer’s disease. Biling: Lang Cogn 27:1029–1038. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728924000221
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    1. DeLuca V,
    2. Rothman J,
    3. Pliatsikas C
    (2019) Linguistic immersion and structural effects on the bilingual brain: a longitudinal study. Biling: Lang Cogn 22:1160–1175. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000883
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    1. de Rooij SR
    (2022) Are brain and cognitive reserve shaped by early life circumstances? Front Neurosci 16:825811. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.825811 pmid:35784851
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Dolan RJ,
    2. Dayan P
    (2013) Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron 80:312–325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.007 pmid:24139036
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Ezzati A,
    2. Katz MJ,
    3. Zammit AR,
    4. Lipton ML,
    5. Zimmerman ME,
    6. Sliwinski MJ,
    7. Lipton RB
    (2016) Differential association of left and right hippocampal volumes with verbal episodic and spatial memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia 93:380–385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.016 pmid:27542320
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Gallo F,
    2. DeLuca V,
    3. Prystauka Y,
    4. Voits T,
    5. Rothman J,
    6. Abutalebi J
    (2022) Bilingualism and aging: implications for (delaying) neurocognitive decline. Front Hum Neurosci 16:819105. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.819105 pmid:35185498
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Gallo F,
    2. Terekhina L,
    3. Shtyrov Y,
    4. Myachykov A
    (2023) Neuroplasticity and cognitive reserve effects in the caudate nucleus of young bilingual adults. Biling: Lang Cogn 27:107–116. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000457
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    1. Grundy JG,
    2. Anderson JAE,
    3. Bialystok E
    (2017) Neural correlates of cognitive processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1396:183–201. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13333 pmid:28415142
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Jack CR,
    2. Twomey CK,
    3. Zinsmeister AR,
    4. Sharbrough FW,
    5. Petersen RC,
    6. Cascino GD
    (1989) Anterior temporal lobes and hippocampal formations: normative volumetric measurements from MR images in young adults. Radiology 172:549–554. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiology.172.2.2748838
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Korenar M,
    2. Treffers-Daller J,
    3. Pliatsikas C
    (2023) Dynamic effects of bilingualism on brain structure map onto general principles of experience-based neuroplasticity. Sci Rep 13:3428. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-30326-3 pmid:36854883
    OpenUrlPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Li L,
    2. Abutalebi J,
    3. Emmorey K,
    4. Gong G,
    5. Yan X,
    6. Feng X,
    7. Zou L,
    8. Ding G
    (2017) How bilingualism protects the brain from aging: insights from bimodal bilinguals. Hum Brain Mapp 38:4109–4124. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23652 pmid:28513102
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Liégeois F,
    2. Connelly A,
    3. Cross JH,
    4. Boyd SG,
    5. Gadian DG,
    6. Vargha-Khadem F,
    7. Baldeweg T
    (2004) Language reorganization in children with early-onset lesions of the left hemisphere: an fMRI study. Brain 127:1229–1236. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh159
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Lindenberger U,
    2. Lövdén M
    (2019) Brain plasticity in human lifespan development: the exploration–selection–refinement model. Annu Rev Dev Psychol 1:197–222. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-085229
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. ↵
    1. Lövdén M,
    2. Wenger E,
    3. Mårtensson J,
    4. Lindenberger U,
    5. Bäckman L
    (2013) Structural brain plasticity in adult learning and development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37:2296–2310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Lupien SJ,
    2. Evans A,
    3. Lord C,
    4. Miles J,
    5. Pruessner M,
    6. Pike B,
    7. Pruessner JC
    (2007) Hippocampal volume is as variable in young as in older adults: implications for the notion of hippocampal atrophy in humans. Neuroimage 34:479–485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.041
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Marian V,
    2. Blumenfeld HK,
    3. Kaushanskaya M
    (2007) The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. J Speech Lang Hear Res 50:940–967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Marin-Marin L,
    2. Costumero V,
    3. Ávila C,
    4. Pliatsikas C
    (2022) Dynamic effects of immersive bilingualism on cortical and subcortical grey matter volumes. Front Psychol 13:886222. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.886222 pmid:35586234
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Mårtensson J,
    2. Eriksson J,
    3. Bodammer NC,
    4. Lindgren M,
    5. Johansson M,
    6. Nyberg L,
    7. Lövdén M
    (2012) Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. Neuroimage 63:240–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Nemati SS,
    2. Sadeghi L,
    3. Dehghan G,
    4. Sheibani N
    (2023) Lateralization of the hippocampus: a review of molecular, functional, and physiological properties in health and disease. Behav Brain Res 454:114657. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2023.114657
    OpenUrl
  26. ↵
    1. Oldfield RC
    (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Panico F,
    2. Sagliano L,
    3. Magliacano A,
    4. Santangelo G,
    5. Trojano L
    (2023) The relationship between cognitive reserve and cognition in healthy adults: a systematic review. Curr Psychol 42:24751–24763. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03523-y
    OpenUrl
  28. ↵
    1. Perani D,
    2. Abutalebi J
    (2015) Bilingualism, dementia, cognitive and neural reserve. Curr Opin Neurol 28:618–625. https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0000000000000267
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. ↵
    1. Pliatsikas C
    (2020) Understanding structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: the dynamic restructuring model. Biling: Lang Cogn 23:459–471. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000130
    OpenUrl
  30. ↵
    1. Raven JJ
    (2003) Raven progressive matrices. In: Handbook of nonverbal assessment (McCallum RS, ed), pp 223–237. Boston, MA: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0153-4_11
  31. ↵
    1. Rothman J,
    2. Bayram F,
    3. DeLuca V,
    4. Di Pisa G,
    5. Dunabeitia JA,
    6. Gharibi K,
    7. Hao J,
    8. Kolb N,
    9. Kubota M,
    10. Kupisch T
    (2023) Monolingual comparative normativity in bilingualism research is out of “control”: arguments and alternatives. Appl Psycholinguist 44:316–329. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716422000315
    OpenUrl
  32. ↵
    1. Schwarz C,
    2. Franz CE,
    3. Kremen WS,
    4. Vuoksimaa E
    (2024) Reserve, resilience and maintenance of episodic memory and other cognitive functions in aging. Neurobiol Aging 140:60–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2024.04.011
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    StataCorp (2021) Stata statistical software. Release 17. Available at: https://www.stata.com/support/faqs/resources/citing-software-documentation-faqs/
  34. ↵
    1. Stern Y
    (2009) Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47:2015–2028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004 pmid:19467352
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Stern Y, et al.
    (2020) Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement 16:1305–1311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219 pmid:30222945
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Tucker AM,
    2. Stern Y
    (2011) Cognitive reserve in aging. Curr Alzheimer Res 8:354–360. https://doi.org/10.2174/156720511795745320 pmid:21222591
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Voits T,
    2. Robson H,
    3. Rothman J,
    4. Pliatsikas C
    (2022) The effects of bilingualism on hippocampal volume in ageing bilinguals. Brain Struct Funct 227:979–994. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-021-02436-z pmid:34985602
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Voits T, et al.
    (2024) Hippocampal adaptations in mild cognitive impairment patients are modulated by bilingual language experiences. Bilingualism 27:263–273. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000354
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    1. Wenger E,
    2. Brozzoli C,
    3. Lindenberger U,
    4. Lövdén M
    (2017a) Expansion and renormalization of human brain structure during skill acquisition. Trends Cogn Sci 21:930–939. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.008 pmid:29149999
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Wenger E,
    2. Kühn S,
    3. Verrel J,
    4. Mårtensson J,
    5. Bodammer NC,
    6. Lindenberger U,
    7. Lövdén M
    (2017b) Repeated structural imaging reveals nonlinear progression of experience-dependent volume changes in human motor cortex. Cereb Cortex 27:2911–2925. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw141
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Yee J,
    2. Yap NT,
    3. Korenar M,
    4. Saddy JD,
    5. Pliatsikas C
    (2024) Subcortical restructuring as a function of multilingualism: insights from monolinguals, bilinguals, trilinguals and quadrilinguals. Biling: Lang Cogn 27:715–728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672892300086X
    OpenUrl
  42. ↵
    1. Yuste R,
    2. Cossart R,
    3. Yaksi E
    (2024) Neuronal ensembles: building blocks of neural circuits. Neuron 112:875–892. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2023.12.008 pmid:38262413
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Zatorre RJ,
    2. Fields RD,
    3. Johansen-Berg H
    (2012) Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat Neurosci 15:528–536. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3045 pmid:22426254
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: James Howard, Brandeis University

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: John Anderson. Note: If this manuscript was transferred from JNeurosci and a decision was made to accept the manuscript without peer review, a brief statement to this effect will instead be what is listed below.

This manuscript examines nonlinear contributions to hippocampal gray matter volume in aging. The paper expands work on the role of hippocampal plasticity in bilingualism and effects related to experience-dependent neuroplasticity. The paper is well-written, with carefully conducted analyses, though the cross-sectional nature of the study remains a limitation. This is a revised manuscript and the authors have addressed all the points raised and made significant changes to the manuscript in response to the reviewers' feedback. We particularly appreciate the care the authors took in revising the introduction and the statistics - including the directed acyclic graph as a justification for inclusion of necessary covariates was a nice touch.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 12 (6)
eNeuro
Vol. 12, Issue 6
June 2025
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Experience-Dependent Neuroplasticity in the Hippocampus of Bilingual Young Adults
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Experience-Dependent Neuroplasticity in the Hippocampus of Bilingual Young Adults
Federico Gallo, Toms Voits, Jason Rothman, Jubin Abutalebi, Yury Shtyrov, Andriy Myachykov
eNeuro 16 May 2025, 12 (6) ENEURO.0128-25.2025; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0128-25.2025

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Experience-Dependent Neuroplasticity in the Hippocampus of Bilingual Young Adults
Federico Gallo, Toms Voits, Jason Rothman, Jubin Abutalebi, Yury Shtyrov, Andriy Myachykov
eNeuro 16 May 2025, 12 (6) ENEURO.0128-25.2025; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0128-25.2025
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • bilingualism
  • expansion–renormalization
  • experience-dependent plasticity
  • hippocampus
  • MRI

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Deletion of endocannabinoid synthesizing enzyme DAGLα in Pcp2-positive cerebellar Purkinje cells decreases depolarization-induced short-term synaptic plasticity, reduces social preference, and heightens anxiety
  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Visual Stimulation Under 4 Hz, Not at 10 Hz, Generates the Highest-Amplitude Frequency-Tagged Responses of the Human Brain: Understanding the Effect of Stimulation Frequency
  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.