Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

Touchscreen Response Precision Is Sensitive to the Explore/Exploit Trade-off

Dana Mueller, Erin Giglio, Cathy S. Chen, Aspen Holm, R. Becket Ebitz and Nicola M. Grissom
eNeuro 17 April 2025, 12 (5) ENEURO.0538-24.2025; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0538-24.2025
Dana Mueller
1Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Erin Giglio
1Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cathy S. Chen
1Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Cathy S. Chen
Aspen Holm
1Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Aspen Holm
R. Becket Ebitz
2Department of Neuroscience, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec H3T 1J4, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nicola M. Grissom
1Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Exploit states and female sex reduce action variability during decision making. a, schematic depicting the timeline of a single trial. White squares indicate left/right spatial choice. b, An example of touchscreen responses from one animal and one session, where light purple indicates explore touches and dark purple indicates exploit touches. c, Schematic depicting the HMM and labeling explore trials along an example two-arm spatial restless bandit probability walk. Orange traces indicate the probability and choices of left side touches. Blue traces indicate the probability and choices of right side touches. Gray-shaded regions indicate HMM-labeled explore trials. d, Schematic of Euclidean distance where the distance is calculated between Touch 1 and Touch 2, Touch 2 and Touch 3, Touch 3 and Touch 4, and so on. Shown here are possible left/right touches in blue and the distance relationship from one to another represented by black lines. e, Average Euclidean distance split by state (left) and sex (right). Exploit touches and females had significantly reduced Euclidean distance. Light purple indicates distance between explore touches, and dark purple indicates distance between exploit touches. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. In violin graphs, individual data points are data from one mouse averaged across all sessions. f, Schematic of Mahalanobis distance where the individual data points are measured from the overall centroid of the dataset. Shown here are possible left/right Mahalanobis clusters (light blue circles) and centroids (stars) and the Mahalanobis distance relationship from each touch (darker blue circles) in a cluster to the centroid represented by black lines. g, Average Mahalanobis distance split by state (left) and sex (right). Exploit touches had significantly reduced Mahalanobis distance. Light purple indicates Mahalanobis distance between explore touches, and dark purple indicates Mahalanobis distance between exploit touches. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. h, Schematic of distance from the center of the screen where touch distance from both left and right choice apertures is measured from the midpoint of the operant screen. Shown here are possible left/right touches in blue and the distance of each from the center of the touchscreen indicated by black lines. i, Average distance from the center of the screen split by state (left) and sex (right). Explore touches were significantly closer to the center of the screen. Light purple indicates distance from the center of the screen for explore touches, and dark purple indicates distance from the center of the screen for exploit touches. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. j, Schematic of response time calculation which is based on the difference between screen display and choice time (nosepoke). k, Average choice response time split by state (left) and sex (right). Exploit touches and female sex significantly reduced latency to respond. Light purple indicates response time for explore touches, and dark purple indicates response time for exploit touches. Red indicates female and blue indicates male mice. For simplicity of visualization, all plots are averages across trials and sessions, so that each individual data point plotted represents the overall average for a mouse. Significance throughout this paper is represented in the following way: *p < 0.05 and *p > 0.01; **p < 0.01 and **p ≥ 0.001; ***p < 0.001. Violin graphs depict median and quartiles of the dataset.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Previous reward reduces action variability independently from explore/exploit balance or female sex. a, Average Euclidean distance comparing rewarded versus nonrewarded trials. Touches following rewarded trials had significantly reduced Euclidean distance. Light green indicates distance between nonrewarded touches, and dark green indicates distance between rewarded touches. In violin graphs, individual data points are data from one mouse averaged across all sessions. b, Average Euclidean distance for rewarded (left) and nonrewarded (right) trials split by state and sex. Exploit touches and females had significantly reduced Euclidean distance. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. c, Average Mahalanobis distance comparing rewarded versus nonrewarded trials. Touches following rewarded trials had significantly reduced Mahalanobis distance. Light green indicates Mahalanobis distance between nonrewarded touches, and dark green indicates Mahalanobis distance between rewarded touches. d, Average Mahalanobis distance for rewarded (left) and nonrewarded (right) trials split by state and sex. Exploit touches had significantly reduced Mahalanobis distance. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. *p < 0.05 and *p > 0.01; **p < 0.01 and **p ≥ 0.001; ***p < 0.001. Violin graphs depict median and quartiles of the dataset.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Bouts of exploit choices are closer to each other and occupy a smaller area than bouts of explore choices. a, An example portion of a probability walk with explore trials shaded gray, illustrating state bouts and transitions between bouts. A “bout” is a period of touches within one HMM-defined behavioral state. Transitions between bouts are referenced with arrows as the state switches from explore to exploit or exploit to explore throughout. b, The average number of bouts per session split by state and sex. No significant sex difference in the number of bouts per session. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. In violin graphs, individual data points are data from one mouse averaged across all sessions. c, An example 2D contour plot from Plotly Graphing Libraries fit over our nosepoke touch locations to visualize the density and range of choice responding. Small gray circles are nosepoke touches within the bout of response data. The color map corresponds with density of data points within each bin, where the darkest purple (outer bin) is the least dense contour bin, which is used to calculate the area and perimeter of the bout. d, The average area of bouts split by state and sex. Exploit touches and females had significantly reduced area. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. e, Average perimeter of bouts split by state and sex. Exploit touches and females had significantly reduced perimeter. f, Schematic depicting centroid shifts, where the Euclidean distance between two successive Mahalanobis centroids is calculated. Stars indicate example centroids associated with bouts, and black lines indicate the distance calculations between those centroids. g, Centroid shifts split by state and sex. Centroid shifts were significantly smaller for exploit bouts. Red indicates female, and blue indicates male mice. *p < 0.05 and *p > 0.01; **p < 0.01 and **p ≥ 0.001; ***p < 0.001. Violin graphs depict median and quartiles of the dataset.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Generalized linear mixed models Equations 1–12

    VariableCoded asGLMM seriesDependent variableFigureBeta Coeff.P value
    dist_mm∼sex×state+(state|mouseID),
    (state|mouseID)RandomEuclideandist_mm1e10.426323.52 × 10−11
    SexCategoricalEuclideandist_mm1e2.6684320.012418
    StateCategoricalEuclideandist_mm1e4.4793420.000118
    Sex × stateInteractionEuclideandist_mm1e−1.636850.112654
    mahalanobis∼state+(state|mouseID),
    (state|mouseID)RandomMahalanobismahalanobis1g42.470321.27 × 10−29
    StateCategoricalMahalanobismahalanobis1g6.1322012.03 × 10−6
    center_dist_mm∼sextimesstate+(state|mouseID),
    (state|mouseID)RandomDistance From Centercent_dist_mm1i24.757771.70 × 10−21
    SexCategoricalDistance From Centercent_dist_mm1i0.2995440.766587
    StateCategoricalDistance From Centercent_dist_mm1i−4.725465.15 × 10−5
    Sex × stateInteractionDistance From Centercent_dist_mm1i0.7671970.448969
    dist_mm∼reward+(reward|mouseID)+(state|mouseID)+sextimesstate,
    (state|mouseID)RandomEuclideandist_mm2a,b11.74351.04 × 10−12
    SexCategoricalEuclideandist_mm2b2.7793540.009519
    StateCategoricalEuclideandist_mm2a−0.302980.764276
    RewardCategoricalEuclideandist_mm2a,b−6.961199.88 × 10−8
    Sex × stateInteractionEuclideandist_mm2a,b−2.171840.038554
    mahalanobis∼reward+state+(state|mouseID)+(reward|mouseID),
    (reward|mouseID)RandomMahalanobismahalanobis2c,d29.751933.78 × 10−38
    StateCategoricalMahalanobismahalanobis2c4.3836630.000181
    RewardCategoricalMahalanobismahalanobis2c,d−4.010220.000364
    loc_dist2_lastLoc∼statetimessextimes(1|mouseID),
    (1|mouseID)RandomLocal centroid distanceloc_dist2_lastLoc3g10.738841.18 × 10−11
    SexCategoricalLocal centroid distanceloc_dist2_lastLoc3g−0.011490.99088
    StateCategoricalLocal centroid distanceloc_dist2_lastLoc3g8.9642874.03 × 10−19
    State × sexInteractionLocal centroid distanceloc_dist2_lastLoc3g2.0783140.03772
    contour_area∼statetimessex+(1|mouseID),
    (1|mouseID)RandomAverage Bout Areacontour_area3d5.0587011.95 × 10−5
    SexCategoricalAverage Bout Areacontour_area3d4.7310834.57 × 10−5
    StateCategoricalAverage Bout Areacontour_area3d−2.6610.00781
    State × sexInteractionAverage Bout Areacontour_area3d−2.875230.004051
    contour_perimeter∼statetimessex×+(1|mouseID),
    (1|mouseID)RandomAverage Bout Perimetercontour_perimeter3e13.831091.05 × 10−14
    SexCategoricalAverage Bout Perimetercontour_perimeter3e3.6434660.000928
    StateCategoricalAverage Bout Perimetercontour_perimeter3e−7.160558.93 × 10−13
    State × sexInteractionAverage Bout Perimetercontour_perimeter3e−1.535370.124742
    RT∼(state|mouseID)+state+sex,
    (state|mouseID)RandomResponse LatencyRT1k8.7310391.23 × 10−9
    SexCategoricalResponse LatencyRT1k3.8915230.000528
    StateCategoricalResponse LatencyRT1k4.543628.54 × 10−5
    distance∼alpha+sex+(1|mouseID),
    (1|mouseID)RandomLearning RatealphaN/A11.520811.76 × 10−14
    SexCategoricalLearning RatealphaN/A2.5121880.017736
    AlphaContinuousLearning RatealphaN/A−2.010350.045508
    num_explore∼sex+(1|mouseID),
    (1|mouseID)RandomBout numbernum_explore3b12.129886.87 × 10−13
    SexCategoricalBout numbernum_explore3b−2.17840.037647
    num_exploit∼sex+(1|mouseID),
    (1|mouseID)RandomBout numbernum_exploit3b10.750281.23 × 10−11
    SexCategoricalBout numbernum_exploit3b−2.367330.024802
    • Table including equations, variables, figure references, beta coefficients, and p values for all included statistics.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 12 (5)
eNeuro
Vol. 12, Issue 5
May 2025
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Touchscreen Response Precision Is Sensitive to the Explore/Exploit Trade-off
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Touchscreen Response Precision Is Sensitive to the Explore/Exploit Trade-off
Dana Mueller, Erin Giglio, Cathy S. Chen, Aspen Holm, R. Becket Ebitz, Nicola M. Grissom
eNeuro 17 April 2025, 12 (5) ENEURO.0538-24.2025; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0538-24.2025

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Touchscreen Response Precision Is Sensitive to the Explore/Exploit Trade-off
Dana Mueller, Erin Giglio, Cathy S. Chen, Aspen Holm, R. Becket Ebitz, Nicola M. Grissom
eNeuro 17 April 2025, 12 (5) ENEURO.0538-24.2025; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0538-24.2025
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • bandit
  • hidden Markov model (HMM)
  • reinforcement learning
  • sex differences
  • touchscreen

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Deletion of endocannabinoid synthesizing enzyme DAGLα in Pcp2-positive cerebellar Purkinje cells decreases depolarization-induced short-term synaptic plasticity, reduces social preference, and heightens anxiety
  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
  • Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Encodes the Trace Period during Appetitive Pavlovian Conditioning
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.