Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

Pharmacological Enhancement of Dopamine Neurotransmission Does Not Affect Illusory Pattern Perception

Elke Smith, Simon Michalski, Kilian Knauth, Deniz Tuzsus, Hendrik Theis, Thilo van Eimeren and Jan Peters
eNeuro 12 July 2024, 11 (7) ENEURO.0465-23.2024; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0465-23.2024
Elke Smith
1Department of Psychology, Biological Psychology, University of Cologne, Cologne 50969, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Elke Smith
Simon Michalski
1Department of Psychology, Biological Psychology, University of Cologne, Cologne 50969, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kilian Knauth
1Department of Psychology, Biological Psychology, University of Cologne, Cologne 50969, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Deniz Tuzsus
1Department of Psychology, Biological Psychology, University of Cologne, Cologne 50969, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hendrik Theis
2Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Cologne, Department of Nuclear Medicine, University of Cologne, 50937 Cologne, Germany
3Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Cologne, Department of Neurology, University of Cologne, 50937 Cologne, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thilo van Eimeren
2Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Cologne, Department of Nuclear Medicine, University of Cologne, 50937 Cologne, Germany
3Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Cologne, Department of Neurology, University of Cologne, 50937 Cologne, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jan Peters
1Department of Psychology, Biological Psychology, University of Cologne, Cologne 50969, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jan Peters
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Psychotic symptoms and delusional beliefs have been linked to dopamine transmission in both healthy and clinical samples and are assumed to result at least in part from perceiving illusory patterns in noise. However, the existing literature on the role of dopamine in detecting patterns in noise is inconclusive. To address this issue, we assessed the effect of manipulating dopaminergic neurotransmission on illusory pattern perception in healthy individuals (n = 48, n = 19 female) in a double-blind placebo-controlled within-subjects design (see preregistration at https://osf.io/a4k9j/). We predicted individuals on versus off ʟ-DOPA to be more likely to perceive illusory patterns, specifically objects in images containing only noise. Using a signal detection model, however, we found no credible evidence that ʟ-DOPA compared with placebo increased false alarm rates. Further, ʟ-DOPA did not reliably modulate measures of accuracy, discrimination sensitivity, and response bias. In all cases, Bayesian statistics revealed strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. The task design followed previous work on illusory pattern perception and comprised a limited number of items per condition. The results therefore need to be interpreted with caution, as power was limited. Future studies should address illusory pattern perception using more items and take into account potential dose-dependent effects and differential effects in healthy versus clinical samples.

  • discrimination sensitivity
  • dopamine
  • ʟ-DOPA
  • pattern perception

Significance Statement

Psychosis and delusional belief have been linked to dopamine transmission in healthy and clinical samples and are assumed to partly result from perceiving illusory patterns in noise. However, the findings on the role of dopamine in detecting illusory patterns are inconclusive. To address this, we assessed the effect of enhancing dopamine transmission on illusory pattern perception in healthy individuals. Our hypothesis that enhancing dopamine transmission would increase participants’ tendency to perceive illusory patterns in noise was not confirmed. This null effect suggests that earlier findings may be less robust than previously thought and that the relationship between dopamine and illusory pattern perception may be subject to dose-dependent effects and that there may be differential effects in healthy versus clinical samples.

Introduction

Detecting relationships between stimuli or events enables individuals to make predictions for the future. Recent theories conceive the brain as a probabilistic inference system, predicting events and causes of sensory input to enable successful interaction with the environment (Friston and Stephan, 2007). Perceiving relationships between unrelated stimuli and patterns in noise may be maladaptive, however, since this prevents the formation of accurate representations. Delusional belief and psychotic symptoms are assumed to result from aberrant changes in dopaminergic signaling. Several studies point toward a link between dopamine transmission and delusional belief in healthy and clinical samples. For instance, manipulating dopamine transmission with haloperidol and ʟ-DOPA in controls changed social attributions of harmful intent (Barnby et al., 2020). Further, the link between dopamine and delusions is supported by the effects of antipsychotics, which alleviate psychotic symptoms by antagonizing D2 dopamine receptors (Kaar et al., 2020) and by PET imaging studies showing dysregulated dopamine synthesis in individuals suffering from delusions and schizophrenia (Cheng et al., 2020; for a review, see Rigney et al., 2021).

According to prevailing theories, this link is imparted by dopamine's role in the encoding of reward prediction errors (RPEs) and the assignment of aberrant salience. Within the framework of the prediction error minimization theory, the brain seeks to minimize the discrepancy between predicted and actual input (Clark, 2013). The theory has been widely adopted for describing decision-making in various domains, including reward-based learning (Schultz, 2016), perceptual (Bell et al., 2016), and social inference (de Bruin and Michael, 2021). A large body of research suggests that dopamine modulates striatal coding of prediction errors to enable learning (Pessiglione et al., 2006; Glimcher, 2011; Schlagenhauf et al., 2013; Schultz, 2016; Basanisi et al., 2023). Imaging studies in human schizophrenic participants have reported striatal dysfunction during learning (Schlagenhauf et al., 2014; Katthagen et al., 2020) and aberrant learning from feedback accompanied by altered EEG correlates (Kirschner et al., 2024). In monkeys, midbrain dopamine neurons have been found to code reward size but also uncertainty or confidence during perceptual decision-making (de Lafuente and Romo, 2011; Lak et al., 2017).

The aberrant salience framework of psychosis describes delusions as a dysfunctional computational mechanism at the neural level within a Bayesian predictive coding framework (Kapur, 2003). Representations are formed by weighting prior beliefs against sensory inputs based on their probabilities (Sterzer et al., 2018). Psychosis is assumed to result from low precision of prior beliefs and dysfunctional belief updating, conditioned by changes in dopamine signaling (Heinz et al., 2019). In mice, increasing striatal dopamine levels related to poor precision, i.e., high-confidence false alarms, in an auditory stimulus detection task, regarded as hallucination-like perceptions, (Schmack et al., 2021). A hyperdopaminergic state is thought to further cause aberrant assignment of salience to formed representations. Delusional beliefs reflect an individual's cognitive effort to explain experiences of aberrant salience, while hallucinations reflect the experience of aberrantly salient internal representations (Kapur, 2003).

Conceptually, paranormal belief, conspiratorial thinking, and schizotypy represent nonpathological states on a continuum converging toward delusional belief and psychosis (Kreweras, 1983; Denovan et al., 2018). For instance, conspiratorial beliefs, i.e., beliefs that certain events result from secret plots by powerful actors, correlated positively with paranormal beliefs, paranoid ideation, and schizotypy (Darwin et al., 2011). Interestingly, for both the social and perceptual domain, dopamine has been linked to delusional belief and psychotic symptoms in healthy and clinical samples (Sekine et al., 2001; Howes and Kapur, 2009; Krummenacher et al., 2010; Barnby et al., 2020). For the perceptual domain, studies assessing the ability to discriminate signals and noise in controls and individuals with hallucinations and schizophrenia yielded mixed results (Bentall and Slade, 1985; Ishigaki and Tanno, 1999; Krummenacher et al., 2010). Early studies report a more liberal criterion, i.e., a tendency to identify signals, but no difference in discrimination sensitivity, for individuals with high compared with low predisposition to hallucination in auditory signal detection (Bentall and Slade, 1985), and decreased discrimination sensitivity in patients with schizophrenia and auditory hallucinations compared with controls in a visual continuous performance test (Ishigaki and Tanno, 1999). In contrast, a more recent study reports individuals with paranormal beliefs to favor false alarms over misses and individuals skeptical about paranormal phenomena to show the reverse strategy. Enhancing dopaminergic neurotransmission lowered discrimination sensitivity compared with placebo in skeptics, but not believers (Krummenacher et al., 2010).

Dopamine has been assumed to reduce noise distortion in neuronal signal transmission (Walter and Spitzer, 2003; Vander Weele et al., 2018). However, considering the above findings and psychosis as hyperdopaminergic state characterized by a poor ability to discriminate relevant and irrelevant, and internal and external stimuli (Morris et al., 2013; Chu et al., 2021), this assumption falls short. The inconsistencies might be related to sample characteristics, for instance, to differences in predisposition to delusional thinking or symptomatology, or to the domain under study, such as auditory or visual. Further, most of the studies rely on rather small subgroup samples (Ishigaki and Tanno, 1999; Krummenacher et al., 2010; Barnby et al., 2020).

In view of these inconclusive findings, we aimed at investigating whether enhancing dopamine transmission elicits delusional beliefs already in healthy controls at an “early” perceptual (in contrast to, for instance, the cognitive process of establishing connections between events by means of complex explanations) stage, i.e., in visual perception. To this end, we used a pharmacological approach in healthy controls, increasing dopamine transmission with the dopamine precursor ʟ-DOPA, and studied the effects on illusory visual pattern perception. Modeling discrimination performance with signal detection theory, we predicted participants on versus off ʟ-DOPA to exhibit increases in false alarms, i.e., to perceive more illusory patterns, specifically objects in images containing only noise (see preregistration at https://osf.io/a4k9j/). With regard to response bias, discrimination sensitivity and accuracy, our hypotheses were nondirectional.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Cologne, Germany.

Sample

A subset of n = 49 out of N = 76 participants from a larger pharmacological study performed a visual perception task, specifically the snowy pictures task (SPT; Ekstrom and Harman, 1976; Whitson and Galinsky, 2008). One participant was excluded due to side effects (nausea and vomiting) and did not complete the task. The final sample analyzed here included 48 participants, including 19 women, all right-handed, aged 25–40 (M = 28.27). The participants were recruited in Cologne, Germany. They were recruited through university bulletins, through mailing lists, and by word-of-mouth recommendation. For practical reasons, only a subset of participants completed the SPT. Therefore, no task-specific a priori power calculation was carried out. A post hoc power analysis (paired samples test with G*Power, version 3.1.9.7; Faul et al., 2009) yielded a power of 0.39 to detect a small effect (d = 0.2), power of 0.96 to detect a medium effect (d = 0.5), and a power of >0.99 to detect a large effect (d = 0.8). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, German as first language (or profound German language skills), and all women were taking hormonal contraceptives. General exclusion criteria for study participation were strongly impaired vision or strabismus, participation in other studies involving medications, intake of nonprescription and prescription drugs, pregnancy, acute infections, alcohol or drug intoxication or abuse, psychiatric disorders (past or current), neurological disorders, metabolic disorders, internal diseases, chronic pain syndrome, complications of anesthesia, and strong emotional burden or physical stress during the study period. Exclusion criteria considering contraindications regarding the intake of ʟ-DOPA were hypersensitivity to ʟ-DOPA or benserazide, intake of nonselective monoamine oxidase inhibitors, metoclopramide or antihypertensive medication (e.g., reserpine), disorders of the central dopaminergic system, e.g., Lewy body dementia or Parkinson's disease, increased intraocular pressure (glaucoma), and breastfeeding.

Procedure

The current study was part of a larger pharmacological project assessing dopamine effects on decision-making and learning. Since the research questions, tasks and methods used are fundamentally different, reporting on all projects would go beyond the scope of the current article. Therefore, we focus on the analysis of the visual perception task and will report on the other projects elsewhere. After passing a medical examination by a physician to check for contraindications, the participants were invited to three testing sessions. During the first session, the participants underwent a baseline screening for putative dopamine proxies, specifically working memory capacity, spontaneous eyeblink rate, and impulsivity (Gibbs and D’Esposito, 2005; Dalley et al., 2007; Jongkees and Colzato, 2016). Since the investigation of the influence of putative proxies of dopamine function was only conducted for the effects of ʟ-DOPA on intertemporal choice and reinforcement learning, the data from the baseline screening will be reported elsewhere. After the baseline screening, the participants completed two identical experimental sessions on separate days with an interval of approximately a week between the sessions. Thirty minutes prior to testing, the participants received a nondistinguishable tablet containing either 150 mg ʟ-DOPA, a dopamine precursor, and benserazide, a peripheral decarboxylase inhibitor, or placebo and then completed an intertemporal choice task and a reinforcement learning task (see preregistration at https://osf.io/a4k9j/). Seventy-six participants completed the intertemporal choice and reinforcement task. Approximately 75 min after intake of the tablet, a subset of participants (n = 49 out of N = 76; see below, Snowy pictures task) additionally completed a visual perception task. The study was realized as double-blind placebo-controlled within-subjects design. Polling the participants showed that they could not guess the correct order of the experimental sessions χ2 (1, N = 48) = 0, p = 1.0.

Snowy pictures task

We used a modified pen-and-paper version of the SPT (Ekstrom and Harman, 1976; Whitson and Galinsky, 2008). The task contains 24 grainy images. Some of the images contain hard-to-detect embedded objects (for instance, a chair and a knife), and some contain only noise (12 images with objects, 12 images with noise). The participants were asked to denote whether or not an object is present in the image, and if so, what object it is. They were instructed to complete the task as fast as possible without sacrificing accuracy. The participants completed two different versions of the task under placebo and ʟ-DOPA, respectively, in counterbalanced order (12 images per session).

Data analysis

To assess the influence of enhancing dopaminergic transmission on the detection of objects in images, we calculated accuracies, false alarm rates, and the signal detection theory measures d-prime and response bias per condition (placebo and ʟ-DOPA). D-prime is an index for the ability to disentangle signal from noise, with higher values reflecting greater discriminability, while the response bias reflects the tendency toward responding “yes” or “no.” Negative values indicate a liberal response criterion (response bias toward responding “yes”), while positive values indicate a conservative response criterion (response bias toward responding “no”). D-prime and response bias were computed participant- and condition-wise in MATLAB (version R2023a) based on Stanislaw and Todorov (1999). Following the 1/2N rule, we corrected perfect hit and false alarm rates by −1/(2ntarget) and +1/(2ndistractor) , respectively (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999). Significant Shapiro–Wilk tests for accuracy W = 0.928, p = 0.006; false alarm rate W = 0.909, p = 0.001; and response bias differences W = 0.934, p < 0.009 indicated that differences between the matched pairs were not normally distributed. Therefore, we used nonparametric Bayesian Wilcoxon signed-rank tests as implemented in JASP (version 0.17.2.1) to compare the group means of accuracy, false alarm rate, and response bias between conditions. Since D-prime was normally distributed (W = 0.971; p = 0.270), we used a Bayesian paired samples t test to compare d-prime between conditions. The posterior distributions were obtained using Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sampling with five chains and 1,000 samples, using a Cauchy distribution with scale = 2 as prior. We report Bayes factors to evaluate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (nondirectional, BF01; directional, BF0−).

We further modeled the decisions with a hierarchical signal detection model (SDT) using Bayesian inference. The model was implemented using the bhsdtr package (version 2; Paulewicz and Blaut, 2020) for R (version 4.1.2; R Core Team, 2021). The model implements a hierarchical regression structure on the SDT parameters and accounts for parameter variability due to factors such as participants and items. The hierarchical general linear regression structure for the SDT parameters requires the parameters to be unconstrained. More specifically, to account for the assumption of normally distributed random effects, the model is reparameterized such that the parameters are unconstrained (since the normal distribution is unbounded). D-prime (d′) is derived from δ=ln(d′) , allowing random effects to be modeled by assuming that δ is normally distributed. For a full description of the model, the reader is referred to Paulewicz and Blaut (2020). For d-prime, we modeled the drug effect as fixed effect and participants and items as random effects. For the threshold (response bias), we modeled the drug effect as fixed effect and participants as random effects:δ=∼drug+(1|id)+(1|trial), andγ=∼drug+(1|id). (.)

For the priors on the random effects correlations, the bhsdtr package implements Cholesky decomposition of the correlation matrices, with uniform priors by default. For both d-prime and the threshold, we used normal priors for fixed effects (with μ=0.5 , σ=1 and μ=0.5 , σ=1 , respectively) and uniform priors for random effects. To assess a possible drug effect on d-prime and the threshold, we calculated the Savage–Dickey density ratio for the posterior difference distributions for placebo versus ʟ-DOPA by dividing the value of the posteriors over the parameters evaluated at θ=0 by the priors. Sampling was performed with Hamiltonian Monte Carlo sampling, using the Stan modeling language (Stan Development Team, 2023) via the rstan interface (version 2.32.2; Stan Development Team, 2024), with four chains, 1,000 warmup samples, and 2,000 iterations. We determined chain convergence by inspecting the traceplots and accepting values of R^≤1.01 (Gelman and Rubin, 1992).

Code and data accessibility

The data and the code used to analyze the data are freely available online at https://osf.io/m5u6v/ and https://osf.io/m7g3p/, respectively.

Results

On average, and across both conditions (placebo and ʟ-DOPA), participants correctly detected objects or correctly rejected noise in 81.77% of all images. The average response bias across both conditions of 0.65 indicates an overall tendency toward responding “no” (i.e., no object identified in an image). Under ʟ-DOPA, participants made false alarms in 11.28% of all images, i.e., identified objects in images containing only noise, compared with 12.15% in the placebo condition (see Table 1 and Fig. 1 for accuracy, hits, false alarms, d-prime, and response bias per drug condition). Testing for group differences in accuracy, false alarm rate, response bias, and d-prime between the conditions revealed that the null effect of no difference in accuracy under ʟ-DOPA and placebo was 14 times more likely than a difference between the conditions (BF01 = 14.48). Likewise, a null effect for the false alarm rate between ʟ-DOPA and placebo was 20 times more likely than an increased false alarm rate under ʟ-DOPA versus placebo (BF0− = 20.25). Furthermore, Bayesian analyses revealed evidence in favor of the null hypothesis for the response bias (BF01 = 6.37) and d-prime (BF01 = 11.46). These results were confirmed when analyzing the data using a hierarchical Bayesian implementation of the SDT (Paulewicz and Blaut, 2020) that may be more robust, given the low trial numbers in individual participants. The group-level posterior distributions for d-prime and threshold are depicted in Figure 2. Given the priors and the data, null effects for the drug effect on d-prime and threshold (response bias) were more likely than the alternative (BF01 = 7.29 and BF01 = 47.76, respectively).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Distribution of accuracy (A), false alarm rate (B), d-prime (C), and response bias (D) per condition. Horizontal line, median; box, first and third quartiles; lower whisker, lowest value not ∼1.5*IQR (interquartile range) from the first quartile; upper whisker, highest value no further than 1.5*IQR from the third quartile; dots, outliers (data points outside the lower and upper whisker). For the hit rate and false alarm rate (panels A and B, respectively), there are overlapping data points.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Top row, Group-level posterior distributions for d-prime and threshold (response bias), for the placebo (green, top) and ʟ-DOPA (magenta, bottom) condition. Bottom row, Difference distributions. The horizontal solid lines indicate the 95% highest posterior density intervals.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Means and standard deviations for task performance per drug condition

Discussion

We studied the effects of enhancing dopamine neurotransmission using the dopamine precursor ʟ-DOPA on illusory pattern perception using a visual perception task. Participants completed two versions of the modified SPT (Ekstrom and Harman, 1976; Whitson and Galinsky, 2008) under placebo and ʟ-DOPA, respectively. Applying signal detection theory, our hypothesis that participants on ʟ-DOPA would be more likely to perceive illusory patterns, specifically objects in images containing only noise was not confirmed. In contrast, Bayesian analyses revealed strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis for false alarm rates. Likewise, for accuracy, discrimination sensitivity, and response bias, Bayesian analyses revealed evidence in favor of the null hypothesis.

Changes in discrimination sensitivity and social attributions relevant to paranoia following dopaminergic modulation have previously been reported in perceptual and social decision-making in healthy samples (Krummenacher et al., 2010; Barnby et al., 2020). The involvement of dopamine in delusional ideation is further substantiated by the efficacy of antipsychotic treatment (Kaar et al., 2020), and a study assessing perceptual discrimination in controls and individuals with hallucinations and schizophrenia reported lower discrimination sensitivity in patients with auditory hallucinations (Ishigaki and Tanno, 1999). The present null effect of ʟ-DOPA on illusory pattern perception in the current study may be for several reasons. First, earlier studies reporting a relationship between dopamine and discrimination sensitivity or paranoid inferences rely on rather low sample sizes [30 participants in within-subjects design in Barnby et al. (2020), 20 participants per belief group in between-subject design in Krummenacher et al. (2010), 11 participants per patient group in Ishigaki and Tanno (1999)]. Low sample sizes increase the variance in effect sizes even under the null, such that previously reported findings may have been false positives. Alternatively, the present null effect may be related to dose-dependent effects or a single dose may have not been sufficient to elicit detectable changes in pattern perception. The effect of pharmacological DA manipulation might also depend on interindividual differences, such as a predisposition to delusional thinking, belief in the paranormal (Krummenacher et al., 2010), magical ideation (Mohr et al., 2006), and predisposition to hallucinations (Bentall and Slade, 1985). For instance, ʟ-DOPA increased semantic priming only in participants with high magical ideation (due to longer response times for unrelated prime-target pairs), and participants with high magical ideation under placebo performed comparable with participants with low magical ideation under ʟ-DOPA (Mohr et al., 2006). It is further conceivable that dopamine is related to delusional beliefs, while manifesting itself only in the pathological state or in individuals scoring high on schizotypy (Mohr and Ettinger, 2014). Lastly, the nature of the task may not be suitable to detect dopamine-related changes in illusory pattern perception, since it covers the visual domain only, and requires no inferences about events or social intentions.

Limitations

The current study has some limitations. Plasma levels peak 30–60 min after intake of ʟ-DOPA, while plasma half-life is ∼90 min (Hauser, 2009; Keller et al., 2011). Being part of a larger project, the present task was performed following two other behavioural tasks. The task was completed ∼75 min after intake and was typically completed in 5–10 min. Accordingly, the task was completed after plasma levels had peaked, but likely before the plasma half-life was reached. Still, the timing, and therefore the dose during task performance, may have contributed to the null effects of ʟ-DOPA on pattern perception. Dose-dependent effects on perceptual decision-making have been reported in another study, which modulated dopamine transmission via methylphenidate (Beste et al., 2018). Further, we did not assess the participants’ baseline dopamine synthesis capacity and their predisposition to delusional thinking or paranormal belief. The participants in our sample may not have had such a predisposition, and a single dose of ʟ-DOPA may therefore not have been sufficient to induce such effects. Also, since overall accuracy was rather high, and false alarm rates rather low, the task may be susceptible to ceiling effects.

The SPT has been frequently used to study illusory pattern perception. However, the task comprises a comparatively small number of items. In the original publication, using a between-subjects design, Whitson and Galinsky (2008) report differences between experimental conditions (lack of control vs baseline) using the same number of items per condition as in the present study. Using the same task, group differences in task performance between controls and patients with schizophrenia have been reported (Moritz et al., 2014), within-subject differences in terms of higher confidence in errors under ʟ-DOPA compared with placebo (Andreou et al., 2015), and associations between illusory pattern perception in the SPT and conspiracy belief (Hartmann and Müller, 2023). Of note, however, a more recent study (van Elk and Lodder, 2018) found no evidence that loss of control affects illusory pattern perception in the SPT. A strength of the current study is the within-subjects design, yielding higher power compared with between-subjects designs. Still, the low number of items may have limited power so as to detect small to medium effects. Furthermore, we additionally implemented a hierarchical Bayesian SDT model that might be more robust given the low number of trials per participant. This model confirmed the original result, such that null effects were substantially more likely given the data. Hierarchical Bayesian models also come with some limitations. Such models are sensitive to the choice of the prior distribution. Using hierarchical Bayesian modeling, we assume a specific type of distribution for the variability across the group, which may fall short in some cases (McGlothlin and Viele, 2018).

Conclusion and perspectives

We assessed the effect of enhancing dopaminergic neurotransmission on illusory pattern perception. There was no evidence that ʟ-DOPA, compared with placebo, increases the detection of patterns in noise. Rather, Bayesian analyses provided strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. Future studies should control for predisposition to delusional thinking and belief in the paranormal or magical ideation and may assess changes in pattern perception across different domains (e.g., auditory and visual).

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available at https://osf.io/m5u6v/. The study protocol was preregistered (see https://osf.io/a4k9j/).

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • We thank Lea Kemalides, Hannah Hacker, and Emily Burlon. This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, project no. PE1627/5-1). H.T. was supported by the Cologne Clinician Scientist Program (CCSP) of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Cologne, funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation, project no. 413543196).

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Andreou C,
    2. Bozikas VP,
    3. Luedtke T,
    4. Moritz S
    (2015) Associations between visual perception accuracy and confidence in a dopaminergic manipulation study. Front Psychol 6:414. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00414 pmid:25932015
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Barnby J,
    2. Bell V,
    3. Deeley Q,
    4. Mehta M
    (2020) Dopamine manipulations modulate paranoid social inferences in healthy people. Transl Psychiatry 10:214. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-00912-4 pmid:32624569
    OpenUrlPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Basanisi R,
    2. Marche K,
    3. Combrisson E,
    4. Apicella P,
    5. Brovelli A
    (2023) Beta oscillations in monkey striatum encode reward prediction error signals. J Neurosci 43:3339–3352. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0952-22.2023 pmid:37015808
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Bell AH,
    2. Summerfield C,
    3. Morin EL,
    4. Malecek NJ,
    5. Ungerleider LG
    (2016) Encoding of stimulus probability in macaque inferior temporal cortex. Curr Biol 26:2280–2290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.007 pmid:27524483
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bentall R,
    2. Slade PD
    (1985) Reality testing and auditory hallucinations: a signal detection analysis. Br J Clin Psychol 24:159–169. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1985.tb01331.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Beste C,
    2. Adelhöfer N,
    3. Gohil K,
    4. Passow S,
    5. Roessner V,
    6. Li SC
    (2018) Dopamine modulates the efficiency of sensory evidence accumulation during perceptual decision making. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 21:649–655. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyy019 pmid:29618012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Cheng PWC, et al.
    (2020) The role of dopamine dysregulation and evidence for the transdiagnostic nature of elevated dopamine synthesis in psychosis: a positron emission tomography (PET) study comparing schizophrenia, delusional disorder, and other psychotic disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 45:1870–1876. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0740-x pmid:32612207
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Chu RS-T,
    2. Ng C-M,
    3. Chan K-N,
    4. Chan K-W,
    5. Lee H-M,
    6. Hui L-M,
    7. Chen E,
    8. Chang W-C
    (2021) Aberrant learned irrelevance in patients with first-episode schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. Brain Sci 11:1370. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111370 pmid:34827368
    OpenUrlPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Clark A
    (2013) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 36:181–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Dalley JW, et al.
    (2007) Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science 315:1267–1270. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1137073 pmid:17332411
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Darwin H,
    2. Neave N,
    3. Holmes J
    (2011) Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Pers Indivd Dif 50:1289–1293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027
    OpenUrl
  12. ↵
    1. de Bruin L,
    2. Michael J
    (2021) Prediction error minimization as a framework for social cognition research. Erkenntnis 86:1–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10670-018-0090-9
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    1. de Lafuente V,
    2. Romo R
    (2011) Dopamine neurons code subjective sensory experience and uncertainty of perceptual decisions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:19767–19771. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117636108 pmid:22106310
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Denovan A,
    2. Dagnall N,
    3. Drinkwater K,
    4. Parker A
    (2018) Latent profile analysis of schizotypy and paranormal belief: associations with probabilistic reasoning performance. Front Psychol 9:35. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00035 pmid:29434562
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Ekstrom RB,
    2. Harman HH
    (1976) Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests, 1976. Princeton: Educational Testing Service.
  16. ↵
    1. Faul F,
    2. Erdfelder E,
    3. Buchner A,
    4. Lang A-G
    (2009) Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods 41:1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Friston KJ,
    2. Stephan KE
    (2007) Free-energy and the brain. Synthese 159:417–458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-007-9237-y pmid:19325932
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Gelman A,
    2. Rubin DB
    (1992) Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat Sci 7:457–472. https://doi.org/10.1214/ss/1177011136
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Gibbs SE,
    2. D’Esposito M
    (2005) Individual capacity differences predict working memory performance and prefrontal activity following dopamine receptor stimulation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 5:212–221. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.5.2.212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Glimcher PW
    (2011) Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: the dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:15647–15654. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014269108 pmid:21389268
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Hartmann M,
    2. Müller P
    (2023) Illusory perception of visual patterns in pure noise is associated with COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. Iperception 14:1. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669522114473
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    1. Hauser RA
    (2009) Levodopa: past, present, and future. Eur Neurol 62:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1159/000215875
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Heinz A,
    2. Murray GK,
    3. Schlagenhauf F,
    4. Sterzer P,
    5. Grace AA,
    6. Waltz JA
    (2019) Towards a unifying cognitive, neurophysiological, and computational neuroscience account of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 45:1092–1100. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby154 pmid:30388260
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Howes OD,
    2. Kapur S
    (2009) The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III—the final common pathway. Schizophr Bull 35:549–562. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp006 pmid:19325164
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Ishigaki T,
    2. Tanno Y
    (1999) The signal detection ability of patients with auditory hallucination: analysis using the continuous performance test. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 53:471–476. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1819.1999.00586.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Jongkees BJ,
    2. Colzato LS
    (2016) Spontaneous eye blink rate as predictor of dopamine-related cognitive function—a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 71:58–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Kaar SJ,
    2. Natesan S,
    3. Mccutcheon R,
    4. Howes OD
    (2020) Antipsychotics: mechanisms underlying clinical response and side-effects and novel treatment approaches based on pathophysiology. Neuropharmacology 172:107704. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.107704
    OpenUrl
  28. ↵
    1. Kapur S
    (2003) Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160:13–23. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Katthagen T,
    2. Kaminski J,
    3. Heinz A,
    4. Buchert R,
    5. Schlagenhauf F
    (2020) Striatal dopamine and reward prediction error signaling in unmedicated schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull 46:1535–1546. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa055 pmid:32318717
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Keller GA,
    2. Czerniuk P,
    3. Bertuola R,
    4. Spatz JG,
    5. Assefi AR,
    6. Di Girolamo G
    (2011) Comparative bioavailability of 2 tablet formulations of levodopa/benserazide in healthy, fasting volunteers: a single-dose, randomized-sequence, open-label crossover study. Clin Ther 33:500–510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinthera.2011.04.012
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Kirschner H, et al.
    (2024) Transdiagnostic inflexible learning dynamics explain deficits in depression and schizophrenia. Brain 147:201–214. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awad362 pmid:38058203
    OpenUrlPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Kreweras G
    (1983) Approche bayesienne des phénomènes ‘paranormaux’. Math Sci Hum 81:59–66.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    1. Krummenacher P,
    2. Mohr C,
    3. Haker H,
    4. Brugger P
    (2010) Dopamine, paranormal belief, and the detection of meaningful stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1670–1681. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21313
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Lak A,
    2. Nomoto K,
    3. Keramati M,
    4. Sakagami M,
    5. Kepecs A
    (2017) Midbrain dopamine neurons signal belief in choice accuracy during a perceptual decision. Curr Biol 27:821–832. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.02.026 pmid:28285994
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. McGlothlin AE,
    2. Viele K
    (2018) Bayesian hierarchical models. J Am Med Assoc 320:2365–2366. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2018.17977
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Mohr C,
    2. Ettinger U
    (2014) An overview of the association between schizotypy and dopamine. Front Psychiatry 5:184. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00184 pmid:25566103
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Mohr C,
    2. Landis T,
    3. Brugger P
    (2006) Lateralized semantic priming: modulation by levodopa, semantic distance, and participants’ magical beliefs. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2:71–84. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00184 pmid:25566103
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Moritz S, et al.
    (2014) Overconfidence in incorrect perceptual judgments in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Cogn 1:165–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2014.09.003 pmid:29379749
    OpenUrlPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Morris R,
    2. Griffiths O,
    3. Le Pelley ME,
    4. Weickert TW
    (2013) Attention to irrelevant cues is related to positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 39:575–582. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbr192 pmid:22267535
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Paulewicz B,
    2. Blaut A
    (2020) The bhsdtr package: a general-purpose method of Bayesian inference for signal detection theory models. Behav Res Methods 52:2122–2141. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01370-y
    OpenUrl
  41. ↵
    1. Pessiglione M,
    2. Seymour B,
    3. Flandin G,
    4. Dolan RJ,
    5. Frith CD
    (2006) Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 442:1042–1045. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05051 pmid:16929307
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    R Core Team (2021). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/
  43. ↵
    1. Rigney G,
    2. Ayubcha C,
    3. Werner TJ,
    4. Alavi A,
    5. Revheim M-E
    (2021) The utility of PET imaging in the diagnosis and management of psychosis: a brief review. Clin Transl Imaging 10:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40336-021-00466-5
    OpenUrl
  44. ↵
    Stan Development Team (2024). ‘RStan: the R interface to Stan.’ R package version 2.32.2. Available at: https://mc-stan.org/
  45. ↵
    1. Schlagenhauf F, et al.
    (2013) Ventral striatal prediction error signaling is associated with dopamine synthesis capacity and fluid intelligence. Hum Brain Mapp 34:1490–1499. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22000 pmid:22344813
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Schlagenhauf F,
    2. Huys QJ,
    3. Deserno L,
    4. Rapp MA,
    5. Beck A,
    6. Heinze HJ,
    7. Dolan R,
    8. Heinz A
    (2014) Striatal dysfunction during reversal learning in unmedicated schizophrenia patients. Neuroimage 89:171–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.034 pmid:24291614
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Schmack K,
    2. Bosc M,
    3. Ott T,
    4. Sturgill JF,
    5. Kepecs A
    (2021) Striatal dopamine mediates hallucination-like perception in mice. Science 372:eabf4740. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abf4740
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Schultz W
    (2016) Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component response. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:183–195. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.26 pmid:26865020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Sekine Y,
    2. Iyo M,
    3. Ouchi Y,
    4. Matsunaga T,
    5. Tsukada H,
    6. Okada H,
    7. Yoshikawa E,
    8. Futatsubashi M,
    9. Takei N,
    10. Mori N
    (2001) Methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms and reduced brain dopamine transporters studied with PET. Am J Psychiatry 158:1206–1214. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Stanislaw H,
    2. Todorov N
    (1999) Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 31:137–149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03207704
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Stan Development Team (2023). Stan modeling language users guide and reference manual, 2.32. Available at: https://mc-stan.org
  52. ↵
    1. Sterzer P,
    2. Adams RA,
    3. Fletcher P,
    4. Frith C,
    5. Lawrie SM,
    6. Muckli L,
    7. Petrovic P,
    8. Uhlhaas P,
    9. Voss M,
    10. Corlett PR
    (2018) The predictive coding account of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 84:634–643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.05.015 pmid:30007575
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Vander Weele CM, et al.
    (2018) Dopamine enhances signal-to-noise ratio in cortical-brainstem encoding of aversive stimuli. Nature 563:397–401. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0682-1 pmid:30405240
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Van Elk M,
    2. Lodder P
    (2018) Experimental manipulations of personal control do not increase illusory pattern perception. Collabra Psychol 4:19. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.155
    OpenUrl
  55. ↵
    1. Walter H,
    2. Spitzer M
    (2003) The cognitive neuroscience of agency in schizophrenia. In: The self in neuroscience and psychiatry (Kircher T, David A, eds), pp 436–444. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  56. ↵
    1. Whitson JA,
    2. Galinsky AD
    (2008) Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science 322:115–117. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Frederike Beyer, Queen Mary University of London

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Stijn Nuiten.

As you can see from their comments below, the reviewers appreciate the efforts that have gone into the revision of this manuscript. However, as evident from our dsicussion on this, we all agree the low trial number seriously limits the conclusions that can be drawn based on the presented results. As such, if further data collection to address this issue is not an option, the article needs to state clearly and early on (i.e. already in the abstract) what the limitations of this study are, and that the results need to be interpreted with caution.

Besides this, further information on the bayesian analyses conducted and some more detail in other areas is required for the article to be acceptable for publication. Please see detailed comments of the reviewers below.

Best wishes,

Frederike Beyer

Reviewer 1 comments

Although the trial counts for each participant remain quite low, the newly added hierarchical Bayesian modelling does add more credence to the null-findings reported in this manuscript. Besides this addition, the manuscript has been sufficiently revised, particularly regarding previous literature and detailing of the methods.

Reviewer 2 comments

The authors argue that levodopa has no effect on the illusory visual pattern perception in the Snowy Pictures Task. This is of potential interest to research into perception, psychosis, and side effects of Parkinson's patients being treated with levodopa, indicating that simply increasing the capacity for dopamine synthesis in neurotypical humans is not sufficient to cause significant illusory perception in the visual modality. The authors acknowledge that the low trial number in the Snowy Pictures Task and the timing of their drug administration may limit the conclusions that they draw. However, the manuscript lacks clarity in various areas which limits the interpretability of their results.

Major comments

1. The introduction says "midbrain (striatal) dopaminergic neurons". Midbrain dopaminergic neurons are not striatal, they project to striatum. Do the authors mean to refer to the terminals of these neurons, or to the dopamine release in striatum? Apart from the Schmack et al 2021 paper, the additions to the introduction relating to learning and prediction error appear irrelevant because they are not integrated into the text. I would suggest that the authors review the literature thoroughly and use it to explain why they have designed their experiment in this way.

2. The hierarchical Bayesian modelling is not well explained in the results or methods sections, and the authors might consider including the limitations of their use of such a method as well.

3. In the limitations section there is text saying 'the timing and therefore the dose during task performance' - I am not clear what this means. Are the authors trying to say that the elimination of the drug may have resulted in their negative findings? If so, this suggests poor experimental design and makes these results very difficult to interpret.

Minor comments

1. The methods are still not completely clear to me. The authors might consider using the same task descriptions throughout the text e.g. is the 'visual perception task' the same as the 'visual discrimination task' the same as the 'snowy pictures task'? Also is the 75 minutes after intake of tablet referring to when the visual task was started or when the other tasks were started?

2. There is reference to a study on methylphenidate added to the limitations section which seemingly does not relate to the text around it.

3. There are typos throughout the text.

Author Response

Synthesis of Reviews:

Synthesis Statement for Author (Required):

As you can see from their comments below, the reviewers appreciate the efforts that have gone into the revision of this manuscript. However, as evident from our discussion on this, we all agree the low trial number seriously limits the conclusions that can be drawn based on the presented results. As such, if further data collection to address this issue is not an option, the article needs to state clearly and early on (i.e. already in the abstract) what the limitations of this study are, and that the results need to be interpreted with caution.

Besides this, further information on the bayesian analyses conducted and some more detail in other areas is required for the article to be acceptable for publication. Please see detailed comments of the reviewers below.

Best wishes, Frederike Beyer > We now point out the limitation of the small number of trials in the Abstract: "Since the task comprised a rather low number of items, power may have been limited so as to detect small to medium effects. The results should therefore be interpreted with caution. Future studies should address illusory pattern perception using more items, and possible dose-dependent effects and differential effects in healthy vs. clinical samples." (p. 1, Abstract) > Further, we describe the hierarchical Bayesian analysis of the task in more detail (see response to reviewer #2 and Supplement).

Reviewer 1 comments Although the trial counts for each participant remain quite low, the newly added hierarchical Bayesian modelling does add more credence to the null-findings reported in this manuscript. Besides this addition, the manuscript has been sufficiently revised, particularly regarding previous literature and detailing of the methods.

Reviewer 2 comments The authors argue that levodopa has no effect on the illusory visual pattern perception in the Snowy Pictures Task. This is of potential interest to research into perception, psychosis, and side effects of Parkinson's patients being treated with levodopa, indicating that simply increasing the capacity for dopamine synthesis in neurotypical humans is not sufficient to cause significant illusory perception in the visual modality. The authors acknowledge that the low trial number in the Snowy Pictures Task and the timing of their drug administration may limit the conclusions that they draw. However, the manuscript lacks clarity in various areas which limits the interpretability of their results.

Major comments 1. The introduction says "midbrain (striatal) dopaminergic neurons". Midbrain dopaminergic neurons are not striatal, they project to striatum. Do the authors mean to refer to the terminals of these neurons, or to the dopamine release in striatum? Apart from the Schmack et al 2021 paper, the additions to the introduction relating to learning and prediction error appear irrelevant because they are not integrated into the text. I would suggest that the authors review the literature thoroughly and use it to explain why they have designed their experiment in this way. > We thank the reviewer for pointing out the imprecise wording. We edited the respective sentence accordingly: "A large body of research suggests that dopamine modulates striatal coding of prediction errors to enable learning (Basanisi et al., 2023; Glimcher, 2011; Pessiglione et al, 2006; Schlagenauf, 2013; Schultz, 2016)." > Following the reviewer's suggestion, we revised the Introduction and provide a more coherent review of the literature and reasoning for our study design (see section 1, Introduction).

2. The hierarchical Bayesian modelling is not well explained in the results or methods sections, and the authors might consider including the limitations of their use of such a method as well. > Following the reviewer's suggestion, we now describe the hierarchical Bayesian model of the SDT parameters in greater detail (see also Supplement). To summarise the edits, we provided further information on the model structure (hierarchical linear regression model), priors, and denote equations for the fixed and random effect structure. > Further, we address limitations of using hierarchical Bayesian models (see also Supplement): "Hierarchical Bayesian models also come with some limitations. Such models are sensitive to the choice of the prior distribution. Using hierarchical Bayesian modelling, we assume a specific type of distribution for the variability across the group, which may fall short in some cases (McGlothlin &Viele, 2018)." 3. In the limitations section there is text saying 'the timing and therefore the dose during task performance' - I am not clear what this means. Are the authors trying to say that the elimination of the drug may have resulted in their negative findings? If so, this suggests poor experimental design and makes these results very difficult to interpret.

Since the study was part of a larger project, the participants completed three tasks in succession. L-DOPA has a plasma half-life of approximately 90 minutes (Hauser, 2009). The pattern perception task was completed approximately 75 minutes after the intake and was typically completed in 5 to 10 minutes. Therefore, there likely was a decrease in plasma levels compared to the previous tasks, but we were still well in the time window of the plasma half-life. We now discuss this in greater detail in the limitations section: "The current study has some limitations. Plasma levels peak 30 to 60 minutes after intake of L-DOPA, while plasma half-life is around 90 minutes (Hauser, 2009; Keller et al., 2011). Being part of a larger project, the present task was performed following two other behavioural tasks. The task was completed approximately 75 minutes after intake and was typically completed in 5 to 10 minutes. Accordingly, the task was completed after plasma levels had peaked, but likely before the plasma half-life was reached. Still, the timing, and therefore the dose during task performance, may have contributed to the null effects of L-DOPA on pattern perception" Minor comments 1. The methods are still not completely clear to me. The authors might consider using the same task descriptions throughout the text e.g. is the 'visual perception task' the same as the 'visual discrimination task' the same as the 'snowy pictures task'? Also is the 75 minutes after intake of tablet referring to when the visual task was started or when the other tasks were started? > We thank the reviewer for pointing out that we need to clarify the procedure and describe this more clearly: "30 minutes prior to testing, the participants received a non-distinguishable tablet containing either 150 mg L-DOPA, a dopamine precursor, and benseracide, a peripheral decarboxylase inhibitor, or placebo, and then completed an intertemporal choice task and a reinforcement learning task [...] Approximately 75 minutes after intake of the tablet, a subset of participants additionally completed a visual perception task [...]" (section 2.2 of Methods) Also, we now consistently describe the Snowy Pictures Task as "visual perception task", and specifically refer to the task as "Snowy Pictures Task" after having described the task in detail in section 2.3.

2. There is reference to a study on methylphenidate added to the limitations section which seemingly does not relate to the text around it. > We thank the reviewer for the hint. The sentence was formulated in a misleading way. We report that in another study, the effect of enhancing dopamine transmission was found to be dose-dependent. We rephrased the sentence accordingly: "Dose-dependent effects on perceptual decision making have been reported in another study, which modulated dopamine transmission via metylphenidate (Beste et al., 2018)." 3. There are typos throughout the text. > We thank the reviewer for the hint and checked the manuscript for spelling errors.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 11 (7)
eNeuro
Vol. 11, Issue 7
July 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Pharmacological Enhancement of Dopamine Neurotransmission Does Not Affect Illusory Pattern Perception
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Pharmacological Enhancement of Dopamine Neurotransmission Does Not Affect Illusory Pattern Perception
Elke Smith, Simon Michalski, Kilian Knauth, Deniz Tuzsus, Hendrik Theis, Thilo van Eimeren, Jan Peters
eNeuro 12 July 2024, 11 (7) ENEURO.0465-23.2024; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0465-23.2024

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Pharmacological Enhancement of Dopamine Neurotransmission Does Not Affect Illusory Pattern Perception
Elke Smith, Simon Michalski, Kilian Knauth, Deniz Tuzsus, Hendrik Theis, Thilo van Eimeren, Jan Peters
eNeuro 12 July 2024, 11 (7) ENEURO.0465-23.2024; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0465-23.2024
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Data Availability
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
    • Author Response
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • discrimination sensitivity
  • dopamine
  • ʟ-DOPA
  • pattern perception

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Deletion of endocannabinoid synthesizing enzyme DAGLα in Pcp2-positive cerebellar Purkinje cells decreases depolarization-induced short-term synaptic plasticity, reduces social preference, and heightens anxiety
  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
  • Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Encodes the Trace Period during Appetitive Pavlovian Conditioning
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.