Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

Short-Term Memory Capacity Predicts Willingness to Expend Cognitive Effort for Reward

Brandon J. Forys, Catharine A. Winstanley, Alan Kingstone and Rebecca M. Todd
eNeuro 12 June 2024, 11 (7) ENEURO.0068-24.2024; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0068-24.2024
Brandon J. Forys
1Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Brandon J. Forys
Catharine A. Winstanley
1Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada
2Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z3, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Catharine A. Winstanley
Alan Kingstone
1Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Alan Kingstone
Rebecca M. Todd
1Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada
2Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z3, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Rebecca M. Todd
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Layout of the experimental tasks. A, The monetary incentive delay task, in which participants indicate their excitement in playing for tickets toward a monetary reward. B, The calibration phase of the change detection task, where participants saw an array of either two, four, six, or eight squares and indicated whether the final square that appeared (the probe) was of the same or a different color from the square that appeared in the same location in the previously show array. C, The reward phase of the change detection task, where participants saw an array of either two or six, or four or eight, shapes onscreen depending on their performance in the calibration phase of the task. Once again, participants indicated with a keyboard press whether the final square that appeared (the probe) is the same or a different color from the square that appeared in the same location in the array. Here, if they made the correct decision, they would receive 1 point (low effort trial) or 10 points (high effort trial); they would receive 0 points for an incorrect decision.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Distribution of A, depressive trait (BDI) proportion scores (score divided by total possible score), B, perceived stress (PSS) scores, C, anhedonia (DARS) scores, and D, mean reward anticipation scores by sex and effort deployment group. BDI, beck depression inventory; PSS, perceived stress scale; DARS, dimensional anhedonia rating scale.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Correlations among depressive traits (BDI), perceived stress (PSS), reward anticipation, and short-term memory ability (K estimate). BDI, beck depression inventory; PSS, perceived stress sale; anticipation, mean reward anticipation; K estimate, visual short-term memory ability (K score).

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Binomial logistic regression predicting whether participants were in the high effort versus low effort group. High effort group, >70% high reward (HR) trials selected; low effort group, ≤70% HR trials selected.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Accuracy and choice latency in the choice phase of the change detection task by motivation group (high effort vs low effort group). The choice latency plot has been log-transformed on the y-axis to more clearly show small choice latency values. High effort group, >70% HR trials selected; low effort group, ≤70% HR trials selected.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    A, Accuracy and B, choice latency in the reward phase of the change detection task by working memory ability (K estimate). The choice latency plot has been log-transformed on the y-axis to more clearly show small choice latency values.

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Outputs of a hierarchical Bayesian drift diffusion model fit to all participants in the effort choice task, divided by effort group. High effort participants had a significantly higher A, drift rate, B, starting point and C, boundary separation compared to low effort participants, but did not significantly differ on D, non-decision time compared to low effort participants.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Demographic information for all participants, by sex and effort deployment group

    GroupSexnMageSDageMinageMaxageMBDIpropSDBDIpropMPSSSDPSSMDARSSDDARSMant.SDant.
    High effortFemale26320.342.3716420.270.1820.836.2135.6111.713.941.81
    Male8020.702.3417320.190.1417.695.3037.699.624.291.75
    Other520.400.8919210.170.1019.603.6543.0010.074.801.64
    Low effortFemale6920.011.9416260.280.1620.686.5438.048.944.001.79
    Male1220.922.9718280.190.1421.084.3839.178.613.961.51
    • BDI prop: beck depression inventory II proportion score (score divided by max score); PSS: perceived stress scale score; DARS: dimensional anhedonia rating scale; Ant.: mean reward anticipation in the behavioral monetary incentive delay task; high effort group: >70% HR trials selected; low effort group: ≤70% HR trials selected.

    • View popup
    Table 2.

    Binomial logistic regression predicting whether participants were in the high effort versus low effort group

    Binomial logistic regression: high versus low effort group
    Working memory ability (K estimate)0.21*
    (0.09)
    BDI (prop. score)−0.78
    (0.96)
    PSS0.00
    (0.03)
    Reward anticipation0.00
    (0.07)
    Num. Obs.429
    AIC419.7
    BIC440.0
    Log. Lik.−204.833
    RMSE0.39
    • High effort group, >70% HR trials selected; low effort group, ≤70% HR trials selected; BDI (prop. score), depression score on the beck depression inventory; PSS, perceived stress scale. Proportion scores are scores divided by total possible score. AIC, Akaike information criterion; BIC, Bayesian information criterion; ICC, intraclass correlation; RMSE, root mean squared error.

    • +p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

    • View popup
    Table 3.

    Linear regression of predictors for proportion of high effort trials selected

    Linear model: proportion of high effort trials selected
    (Intercept)0.84***
    (0.04)
    Working memory ability (K estimate)0.02**
    (0.01)
    BDI (prop. score)−0.03
    (0.07)
    PSS0.00
    (0.00)
    Reward anticipation0.00
    (0.00)
    Num. Obs.429
    R20.018
    R2 Adj.0.009
    AIC−274.2
    BIC−249.8
    Log. Lik.143.105
    RMSE0.17
    • High effort group, >70% HR trials selected; low effort group, ≤70% HR trials selected; BDI (prop. score), depression score on the beck depression inventory; PSS, perceived stress scale. Proportion scores are scores divided by total possible score. AIC, Akaike information criterion; BIC, Bayesian information criterion; ICC, intraclass correlation; RMSE, root mean squared error.

    • +p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

    • View popup
    Table 4.

    Summary table of accuracy and choice latency by group and effort level chosen

    GroupEffort levelMAccuracySDAccuracyMLatencySDLatency
    High effortHigh0.700.111.271.04
    High effortLow0.950.112.541.99
    Low effortHigh0.700.111.231.10
    Low effortLow0.900.111.601.92
    • High effort group, >70% HR trials selected; low effort group, ≤70% HR trials selected.

    • View popup
    Table 5.

    Multi-level model analysis coefficients and standard errors for accuracy in the reward phase of the task

    Multi-level model: accuracy
    (Intercept)0.64***
    (0.02)
    Sex0.01
    (0.01)
    Working memory ability (K estimate)0.01**
    (0.00)
    BDI (prop. score)−0.01
    (0.04)
    PSS0.00
    (0.00)
    Reward anticipation0.00
    (0.00)
    Effort level0.25***
    (0.01)
    SD (intercept participant)0.04
    SD (observations)0.10
    Num. Obs.542
    R2 Marg.0.592
    R2 Cond.0.656
    AIC−840.0
    BIC−801.3
    ICC0.2
    RMSE0.09
    • BDI (prop. score), depression score on the beck depression inventory; PSS, perceived stress scale. Proportion scores are scores divided by total possible score. AIC, Akaike information criterion; BIC, Bayesian information criterion; ICC, intraclass correlation; RMSE, root mean squared error.

    • +p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

    • View popup
    Table 6.

    Multi-level model analysis coefficients and standard errors for choice latency in the reward phase of the task

    Multi-level model: choice latency
    (Intercept)1.25***
    (0.34)
    Sex−0.07
    (0.17)
    Working memory ability (K estimate)0.05
    (0.05)
    BDI (prop. score)−1.11*
    (0.54)
    PSS0.01
    (0.01)
    Reward anticipation−0.04
    (0.04)
    Effort level1.06***
    (0.13)
    SD (intercept participant)0.18
    SD (observations)1.53
    Num. Obs.542
    R2 Marg.0.114
    R2 Cond.0.127
    AIC2042.9
    BIC2081.6
    ICC0.0
    RMSE1.51
    • BDI (prop. score), depression score on the beck depression inventory; PSS, perceived stress scale. Proportion scores are scores divided by total possible score. AIC, Akaike information criterion; BIC, Bayesian information criterion; ICC, intraclass correlation; RMSE, root mean squared error.

    • +p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 11 (7)
eNeuro
Vol. 11, Issue 7
July 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Short-Term Memory Capacity Predicts Willingness to Expend Cognitive Effort for Reward
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Short-Term Memory Capacity Predicts Willingness to Expend Cognitive Effort for Reward
Brandon J. Forys, Catharine A. Winstanley, Alan Kingstone, Rebecca M. Todd
eNeuro 12 June 2024, 11 (7) ENEURO.0068-24.2024; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0068-24.2024

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Short-Term Memory Capacity Predicts Willingness to Expend Cognitive Effort for Reward
Brandon J. Forys, Catharine A. Winstanley, Alan Kingstone, Rebecca M. Todd
eNeuro 12 June 2024, 11 (7) ENEURO.0068-24.2024; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0068-24.2024
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Data and Code Availability
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
    • Author Response
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Novel roles for the GPI-anchor cleaving enzyme, GDE2, in hippocampal synaptic morphology and function
  • Upright posture: a singular condition stabilizing sensorimotor coordination
  • EEG Signatures of Auditory Distraction: Neural Responses to Spectral Novelty in Real-World Soundscapes
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • EEG Signatures of Auditory Distraction: Neural Responses to Spectral Novelty in Real-World Soundscapes
  • Excess neonatal testosterone causes male-specific social and fear memory deficits in wild-type mice
  • The effects of mindfulness meditation on mechanisms of attentional control in young and older adults: a preregistered eye tracking study
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.