Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

Comparative Roles of the Caudate and Putamen in the Serial Order of Behavior: Effects of Striatal Glutamate Receptor Blockade on Variable versus Fixed Spatial Self-Ordered Sequencing in Marmosets

Stacey Anne Gould, Amy Hodgson, Hannah F. Clarke, Trevor W. Robbins and Angela C. Roberts
eNeuro 12 March 2024, 11 (3) ENEURO.0541-23.2024; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0541-23.2024
Stacey Anne Gould
1Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Amy Hodgson
1Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hannah F. Clarke
1Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hannah F. Clarke
Trevor W. Robbins
2Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Angela C. Roberts
1Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Angela C. Roberts
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Self-ordered sequencing is an important executive function involving planning and executing a series of steps to achieve goal-directed outcomes. The lateral frontal cortex is implicated in this behavior, but downstream striatal outputs remain relatively unexplored. We trained marmosets on a three-stimulus self-ordered spatial sequencing task using a touch-sensitive screen to explore the role of the caudate nucleus and putamen in random and fixed response arrays. By transiently blocking glutamatergic inputs to these regions, using intrastriatal CNQX microinfusions, we demonstrate that the caudate and putamen are both required for, but contribute differently to, flexible and fixed sequencing. CNQX into either the caudate or putamen impaired variable array accuracy, and infusions into both simultaneously elicited greater impairment. We demonstrated that continuous perseverative errors in variable array were caused by putamen infusions, likely due to interference with the putamen's established role in monitoring motor feedback. Caudate infusions, however, did not affect continuous errors, but did cause an upward trend in recurrent perseveration, possibly reflecting interference with the caudate's established role in spatial working memory and goal-directed planning. In contrast to variable array performance, while both caudate and putamen infusions impaired fixed array responding, the combined effects were not additive, suggesting possible competing roles. Infusions into either region individually, but not simultaneously, led to continuous perseveration. Recurrent perseveration in fixed arrays was caused by putamen, but not caudate, infusions. These results are consistent overall with a role of caudate in planning and flexible responding and the putamen in more rigid habitual or automatic responding.

  • caudate
  • goal directed
  • habit
  • putamen
  • self-ordered sequencing
  • serial order of behavior

Significance Statement

This investigation employing local intrastriatal infusions into the caudate nucleus and/or putamen of the New World marmoset reveals important roles for these regions in variable and fixed spatial self-ordered sequencing. Here, we directly implicate subcortical output regions of the lateral prefrontal cortex in self-ordered sequencing behavior. The ability to self-order sequences, as well as more broadly to plan, organize information, and respond flexibly, is impaired in many neurological diseases and psychiatric disorders. By understanding the basic neural circuitry underlying these cognitive abilities, we may better understand how to rectify them in people with deficits across a plethora of disorders.

Introduction

Self-ordered sequencing is the ability to organize information and plan and execute a series of related steps in order to achieve a goal (Conen and Desrochers, 2022). Self-ordered sequencing is an executive cognitive function requiring elements of working memory, planning, and monitoring, occurring in a goal-directed fashion. The ability to self-order nonspatial sequences is disrupted by lesions to frontal and temporal lobes in humans (Petrides and Milner, 1982) and by mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) lesions in rhesus monkeys (Petrides, 1995). Spatial working memory deficits in tasks requiring planning of self-ordered response sequences are also observed in both humans and rhesus monkeys following damage to the dorsolateral (dl)PFC (Passingham, 1985; Owen et al., 1990). Spatial sequencing deficits are also present in people with disorders such as obsessive–compulsive disorder (Vaghi et al., 2017; Banca et al., 2023), schizophrenia (Pantelis et al., 1997), Alzheimer's disease (Lafleche and Albert, 1995), and Parkinson's disease (Cooper et al., 1991). In the marmoset monkey, fiber-sparing lesions to the combined lateral, orbital, and anteroventral regions of the PFC confirmed its role in self-ordered sequencing (Collins et al., 1998). Subsequently, this effect was localized to ventrolateral (vl)PFC, but not orbitofrontal cortex (Walker et al., 2009; Axelsson et al., 2021).

In the marmoset, vlPFC projects to the striatum via glutamatergic pyramidal neurons (Roberts et al., 2007) as a relay of a fronto-striatal “loop” (Alexander et al., 1986). The striatum is also implicated in self-ordered sequencing in humans (Jahanshahi et al., 2002) with potential contributions from both the caudate and putamen, which are implicated respectively in flexible goal-directed behavior and relatively automatic stimulus-response habits (Grahn et al., 2008; Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010). Previously, we have shown that the contribution of vlPFC in marmosets to self-ordered response sequencing of spatial arrays is limited to when the arrays vary across trials, requiring flexible, goal-directed behavior for successful performance (Axelsson et al., 2021). In contrast, when the same spatial array was used across trials in a fixed version of the self-ordered sequencing task which encouraged more automated responding involving use of a restricted set of preferred response sequences, inactivation of the vlPFC was without effect (Axelsson et al., 2021). In macaque, as well as marmoset, vlPFC projects to both the caudate and putamen (Roberts et al., 2007; Choi et al., 2017) which may indicate a mass action effect. However, the vlPFC striatal projection zone in macaque receives different patterns of additional cortical input into these striatal sectors (Choi et al., 2017), suggestive of at least partially distinct functional systems.

In the variable spatial array version of the self-ordered sequencing task, three stimuli appear in a random position on a computer screen for each trial, and the animal must press each stimulus once and once only without returning to a previously selected one. Thus, in this version of the task, the animal must generalize application of a rule across trials with differing arrays, a process which involves executive functions including working memory and flexible planning. In contrast, for the fixed array, stimuli invariably appear in the same three locations. However, the marmoset can select whichever sequence of responding in these locations it prefers, thus allowing the animal to optimize its performance by utilizing more automated response sequences. Variable array responding is hypothetically more goal directed (i.e., flexible) than fixed array responding, which is more habitual (i.e., automatic and rigid; Dezfouli et al., 2014). Impairments in both forms of sequencing could arise from a tendency to maladaptively repeat responding (perseveration), a tendency that can arise in different forms (Sandson and Albert, 1984). Thus, repeating the just-performed response is termed continuous perseveration and interference caused by repetition later in the sequence is termed recurrent perseveration.

One aim of this study was to determine whether the vlPFC projection zone in the striatum (including both the caudate nucleus and putamen) is causally implicated in spatial self-ordered sequencing behavior. The second aim was to determine whether or not the caudate and putamen sectors make differential contributions, for example, based on their respective roles in goal-directed and habitual behavior (Kermadi and Joseph, 1995; Levy et al., 1997; Postle and D’Esposito, 1999; Collins et al., 2000; Lewis et al., 2004; Grahn et al., 2008; Kubota et al., 2009; Yin et al., 2009; Yin, 2010; Akhlaghpour et al., 2016; Turner et al., 2022). To address these aims, we targeted regions of the striatum receiving anatomical projections from vlPFC using chronic indwelling cannulae. We transiently blocked transmission of glutamatergic inputs to the caudate and/or putamen using microinfusions of AMPA (α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole-propionate)/kainate glutamate receptor competitive antagonist CNQX (6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione; Smith et al., 1996), either independently into each region or simultaneously into both combined.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Six common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) were bred on-site at the University of Cambridge marmoset breeding colony. The marmoset holding rooms were kept at a constant 24°C with relative humidity of 55%. Holding rooms were gradually illuminated from 7:30 to 8 A.M. and gradually dimmed from 7:30 to 8 P.M., for a 12 h light/dark cycle with 30 min of dusk/dawn. Cages (2.8 × 1.2 × 0.98 m) contained a food tray, a nest box, wooden platforms at different heights, and a variety of enrichment objects, including ladders, wooden branches, and ropes. Five days a week, animals had access to water for 2 h after behavioral testing and during this time period were fed MP.E1 primate diet (Special Diet Services) and carrots. During weekends animals had ad libitum access to water and were fed a calorically equal diet consisting of bread, egg, rusk, fruit, and nuts. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 as amended in 2012, under project license P09631465. In addition, the University of Cambridge Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB) provided ethical approval of the project license and its amendments, as well as individual studies and procedures via delegation of authorization to the NACWO for individual study plans. The study is summarized in Figure 1A.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Outline of the spatial self-ordered sequencing task showing the experimental timeline (A), touchscreen stimuli positions (B), and variable and fixed array tasks (C,D). An example of a correct sequence is shown (E), as well as perseverative errors of the recurrent (F) and continuous (G) type. Details relating to counterbalancing can be found in Extended Data Table 1-1A–F.

Table 1-1

Individual marmoset details and counterbalancing tables showing order of array type, brain region targeted, and concentration of CNQX administered. Download Table 1-1, XLSX file.

Apparatus

All behavioral testing was performed in a custom-built testing apparatus located in a separate room from the marmoset holding rooms. Animals were trained to enter a custom-made Perspex transport box (Biotronix), in which they remained during testing. A door on the box was removed to provide access to a touch-sensitive computer monitor (NEX121 TFT LCD Monitor, Nexio). Animals had to reach through an array of vertical bars to respond to visual stimuli on the touch screen. Reward, in the form of banana milk (Nesquik banana powder in milk, Nestlé), was delivered through a peristaltic pump to a licking spout with lick sensor which was accessible through the vertical bars. Auditory stimuli were presented through a speaker, out of sight of the subjects. Reward delivery and presentation of visual and auditory stimuli were controlled by the application MonkeyCantab (R. N. Cardinal), using the Whisker control system (Cardinal and Aitken, 2010).

Behavioral training

Preoperative training

Subjects were trained to enter the transport box and habituated to the test apparatus. After successful habituation, animals were familiarized with the liquid reward, learned the association between an auditory stimulus and access to reward, and then acquired a touchscreen response for that reward; all previously described in Roberts et al. (1988). Subsequently, animals were trained on a spatial self-ordered sequencing task (Fig. 1B) designed in Monkey Cantab by A.C.R. and T.W.R., in which they were required to select each of an array of identical stimuli presented on the screen, once only. Subjects were first trained to touch a stimulus, presented in a random spatial location for each trial, based on an eight-location grid. Once animals performed 20 trials in a session, task difficulty was gradually increased. The first step of subsequent training was the addition of a second identical stimulus in a distinct spatial location, and animals were required to respond to both spatial locations, sequentially, in any order, to receive reward. Once a response was made to a stimulus, that stimulus disappeared for a set amount of time, denoted “vanishing time” (vt). Animals were allowed to continue responding throughout the vt, but if they responded to the same stimulus more than once, the trial ended prematurely, the houselight was turned off for 3 s, and the trial was scored as incorrect. Vt was gradually decreased during training, and the number of stimuli was increased until animals could perform 20 two-stimuli trials with an accuracy of 80% and then 30–40 three-stimuli trials with an accuracy ≥50%, both with a vt = 0.5 s.

Postoperative training

After surgery, a stable baseline was established in animals on the final version of the variable array, with the most difficult parameters; three visual stimuli with a vt of 0.5 s. When animals consistently performed above 50% accuracy and were habituated to their holders (experimental staff that would gently hold them for infusions) such that task accuracy was not impaired, they began receiving infusions into either the caudate nucleus, the putamen, or combined caudate and putamen.

Task versions: variable array versus fixed array

Two versions of a spatial self-ordered sequencing task were used for these experiments involving either a variable array (Fig. 1C) or a fixed array (Fig. 1D), as used previously (Axelsson et al., 2021). In the variable array, stimuli appeared randomly in three of eight possible locations which varied across trials. In the fixed array, animals were presented with three stimuli that always appeared in the same locations across trials and sessions. The variable array required animals to flexibly create and apply a plan to action a correct self-ordered spatial sequence, thereby applying a generalized abstract schema to solve the problem. The fixed array allowed animals to develop an optimal response strategy (or sequence) to complete the task in a more habitual manner. Both tasks required subjects to self-order and execute a sequence of responses to three identical, but spatially distinct, stimuli presented on a screen, touching each stimulus once and only once, with a vt of 0.5 s. If the animal touched the same stimulus twice within a trial—either twice consecutively (a continuous perseverative error) or returned to the first stimulus after selecting two different stimuli (a recurrent perseverative error)—the trial was deemed incorrect, terminated early, and the animal experienced 3 s of darkness before the next trial commenced.

Spatial locations of the three stimuli in the fixed array were selected specifically for each animal, thus while constant across trials and sessions for an individual animal, it varied between animals (Extended Data Table 1-1A). By presenting the same array to an animal repeatedly across trials and sessions, it was hypothesized that they would develop a preferred sequence strategy across time to reduce working memory load. Having shown development of a response sequence strategy, the effects of striatal manipulations would then be compared with that seen on variable arrays where there was a greater working memory load. Thus, the set of three stimuli chosen for each animal was based on their overall performance of this particular array during variable task 1. First, subjects had to have performed at least five of the six possible correct response sequences for the proposed array. Second, animals did not show a strong preference for any particular one of the response sequences (i.e., the Shannon entropy value was as close to that predicted if responses were random, 2.58 bits). Third, animals had an accuracy score for the specific fixed sequence of at least 50%, which was considerably superior to chance performance (22%).

Surgery

Animals had permanent indwelling cannulae implanted to allow infusion of drugs into the caudate nucleus, putamen, or both together. For surgery, animals were premedicated with 0.1 ml of 100 mg/ml ketamine (Ketavet, Henry Schein Medical) and given prophylactic analgesic (0.075 ml of 50 mg/ml subcutaneous metacam, Pfizer) before being intubated (using Intubeaze 20 mg/ml lidocaine hydrochloride spray, Dechra Veterinary Products) and anesthesia maintained using a mixture of vaporized isoflurane (Novartis Animal Health) and O2 (2.25% isoflurane in 0.3 L/min O2). Animals were then placed in a marmoset stereotaxic frame (David Kopf). Heart rate, O2 saturation, respiratory rate, and CO2 saturation were all monitored by pulse oximetry and capnography (Microcap Handheld Capnograph, Oridion Capnography) while core body temperature was monitored rectally (TES-1319K-type digital thermometer, TES Electrical Electronic).

Cortical depth was measured at +17.5 on the anterior–posterior (AP) axis, and −1.5 on the latero-medial (LM) axis to allow for corrections to cannula target placements, as previously described in Dias et al. (1997). Double guide cannulae measuring 7 mm in length with a center–center distance of 1.4 mm (Plastics One) were then inserted, with the medial guide at AP +11 LM ±3.3. Precise locations of all cannulae in all animals were determined using postmortem histology (Fig. 2). Guides were fixed in place by skull screws and dental acrylic (Simplex Rapid, Kemdent Works). Postsurgically, subjects were administered 0.18 ml of 3.8 mg/ml dexamethasone (0.09 ml injected into each quadricep; Aspen Pharma Trading). Subjects were also given analgesic once daily in the morning, for 3 d after surgery (meloxicam, 0.1 ml of a 1.5 mg/ml oral suspension; Boehringer Ingelheim). After surgery, animals had ad libitum access to water for at least 2 weeks and were provided food that was otherwise only available to them on weekends.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Cannulae placements for each of the six experimental marmosets located within the vlPFC projection zone in the caudate and putamen. Males are open symbols, females filled symbols. Square, M1 and M2; triangle, M3 and M4; inverted triangle, M5 and M6.

Drug preparation and treatment

For drug infusions, animals were gently restrained by a familiar person (holder) other than the researcher and taken to a designated infusion room. The researcher gently removed caps and dummies from cannula guides and cleaned the guides with injection wipes. For all infusions, an injector (Plastics One) was used that protruded +0.1 mm from the cannula directed to the caudate nucleus or +0.3 mm from the cannula directed to the putamen. The injectors were connected to a 10 µl Hamilton syringe (701RN; Hamilton) via PTFE tubing (0.3 mm in diameter). Solvent flexible tubing was used to connect PFTE tubing to injector and syringe (0.38 mm in inner diameter, Elkay Laboratory Products). Drug was accurately delivered by an infusion pump (KDS230, KD Scientific). Injectors, tubing, and syringes were all sterilized before setup.

CNQX disodium salt (Tocris; MW 312.15), an AMPA receptor antagonist, was dissolved in sterile saline (Hameln) to a 3 mM solution and further diluted in sterile saline to 1 mM solution. Solutions were filtered (Whatman Uniflo syringe filter; 0.2 µm pore; Cytiva) and aliquoted into sterile Eppendorfs for immediate use or storage at −20°C for up to 1 month. After 1 month, new solutions were made. For control infusions, sterile saline was infused instead of CNQX.

Experimental design, measurements, and statistical analysis

Animals performed the task once daily on Monday through to Friday every week, at approximately the same time each day. A within-subject study design was used whereby each animal performed both tasks and received all control and drug infusions across all brain regions. All animals started on the more difficult variable array (Task 1) and moved to the fixed array (Task 2) with some animals returning to variable after being on fixed (detailed in Extended Data Table 1-1B). Drug dose and brain region were counterbalanced across all animals, as summarized in Extended Data Table1-1C–F. Typically, animals received a mock infusion on Wednesday, saline infusion on Thursday, and a CNQX infusion on Friday. Occasionally, animals received a mock infusion on Tuesday, saline infusion on Wednesday, and CNQX on Thursday. A paired t test revealed no significant difference between saline control infusions for 1 mM and 3 mM CNQX, and thus control performance was averaged for each animal for each brain region for comparisons to 1 and 3 mM CNQX.

Before infusions began, animals were required to reach stable performance such that it was reliable across the proposed infusion days and was not affected by handling of the animal prior to sessions with variable (Extended Data Fig. 4-1) and fixed (Extended Data Fig. 4-2) arrays. Animals received acute infusions of low concentration (1 mM) or high concentration (3 mM) of CNQX (an AMPA/kainate glutamate receptor competitive antagonist) or saline into either the caudate nucleus, putamen, or both together. Since most of the input into the striatum is glutamatergic, CNQX infusions acted to inhibit that input. The effects of these infusions on task performance were investigated.

Response variables measured include the number of trials completed, accuracy, and type and number of errors performed, and omissions. Trials completed were the total number of trials performed, including correct, incorrect, and omissions. If three consecutive omissions were made (i.e., the animal did not respond to stimuli presented for 60 s on three consecutive occasions), the session ended.

Accuracy was the number of sequences performed correctly divided by the number of trials completed minus the number of omissions (correct trials / (total trials − omissions)). Errors per session were grouped together based on whether they were continuous or recurrent. Error types were presented as a percentage of total trials [continuous errors / (total trials − omissions); recurrent errors / (total trials − omissions)].

Entropy was calculated for the fixed array after selecting only correct responses and analyzing the proportion of each of the six possible correct patterns employed by the animals using the following equation:H(X)=∑i=1np(xi)logb1p(xi) Testing data were collected in a Microsoft Access database. Data were extracted and organized, using RStudio [version 4.1.2 (2021-11-01), RStudio: Integrated Development for R; RStudio] and Microsoft Excel (Office 16). Statistical analysis and graphical representation were performed in GraphPad Prism (version 9.3.1 for Windows, GraphPad Software). Data are presented as mean values ± SEM. One-way or two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs were performed on data followed by post hoc corrections for multiple comparisons, as appropriate and indicated in figure legends. Results were considered significant when p < 0.05. For all data displayed in Figures 4 and 5, a two-way ANOVA with factors of treatment (saline, 1 mM CNQX, 3 mM CNQX) and brain region (caudate nucleus, putamen, or combined caudate and putamen) were employed, followed by Sidak's multiple-comparisons test.

Results

Accuracy on the fixed array task was greater than that on the variable array task, a likely consequence of increased automaticity of responding

Marmoset accuracy on the variable array improved significantly after training when comparing initial, to final, task performance after control saline infusions (Fig. 3A). This performance then reached asymptote, remaining stable for the duration of the variable array component of the study, as confirmed by a one-way rmANOVA which revealed no main effect across time on accuracy of performance measured across all saline control infusions (F(5,25) = 0.3756; p = 0.8606; data not shown). Overall performance improved after transfer from variable to fixed array (Fig. 3B), where animals reached asymptote at a significantly higher level for the fixed array (Fig. 3C). This increased accuracy was associated with a decrease in entropy (Fig. 3D) which occurred because marmosets developed a preferred set of “correct” response sequences out of the six possible “correct” sequences available. Development of a preferred pattern of responding is consistent with the animals’ performance of the fixed array task becoming increasingly automated. Post-training, accuracy remained stable for the duration of the fixed array component of the study, as determined by a one-way rmANOVA which revealed no main effect of time on accuracy of performance across saline control infusions (F(5,25) = 1.808; p = 0.1477; data not shown).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Task performance accuracy on variable and fixed array tasks (A–C) and entropy measures across training compared with saline infusion performance on the fixed task (D). Arrow on the y-axis indicates the entropy score if correct sequences were selected randomly (2.58 bits). For the variable array (A) one-way rmANOVA revealed a main effect of training stage on accuracy (F(2,10) = 20.45; p = 0.0003); Sidak-corrected post hoc showed the following: Training Stage 1 versus Final Performance (p = 0.0032) and Training Stage 2 versus Final Performance (p = 0.0003). For the fixed array (B) one-way rmANOVA showed a main effect of training stage on accuracy (F(2,10) = 8.512; p = 0.0069); Sidak-corrected post hoc showed the following: Selected Sequence versus Final Performance (p = 0.0099) and Training Stage 1 versus Final Performance (p = 0.0263). The significant difference between variable and fixed array accuracy (C) was determined via paired t test (p = 0.0014). Finally, a one-way rmANOVA showed a main effect of training stage on entropy (F(2,10) = 20.81; p = 0.0003); Sidak-corrected post hoc showed the following: Selected Sequence versus Initial Training (p = 0.0044) and Selected Sequence versus Final Performance (p = 0.0003). Data are represented as mean ± standard error of mean and Sidak comparisons indicated by *p > 0.05, **p > 0.01, and ***p > 0.001; ns, not significant. Data are represented as mean ± standard error of mean. Data were collected from six monkeys, each with their own symbol as designated in Figure 2.

High-dose CNQX (3 mM) impaired accuracy on both variable and fixed array tasks in the caudate nucleus, putamen, or both combined

High-dose CNQX (3 mM) significantly impaired accuracy in both variable and fixed arrays regardless of striatal location (Fig. 4). On the variable array (Fig. 4A), 3 mM CNQX impaired accuracy to similar extents when infused into either the caudate or putamen alone. However, when combined, the impairments were additive, leading to a greater decline in accuracy than when either the caudate or putamen received 3 mM CNQX alone. In the fixed array (Fig. 4B), these same manipulations caused a decline in accuracy, but the effect appeared greatest when the putamen was targeted alone.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Effects of 1 or 3 mM CNQX into either the caudate nucleus, the putamen, or combined caudate and putamen on variable (A) and fixed (B) array accuracy. Data were collected across all regions and both CNQX doses from six monkeys, each with their own symbol defined in the key. For the variable array (A) two-way rmANOVA revealed a main effect of treatment (F(2,10) = 13.04; p = 0.0016); but no effect of brain region (F < 1). Sidak-corrected post hoc showed the following: caudate saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0088), putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0073), combined caudate + putamen saline versus 1 or 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0064; p < 0.0001, respectively). The fixed array (B), two-way rmANOVA revealed a main effect of treatment (F(2,10) = 23.91; p = 0.0002) but no effect of brain region F(2,10) = 2.495; p = 0.13211); Sidak-corrected post hoc showed the following: caudate saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.265), putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0007), and combined caudate + putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0033). Data are represented as mean ± standard error of mean and Sidak comparisons indicated by *p > 0.05, **p > 0.01, ***p > 0.001; or ***p > 0.0001. Additional control data for the variable and fixed arrays are presented in Extended Data Figure 4-1 and Extended Data Figure 4-2, respectively.

Figure 4-1

Typical experimental schedule for monkeys on the variable array (A). Control experiments showing that accuracy (B-D) and error types (E-J) are not affected by day of the week (B, E, H) or the animal being held (C, F, I). In addition, receiving an infusion (saline) or being held for consecutive days (D, G, J) has no effect on Friday performance (when CNQX infusions are performed). Each data point represents an average of three measurements for the weekday baseline measures (B, E, H); an average of 6-10 values for no-handling-mock comparisons (C, F, I), and single values for Mock/Saline/Mock comparisons (D, G, and J). Each symbol on a graph represents an individual monkey. Paired t-tests (n=3) showed that there was no difference in accuracy (p>0.05), or errors of the continuous (p>0.05) or recurrent (p>0.05) kind (B, E, H); nor were there significant effects (p>0.05) on accuracy or either error type when the animal was handled during mocks compared to no handling (C, F, I). A one-way rmANOVA revealed no main effects after being held on consecutive days/receiving a control infusion on accuracy (F(2, 6) <1), or errors of the continuous (F(2, 2) = 10.33; p=0.0882) or recurrent (F(1, 1) = 2.385; p=0.3658) kind (D, G, J). Download Figure 4-1, TIF file.

Figure 4-2

Typical experimental schedule for monkeys on the fixed array (A). Control experiments showing that accuracy (B-D) and error types (E-J) are not affected by day of the week (B, E, H) or the animal being held (C, F, I). In addition, receiving an infusion (saline) or being held for consecutive days (D, G, J) has no effect on Friday performance (when CNQX infusions are performed). Each data point represents an average of three measurements for the weekday baseline measures (B, E, H); an average of 6-10 values for baseline-mock comparisons (C, F, I), and single values for Mock/Saline/Mock graphs (D, G, and J). Each symbol on the graphs indicates a different monkey. A one-way rmANOVA revealed no main effect after being held on consecutive days/receiving a control infusion on accuracy (F (2, 6) = 3-355; p=0.1052), or errors of the continuous (F (2, 2) = 7; p=0.1250) or recurrent (F<1) kind (D, G, J). A one-way rmANOVA revealed no main effect of day of the week on accuracy (F<1), or errors of the continuous (F<1) or recurrent (F<1) kind (B, E, H). A paired t-test showed no significant effects (p>0.05) on accuracy or either error type when the animal was handled during mocks compared to no handling (C, F, I). A one-way rmANOVA revealed no main effect after being held on consecutive days/receiving a control infusion on accuracy (F <1), or errors of the continuous (F <1) or recurrent (F<1) kind (D, G, J). Download Figure 4-2, TIF file.

High-dose CNQX (3 mM) had differing effects on error types in both variable and fixed arrays depending on brain region(s) targeted

For the variable array (Fig. 5A,B), there was an increase in “continuous,” but not “recurrent,” errors after CNQX into the putamen. In contrast, there was no effect on continuous errors after CNQX into the caudate (Fig. 5A), though recurrent errors trended toward an increase (p = 0.0701; Fig. 5B). Continuous errors significantly increased when the caudate and putamen simultaneously received 3 mM CNQX (Fig. 5A), which resembled the effect seen when the putamen alone was targeted. Similarly, there was a trend toward increased recurrent errors after combined targeting of the caudate and putamen (p = 0.0755; Fig. 5B), comparable with when the caudate was targeted alone.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Effects of 1 or 3 mM CNQX into either the caudate nucleus, the putamen, or combined caudate and putamen on perseverative errors of the continuous (A,C) and recurrent type (B,D) in variable (A,B) and fixed (C,D) arrays. Data were collected across all timepoints from six monkeys, each with their own symbol defined in the key. For the variable array, two-way rmANOVAs revealed a main effect of treatment on continuous (F(2,10) = 4.694; p = 0.0365) and recurrent errors (F(2,10) = 7.287; p = 0.0112) but no effect on brain region (F < 1 for both continuous and recurrent errors). Sidak-corrected post hoc showed for variable array continuous errors the following: putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0011) and combined caudate + putamen saline versus 1 mM or 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0164; p = 0.0004, respectively). For variable array recurrent errors, it revealed the following: caudate saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0701) and combined caudate + putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0755). The fixed array two-way rmANOVAs revealed a main effect of treatment on both continuous (F(2,10) = 6.481; p = 0.0157) and recurrent errors (F(2,10) = 15.56; p = 0.0009) but no effect of brain region (F(2,10) = 2.477; p = 0.1337 for continuous errors, and F < 1 for recurrent errors). Sidak-corrected post hoc showed for fixed array continuous errors as follows: caudate saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0429) and putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0029). For fixed array recurrent errors, it revealed: putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0069) and combined caudate + putamen saline versus 3 mM CNQX (p = 0.0062). Data are represented as mean ± standard error of mean and Sidak comparisons indicated by *p > 0.05, **p > 0.01, or ***p > 0.001.

In the fixed array (Fig. 5C,D), continuous errors increased significantly after 3 mM CNQX into the caudate, though recurrent errors were unaffected. However, both continuous and recurrent errors increased after 3 mM CNQX into the putamen. While there was only a trend (p = 0.0748) toward increased continuous errors after 3 mM CNQX infusion into the caudate and putamen combined (Fig. 5C), there was a significant increase in recurrent errors (Fig. 5D).

Low-dose CNQX (1 mM) only impaired accuracy on variable arrays following combined infusions into the caudate and putamen

Low-dose CNQX (1 mM) into either the caudate nucleus or putamen individually did not impair accuracy for either variable or fixed arrays (Fig. 4). However, for the variable array (Fig. 4A), when both the caudate nucleus and putamen received 1 mM CNQX simultaneously, accuracy was significantly impaired, associated with an increase in continuous errors (Fig. 5A). In contrast, the same combined manipulation did not affect accuracy in the fixed array (Fig. 4B).

It should also be noted that no experimental interventions affected the fixed array entropy measures; the largest F value was F(2,10) 3.18; p = 0.0853. A summary of the main experimental findings from this study are shown in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Summary of effects seen after infusions on the variable (A) and fixed arrays (B)

Discussion

Previously, inactivation of the vlPFC impaired spatial self-ordered sequencing in the variable, but not fixed array (Axelsson et al., 2021). In this study, only low-dose CNQX, targeting vlPFC downstream glutamatergic striatal inputs, selectively impaired variable array performance and only when infused into the caudate and putamen combined. In contrast, we generally found impairments in both tasks with high-dose CNQX, which impaired accuracy on both variable and fixed arrays in the caudate, the putamen, or both combined. There were qualitatively different effects on error types though, dependent on striatal region and whether the array was variable or fixed. Specifically, CNQX into the putamen, but not caudate, selectively increased continuous perseverative responses in the variable array, which requires the animal to use an overall schema, plan, or strategy to guide performance. Instead, caudate infusions selectively led to an upward (nonsignificant) trend in recurrent perseverative errors in the same array. In the case of the putamen, these selective effects on continuous perseverative errors could be attributed to response sequencing deficits resulting from a failure to monitor motor feedback from the immediately preceding response, thus resulting in motor perseveration (Kubota et al., 2009; Yin et al., 2009; Yin, 2010; Turner et al., 2022). In contrast, for the caudate nucleus, the trend toward increased recurrent errors instead likely reflects interference in spatial working memory, broadly consistent with predicted roles of the caudate nucleus in working memory (Kermadi and Joseph, 1995; Levy et al., 1997; Postle and D’Esposito, 1999; Collins et al., 2000; Lewis et al., 2004; Grahn et al., 2008; Akhlaghpour et al., 2016).

A greater role for the striatum than the vlPFC in controlling responding in the fixed array was hypothesized since the putamen, in particular, has been implicated in learning and performance of skilled or habitual motor sequences in rodents (Kubota et al., 2009; Yin et al., 2009; Yin, 2010; Turner et al., 2022). In the present study, we demonstrate development of greater response automaticity in the fixed array as evidenced by a decrease in entropy, suggesting skilled or habitual control (Dezfouli et al., 2014). Consistent with the rodent evidence, there was some indication here that glutamate receptor blockade in the putamen produced deficits that were greater than in the caudate for the fixed array task and comparable with combined infusions into the caudate and putamen (Fig. 4B). Moreover, the effects of putamen infusions on continuous errors were greater than for either the caudate, or combined infusions (Fig. 5C). However, it should be noted that there were evidently some marginal detrimental effects of caudate CNQX infusions on fixed array performance, and we did not demonstrate greater selectivity in the putamen at the lower dose of CNQX.

For the variable array, both caudate and putamen infusions impaired performance, with combined infusions being additive even at low-dose (as well as high-dose) CNQX, unlike for the fixed array (Fig. 4) where effects were not additive at either dose. This is consistent with evidence that the vlPFC projects to both striatal regions targeted by these infusions (Roberts et al., 2007) which may suggest a combined role in flexible responding. Indeed, a human fMRI study demonstrated that anterior putamen (in addition to caudate) is active during the planning stage of variable responses to numerical sequences (Jankowski et al., 2009). However, given that the caudate and putamen have opposing roles in reversal learning in marmoset (Jackson et al., 2019) and in response sequencing in rat (Turner et al., 2022), it is also plausible that the two regions mediate distinct aspects of performance. In the present study, the pattern of perseverative errors differed for the two sites: the putamen being associated with elevated continuous errors and caudate being associated with increased recurrent errors. This is consistent with inactivation of the dorsolateral striatum (i.e., rodent putamen) inducing continuous errors in a serial order task (Rothwell et al., 2015) and a role of the dorsomedial striatum (i.e., rodent caudate) in self-monitoring during execution of goal-directed action sequences (Vandaele et al., 2021). Such a pattern implies deficits in spatial working memory and impaired motor control, respectively (see above), although it should be noted that both tasks were designed to minimize the load of holding online motor sequences in working memory (Collins et al., 1998). A more general strategic deficit in applying a strategy or schema to the sequencing task (Owen et al., 1990) might be implicated instead. Inactivation of the vlPFC or antagonism of its 5-HT2A receptors increased only recurrent errors in variable arrays, although dopamine D2 receptor antagonism did enhance continuous perseverative errors (Axelsson et al., 2021). Hence, it is possible that different projections of the vlPFC to the striatum contribute to different aspects of sequencing performance in the variable condition. Of course, it is also plausible that influence of other cortical or thalamic projections to these regions of the striatum was impaired by intrastriatal blockade of glutamate receptors. Indeed, the lack of effect of vlPFC infusions on fixed array performance suggests that striatal involvement in this task is mediated by different cortical inputs, hypothetically involving cortical sensorimotor regions (Sánchez-Fuentes et al., 2021).

The striatum receives vast numbers of glutamatergic inputs from cortical and subcortical regions which converge anatomically with dopaminergic afferents from the midbrain. All major glutamate ionotropic [NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartate), kainate, and AMPA] as well as metabotropic receptors are represented. AMPA receptors in the striatum are located on terminals of corticostriatal afferents (Fujiyama et al., 2004) and act to regulate glutamate release through a positive feedback mechanism (Patel et al., 2001; Fujiyama et al., 2004). To this effect, 1 mM CNQX has successfully been used in the dorsal striatum of rats to alter the balance between goal-directed and habitual control of behavior (Furlong et al., 2014), as well as altering control of goal-directed behavior following intracaudate infusions in marmosets (Duan et al., 2021).

Limitations

A possible limitation of the present study is the pharmacological specificity of CNQX. Effects were mainly observed after 3 mM CNQX infusions. While CNQX is relatively selective for AMPA receptors (IC value 400 nM) with additional actions on kainate receptors (IC, 4 µM), and at high concentrations, NMDA receptors (Lester et al., 1988; Stein and Verdoorn, 1992; Brickley et al., 2001), it is likely that, at concentrations used here, CNQX nonselectively impaired striatal glutamatergic transmission. This might include, for example, NMDA-mediated processes of neuronal plasticity, such as long-term potentiation (Park et al., 2014) or long-term depression (Lüscher and Malenka, 2012).

A second limitation is that we have not specified the precise cortical–striatal circuitry implicated in this and previous studies (Axelsson et al., 2021). Future studies will employ chemogenetics to address pathway specificity (Wood et al., 2023).

Conclusions

By employing local intrastriatal CNQX infusions to block glutamatergic transmission, we implicate marmoset striatal regions (caudate and putamen) in self-ordered sequencing behavior. Both flexible responding driven by caudate and more rigid habitual responding driven by the putamen are differentially required for marmosets to successfully self-order a sequence in variable and fixed arrays. The caudate and putamen have additive roles in variable (but not fixed) array self-ordered sequencing, and infusions into the two regions had differing effects on types of perseverative error. By understanding the basic neural circuitry underlying such sequencing abilities, we may better understand how to improve them in people with deficits associated with neuropsychiatric disorders, such as obsessive–compulsive disorder, as well as basal ganglia disorders.

Footnotes

  • T.W.R. discloses consultancy and royalties for Cambridge Cognition as well as consultancy for Supernus. He has received research grants from Shionogi and editorial honoraria from Elsevier and Springer Nature. A.C.R. has received research grants from Shionogi.

  • This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust (Grant 104631/Z/14/Z to T.W.R.). For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright license to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

  • ↵* T.W.R. and A.C.R. are the joint senior authors.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Akhlaghpour H,
    2. Wiskerke J,
    3. Choi JY,
    4. Taliaferro JP,
    5. Au J,
    6. Witten IB
    (2016) Dissociated sequential activity and stimulus encoding in the dorsomedial striatum during spatial working memory. Elife 5:e19507. doi:10.7554/eLife.19507
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Alexander GE,
    2. DeLong MR,
    3. Strick PL
    (1986) Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 9:357–381. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Axelsson SFA,
    2. Horst NK,
    3. Horiguchi N,
    4. Roberts AC,
    5. Robbins TW
    (2021) Flexible versus fixed spatial self-ordered response sequencing: effects of inactivation and neurochemical modulation of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 41:7246–7258. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0227-21.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Balleine BW,
    2. O’Doherty JP
    (2010) Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacol 35:48–69. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.131
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Banca P,
    2. Ruiz MH,
    3. Gonzalez-Zalba MF,
    4. Biria M,
    5. Marzuki AA,
    6. Piercy T,
    7. Sule A,
    8. Fineberg NA,
    9. Robbins TW
    (2023) Action-sequence learning, habits and automaticity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Elife 12:RP87346. doi:10.7554/eLife.87346.2
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    1. Brickley SG,
    2. Farrant M,
    3. Swanson GT,
    4. Cull-Candy SG
    (2001) CNQX increases GABA-mediated synaptic transmission in the cerebellum by an AMPA/kainate receptor-independent mechanism. Neuropharmacology 41:730–736. doi:10.1016/S0028-3908(01)00135-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Cardinal RN,
    2. Aitken MRF
    (2010) Whisker: a client-server high-performance multimedia research control system. Behav Res Methods 42:1059–1071. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.4.1059
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Choi EY,
    2. Tanimura Y,
    3. Vage PR,
    4. Yates EH,
    5. Haber SN
    (2017) Convergence of prefrontal and parietal anatomical projections in a connectional hub in the striatum. NeuroImage 146:821–832. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.037
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Collins P,
    2. Roberts AC,
    3. Dias R,
    4. Everitt BJ,
    5. Robbins TW
    (1998) Perseveration and strategy in a novel spatial self-ordered sequencing task for nonhuman primates: effects of excitotoxic lesions and dopamine depletions of the prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 10:332–354. doi:10.1162/089892998562771
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Collins P,
    2. Wilkinson LS,
    3. Everitt BJ,
    4. Robbins TW,
    5. Roberts AC
    (2000) The effect of dopamine depletion from the caudate nucleus of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) on tests of prefrontal cognitive function. Behav Neurosci 114:3–17. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.114.1.3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Conen KE,
    2. Desrochers TM
    (2022) Unpacking self-ordered sequences. Neuron 110:566–568. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2022.01.022
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. ↵
    1. Cooper JA,
    2. Sagar HJ,
    3. Jordan N,
    4. Harvey NS,
    5. Sullivan EV
    (1991) Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson’s disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain J Neurol 114:2095–2122. doi:10.1093/brain/114.5.2095
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Dezfouli A,
    2. Lingawi NW,
    3. Balleine BW
    (2014) Habits as action sequences: hierarchical action control and changes in outcome value. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 369:20130482. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0482
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Dias R,
    2. Robbins TW,
    3. Roberts AC
    (1997) Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin card sort test: restriction to novel situations and independence from “on-line” processing. J Neurosci 17:9285–9297. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-09285.1997
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Duan LY,
    2. Horst NK,
    3. Cranmore SAW,
    4. Horiguchi N,
    5. Cardinal RN,
    6. Roberts AC,
    7. Robbins TW
    (2021) Controlling one’s world: identification of sub-regions of primate PFC underlying goal-directed behavior. Neuron 109:2485–2498.e5. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2021.06.003
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Fujiyama F,
    2. Kuramoto E,
    3. Okamoto K,
    4. Hioki H,
    5. Furuta T,
    6. Zhou L,
    7. Nomura S,
    8. Kaneko T
    (2004) Presynaptic localization of an AMPA-type glutamate receptor in corticostriatal and thalamostriatal axon terminals. Eur J Neurosci 20:3322–3330. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03807.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Furlong TM,
    2. Jayaweera HK,
    3. Balleine BW,
    4. Corbit LH
    (2014) Binge-like consumption of a palatable food accelerates habitual control of behavior and is dependent on activation of the dorsolateral striatum. J Neurosci 34:5012–5022. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3707-13.2014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Grahn JA,
    2. Parkinson JA,
    3. Owen AM
    (2008) The cognitive functions of the caudate nucleus. Prog Neurobiol 86:141–155. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Jackson SAW,
    2. Horst NK,
    3. Axelsson SFA,
    4. Horiguchi N,
    5. Cockcroft GJ,
    6. Robbins TW,
    7. Roberts AC
    (2019) Selective role of the putamen in serial reversal learning in the marmoset. Cereb Cortex 29:447–460. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy276
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Jahanshahi M,
    2. Rowe J,
    3. Saleem T,
    4. Brown RG,
    5. Limousin-Dowsey P,
    6. Rothwell JC,
    7. Thomas DGT,
    8. Quinn NP
    (2002) Striatal contribution to cognition: working memory and executive function in Parkinson’s disease before and after unilateral posteroventral pallidotomy. J Cogn Neurosci 14:298–310. doi:10.1162/089892902317236911
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Jankowski J,
    2. Scheef L,
    3. Huppe C,
    4. Boecker H
    (2009) Distinct striatal regions for planning and executing novel and automated movement sequences. NeuroImage 44:1369–1379. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.059
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Kermadi I,
    2. Joseph JP
    (1995) Activity in the caudate nucleus of monkey during spatial sequencing. J Neurophysiol 74:911–933. doi:10.1152/jn.1995.74.3.911
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Kubota Y,
    2. Liu J,
    3. Hu D,
    4. DeCoteau WE,
    5. Eden UT,
    6. Smith AC,
    7. Graybiel AM
    (2009) Stable encoding of task structure coexists with flexible coding of task events in sensorimotor striatum. J Neurophysiol 102:2142–2160. doi:10.1152/jn.00522.2009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Lafleche G,
    2. Albert MS
    (1995) Executive function deficits in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 9:313–320. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.9.3.313
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Lester AJ,
    2. Quarum L,
    3. Parker D,
    4. Jahr E
    (1988) Interaction of 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2, 3-dione with the N-methyl-o-aspartate receptor-associated glycine binding site.
  26. ↵
    1. Levy R,
    2. Friedman HR,
    3. Davachi L,
    4. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1997) Differential activation of the caudate nucleus in primates performing spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks. J Neurosci 17:3870–3882. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-10-03870.1997
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Lewis SJG,
    2. Dove A,
    3. Robbins TW,
    4. Barker RA,
    5. Owen AM
    (2004) Striatal contributions to working memory: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in humans. Eur J Neurosci 19:755–760. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03108.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Lüscher C,
    2. Malenka RC
    (2012) NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation and long-term depression (LTP/LTD). Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 4:a005710. doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a005710
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Owen AM,
    2. Downes JJ,
    3. Sahakian BJ,
    4. Polkey CE,
    5. Robbins TW
    (1990) Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 28:1021–1034. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-D
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Pantelis C,
    2. Barnes TR,
    3. Nelson HE,
    4. Tanner S,
    5. Weatherley L,
    6. Owen AM,
    7. Robbins TW
    (1997) Frontal-striatal cognitive deficits in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Brain J Neurol 120:1823–1843. doi:10.1093/brain/120.10.1823
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Park H,
    2. Popescu A,
    3. Poo M
    (2014) Essential role of presynaptic NMDA receptors in activity-dependent BDNF secretion and corticostriatal LTP. Neuron 84:1009–1022. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.045
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Passingham RE
    (1985) Memory of monkeys (Macaca mulatta) with lesions in prefrontal cortex. Behav Neurosci 99:3–21. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.99.1.3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Patel DR,
    2. Young AMJ,
    3. Croucher MJ
    (2001) Presynaptic α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionate receptor-mediated stimulation of glutamate and GABA release in the rat striatum in vivo: a dual-label microdialysis study. Neuroscience 102:101–111. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00463-2
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    1. Petrides M
    (1995) Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey. J Neurosci 15:359. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-01-00359.1995
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Petrides M,
    2. Milner B
    (1982) Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 20:249–262. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(82)90100-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Postle B,
    2. D’Esposito M
    (1999) Dissociation of human caudate nucleus activity in spatial and nonspatial working memory: an event-related fMRI study. Cogn Brain Res 8:107–115. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00010-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Roberts AC,
    2. Robbins TW,
    3. Everitt BJ
    (1988) The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates. Q J Exp Psychol B 40:321–341.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Roberts AC,
    2. Tomic DL,
    3. Parkinson CH,
    4. Roeling TA,
    5. Cutter DJ,
    6. Robbins TW,
    7. Everitt BJ
    (2007) Forebrain connectivity of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus): an anterograde and retrograde tract-tracing study. J Comp Neurol 502:86–112. doi:10.1002/cne.21300
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Rothwell PE,
    2. Hayton SJ,
    3. Sun GL,
    4. Fuccillo MV,
    5. Lim BK,
    6. Malenka RC
    (2015) Input- and output-specific regulation of serial order performance by corticostriatal circuits. Neuron 88:345–356. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.035
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Sánchez-Fuentes A,
    2. Ramírez-Armenta KI,
    3. Verma-Rodríguez AK,
    4. Díaz-Hernández E,
    5. Aguilar-Palomares A,
    6. Ramírez-Jarquín JO,
    7. Tecuapetla F
    (2021) The contribution of premotor cortico-striatal projections to the execution of serial order sequences. eNeuro 8. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0173-21.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Sandson J,
    2. Albert ML
    (1984) Varieties of perseveration. Neuropsychologia 22:715–732. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(84)90098-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Smith ID,
    2. Todd MJ,
    3. Beninger RJ
    (1996) Glutamate receptor agonist injections into the dorsal striatum cause contralateral turning in the rat: involvement of kainate and AMPA receptors. Eur J Pharmacol 301:7–17. doi:10.1016/0014-2999(96)00024-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Stein E,
    2. Verdoorn A
    (1992) Complex pharmacological properties of recombinant a-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionate receptor subtypes.
  44. ↵
    1. Turner KM,
    2. Svegborn A,
    3. Langguth M,
    4. McKenzie C,
    5. Robbins TW
    (2022) Opposing roles of the dorsolateral and dorsomedial striatum in the acquisition of skilled action sequencing in rats. J Neurosci 42:2039–2051. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1907-21.2022
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Vaghi MM,
    2. Hampshire A,
    3. Fineberg NA,
    4. Kaser M,
    5. Brühl AB,
    6. Sahakian BJ,
    7. Chamberlain SR,
    8. Robbins TW
    (2017) Hypoactivation and dysconnectivity of a frontostriatal circuit during goal-directed planning as an endophenotype for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2:655–663. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.05.005
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    1. Vandaele Y,
    2. Ottenheimer DJ,
    3. Janak PH
    (2021) Dorsomedial striatal activity tracks completion of behavioral sequences in rats. eNeuro 8. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0279-21.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    1. Walker SC,
    2. Robbins TW,
    3. Roberts AC
    (2009) Response disengagement on a spatial self-ordered sequencing task: effects of regionally selective excitotoxic lesions and serotonin depletion within the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 29:6033–6041. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0312-09.2009
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Wood CM,
    2. Alexander L,
    3. Alsiö J,
    4. Santangelo AM,
    5. McIver L,
    6. Cockcroft GJ,
    7. Roberts AC
    (2023) Chemogenetics identifies separate area 25 brain circuits involved in anhedonia and anxiety in marmosets. Sci Transl Med 15:eade1779. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.ade1779
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  49. ↵
    1. Yin HH
    (2010) The sensorimotor striatum is necessary for serial order learning. J Neurosci 30:14719–14723. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3989-10.2010
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Yin HH,
    2. Mulcare SP,
    3. Hilário MRF,
    4. Clouse E,
    5. Holloway T,
    6. Davis MI,
    7. Hansson AC,
    8. Lovinger DM,
    9. Costa RM
    (2009) Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nat Neurosci 12:333–341. doi:10.1038/nn.2261
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Mark Laubach, American University

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: NONE.

Thank you for submitting your study to eNeuro! It received favorable reviews and I will assess your revisions before making a decision. The full reviews are given below. There are four issues to address:

1. The description of effects of infusions into the caudate, that the statistical results were not significant.

2. Reframe the paper on the differences between the caudate and putamen, and reduced emphasis on the role of vlPFC. Consider adding text about the limitations of the study, and how it could have been done using pathway-specific methods.

3. Revise the Introduction to improve clarity of the experimental goals

4. Discuss your results with regards to NMDA receptors, what you might have found if you used an antagonist for those receptors, and how the dose of CNQX mgiht have impacted the NMDA system.

Reviewer 1

This manuscript presents data examining the different and combined roles of glutamatergic inputs in two regions of the striatum, the caudate and putamen, while marmosets perform a self-ordered sequencing task. Microinfusions of the AMPA receptor antagonist, CNQX, were used to block glutamatergic inputs to either the caudate nucleus, the putamen, or infused into both regions simultaneously while marmosets performed either a fixed-array or random-array sequencing task. The data presented here demonstrate that high concentration CNQX (3mM) infusions into the caudate and the putamen disrupt performance accuracy on both versions of the task, implicating the striatum in self-ordered sequencing behavior. The manuscript partitions the behavior into two types of errors, continuous errors when the marmosets respond to the same cue as their previous choice, and recurrent errors when the marmosets respond to two different cues one after the other, and then on the third choice respond to their first option instead of the correct third option. With high concentration infusions of CNQX into the putamen, marmosets increased both the continuous and recurrent errors on the fixed array trials and increased continuous errors on the random-array trials. In contrast, CNQX infusion into the caudate led to a trend of increased recurrent errors on the variable array trials. Together these data support different contributions of caudate and the putamen in self-ordered sequence behaviors. The authors make the claim that the caudate plays a role in planning and cognitive flexibility as it relates to their task, whereas the putamen plays a role in habitual behaviors. The task and data are clearly presented, the manuscript is well written, and the data supports many of the conclusions presented. That said, there are a few ways the manuscript can be improved.

Questions/Suggestions

1) My hesitation with this paper is the conclusions drawn on the data from the caudate infusions, given the main result the authors lean on is based on a trend (p = 0.0701; Figure 5B). It's clearly demonstrated that the infusions into the putamen have robust impacts on the different error behavior in both the variable array and fixed array versions of the task, which supports the overall claim that this region contributes differently than the caudate does. I think the effects of infusion in the putamen on the fixed-array trial errors and on the continuous errors in the random-array trials with a lack of effect for caudate infusions on the behavior is enough to say that these striatal regions play a different role in aspects of the behavior. I think it is fine to include in the figures and text that there was a trend of the caudate infusion increasing recurrent errors on random-array trials to support the different contributions of these striatal regions. However, the authors should make it clear in the abstract and discussion text that the caudate data showed a trend, which still supports the role of caudate in planning/working memory in the task, but that more studies would need to be completed to further tease out that role for the caudate. The authors should address this in the main text.

2) Another concern I had about the manuscript was the framing in relation exclusively to the vlPFC projections to the striatum, when the paper itself presents data from non-specific infusions of CNQX into the separate regions of striatum, the caudate and putamen. Without use of a circuit specific manipulation between vlPFC and these two regions (eg., viral techniques with opto or DREADDs) I caution against emphasizing the anatomical connection between these regions as the driver of the results when CNQX is infused. The authors note in the discussion that there in the fixed-array version of the task, there's no impact of vlPFC infusions in a different experimental cohort, and thus effects seen with in the present manuscript with striatal infusions may be due to the influence of different cortical or thalamic inputs to the striatum. These are the reasons I would caution against the framing of results nearly exclusively between vlPFC and striatum. One option to strengthen the argument for these interpretations would be to repeat the experiment with a more targeted approach for vlPFC inputs to the striatum using viral techniques and DREADDs, though this would be time-consuming and resource heavy. The more simple and feasible option would be to focus the introduction and discussion sections on the caudate and putamen with respect to each other (see the next point) and other potential input regions alongside the vlPFC.

3) It was not clear from the introduction why the authors were motivated to investigate potential similarities or differences between the two regions of the striatum they studied here. The authors should state more clearly their justification (different anatomical connections/density? Previous behavioral findings?) for infusing these regions separately and together in the introduction.

Reviewer 2

The current paper provides a rich, qualitative assessment of the differing roles of caudate vs. putamen in self-ordering behavior, under fixed vs variable conditions. The paper is very well written, and is so articulate that it is easy to follow, even though the data are actually rather complex. It is particularly interesting to compare the current pattern of results in caudate vs. putamen with those from a previous study using prefrontal cortical inactivation, which selectively impaired responding only of the variable condition. The results advance emerging views on the relative contributions of the caudate vs. putamen in flexible vs. more habitual responding.

The last paragraph of the Discussion, regarding the roles of glutamate receptor mechanisms, brings up important points and could be expanded. In particular, there is evidence from mouse striatum that presynaptic NMDAR on corticostriatal terminals are important for LTP (Park et al Neuron. 2014 Dec 3;84(5):1009-22), which suggests that NMDAR may be particularly important for performing this task e.g. under variable conditions. Did the authors ever try NMDAR blockade? Might they expect a different result? Is there any evidence that a high dose of CNQX was required to engage these receptors?

View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 11 (3)
eNeuro
Vol. 11, Issue 3
March 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Comparative Roles of the Caudate and Putamen in the Serial Order of Behavior: Effects of Striatal Glutamate Receptor Blockade on Variable versus Fixed Spatial Self-Ordered Sequencing in Marmosets
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Comparative Roles of the Caudate and Putamen in the Serial Order of Behavior: Effects of Striatal Glutamate Receptor Blockade on Variable versus Fixed Spatial Self-Ordered Sequencing in Marmosets
Stacey Anne Gould, Amy Hodgson, Hannah F. Clarke, Trevor W. Robbins, Angela C. Roberts
eNeuro 12 March 2024, 11 (3) ENEURO.0541-23.2024; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0541-23.2024

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Comparative Roles of the Caudate and Putamen in the Serial Order of Behavior: Effects of Striatal Glutamate Receptor Blockade on Variable versus Fixed Spatial Self-Ordered Sequencing in Marmosets
Stacey Anne Gould, Amy Hodgson, Hannah F. Clarke, Trevor W. Robbins, Angela C. Roberts
eNeuro 12 March 2024, 11 (3) ENEURO.0541-23.2024; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0541-23.2024
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • caudate
  • goal directed
  • habit
  • putamen
  • self-ordered sequencing
  • serial order of behavior

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Deletion of endocannabinoid synthesizing enzyme DAGLα in Pcp2-positive cerebellar Purkinje cells decreases depolarization-induced short-term synaptic plasticity, reduces social preference, and heightens anxiety
  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Visual Stimulation Under 4 Hz, Not at 10 Hz, Generates the Highest-Amplitude Frequency-Tagged Responses of the Human Brain: Understanding the Effect of Stimulation Frequency
  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.