Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

Deprivation-Induced Plasticity in the Early Central Circuits of the Rodent Visual, Auditory, and Olfactory Systems

Li Huang, Francesca Hardyman, Megan Edwards and Elisa Galliano
eNeuro 9 January 2024, 11 (2) ENEURO.0435-23.2023; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0435-23.2023
Li Huang
Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, CB23EL Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Li Huang
Francesca Hardyman
Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, CB23EL Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Francesca Hardyman
Megan Edwards
Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, CB23EL Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Megan Edwards
Elisa Galliano
Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, CB23EL Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Elisa Galliano
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Activity-dependent neuronal plasticity is crucial for animals to adapt to dynamic sensory environments. Traditionally, it has been investigated using deprivation approaches in animal models primarily in sensory cortices. Nevertheless, emerging evidence emphasizes its significance in sensory organs and in subcortical regions where cranial nerves relay information to the brain. Additionally, critical questions started to arise. Do different sensory modalities share common cellular mechanisms for deprivation-induced plasticity at these central entry points? Does the deprivation duration correlate with specific plasticity mechanisms? This study systematically reviews and meta-analyzes research papers that investigated visual, auditory, or olfactory deprivation in rodents of both sexes. It examines the consequences of sensory deprivation in homologous regions at the first central synapse following cranial nerve transmission (vision - lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus; audition - ventral and dorsal cochlear nucleus; olfaction - olfactory bulb). The systematic search yielded 91 papers (39 vision, 22 audition, 30 olfaction), revealing substantial heterogeneity in publication trends, experimental methods, measures of plasticity, and reporting across the sensory modalities. Despite these differences, commonalities emerged when correlating plasticity mechanisms with the duration of sensory deprivation. Short-term deprivation (up to 1 d) reduced activity and increased disinhibition, medium-term deprivation (1 d to a week) involved glial changes and synaptic remodeling, and long-term deprivation (over a week) primarily led to structural alterations. These findings underscore the importance of standardizing methodologies and reporting practices. Additionally, they highlight the value of cross-modal synthesis for understanding how the nervous system, including peripheral, precortical, and cortical areas, respond to and compensate for sensory inputs loss.

  • audition
  • olfaction
  • plasticity
  • rodent
  • sensory deprivation
  • vision

Significance Statement

This study addresses the critical issue of sensory loss and its impact on the brain’s adaptability, shedding light on how different sensory systems respond to loss of inputs from the environment. While past research has primarily explored early-life sensory deprivation, this study focuses on the effects of sensory loss in postweaning rodents. By systematically reviewing 91 research articles, the findings reveal distinct responses based on the duration of sensory deprivation. This research not only enhances our understanding of brain plasticity but also has broad implications for translational applications, particularly in cross-modal plasticity, offering valuable insights into neuroscientific research and potential clinical interventions.

Introduction

Animals rely on sophisticated sensory organs to effectively perceive and interact with their surroundings. These sensory organs can convert various environmental stimuli, such as electromagnetic waves, mechanical pressure, and chemicals, into trains of action potentials that are relayed and computed in dedicated brain areas. The disruption of the sensory transduction cascade is a common occurrence attributable to factors such as trauma, ischemia, viral infection, and aging (Howarth and Shone, 2006; Worrall, 2007; Ryan et al., 2016; Boesveldt et al., 2017; Muth, 2017; Mullol et al., 2020). If left unattended, sudden sensory loss can significantly impact an individual’s behavior and well-being. Consequently, the nervous system must promptly adopt strategies to compensate for such losses. Unlike organs like the bones or skin, the adult brain cannot regenerate damaged peripheral sensors or central neurons, with only the olfactory system being a notable exception (Lepousez et al., 2013; Schwob et al., 2017). However, neurons can partially counteract the loss of sensory information by engaging a range of activity-dependent plasticity mechanisms (Carulli et al., 2011). These encompass both functional and structural changes at synapses, as well as adjustments in the intrinsic excitability and firing rates of neurons (Turrigiano, 2011; Wefelmeyer et al., 2016; Keck et al., 2017). Furthermore, glial cells also play a pivotal role in facilitating neuronal plasticity (Allen and Lyons, 2018; Sancho et al., 2021). The investigation of the mechanisms behind deprivation-induced adaptive plasticity across different timeframes, from immediate sensory loss to subsequent functional recovery, not only enhances our fundamental understanding of how neural circuits adapt to changing sensory inputs but also holds great significance for translational research in improving recovery after sudden sensory loss (Syka, 2002; Chen et al., 2011).

Since the seminal experiments of Hubel and Wiesel in monocularly deprived kittens (Hubel and Wiesel, 1963), multiple studies have dissected the mechanisms of deprivation-induced plasticity in animal models. This extensive body of work has largely examined the deprivation effects in primary sensory cortices during development and adulthood (Hensch, 2005; Feldman, 2009). While less emphasis has been placed on precortical regions (Jones, 2000; Duménieu et al., 2021), it is important to note that changes in these areas have a cascading impact on cortical adaptation and processing. Furthermore, existing studies in both the cortex and precortical areas have primarily focused on individual sensory modalities, making meaningful cross-modal comparisons challenging due to substantial experimental variability. This variability arises from factors such as the animals’ developmental stage, the experimental model, and heterogeneous methods for inducing adaptive plasticity through sensory deprivation (Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008; Pozo and Goda, 2010). Additionally, diversity in experimental design and result reporting complicates efforts to establish overarching principles governing the recruitment of different plasticity mechanisms in excitatory and inhibitory neurons, as well as glial cells. Synthesizing general principles is further hindered by anatomical and physiological diversity in the various sensory pathways, including differences in sensory organ complexity, transduction mechanisms, and the number of precortical relays.

To overcome these complications, this study focuses on deprivation-induced plasticity in anatomically homologous subcortical hubs in the olfactory, visual, and auditory pathways in rodents. The olfactory bulb (OB), lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus (LGN and SC), and the dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei (DCN and VCN) receive the primary synapse in the brain made by the respective cranial nerves (Fig. 1). While their circuit architectures differs granularly, these five circuits share multiple features (Oertel and Young, 2004; Shepherd, 2004; Campagnola and Manis, 2014; Kosaka and Kosaka, 2016; Weyand, 2016; Duménieu et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2022). Principal neurons receive glutamatergic inputs from the olfactory, optic, or cochlear nerves and send their axons to higher processing areas (from which they receive feedback projections which are beyond the scope of this study). Importantly, all five circuits heavily feature local inhibitory interneurons modulating information transfer, with additional excitatory interneurons described in both OB and DCN.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Architecture of the early olfactory, visual, and auditory pathways. A, Schematic representation of the mouse brain and location of the olfactory bulb (OB), lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN; dorsal, dLGN; and ventral, vLGN, combined), superior colliculus (SC), dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei (DCN and VCN), and their respective cranial nerves input from the sensory organs. B–D, Simplified circuitry of the early central circuits processing olfactory, visual, and auditory information (summarized and adapted from Oertel and Young, 2004; Shepherd, 2004; Campagnola and Manis, 2014; Kosaka and Kosaka, 2016; Weyand, 2016; Duménieu et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2022). Black line and triangle indicate the cranial nerve ending, gray cells are principal neurons projecting outside these early circuits to higher processing areas, red and blue cells are local interneurons, respectively, excitatory and inhibitory. For ease of representation, the many central inputs to these circuits are not depicted. In the bulbar circuit: ONL, olfactory nerve layer; GL, glomerular layer; ML, mitral layer; GrC, granule cells layer; M/T, mitral/tufted cell; pg, periglomerular cell; etc, external tufted cell; gr, granule cell. In the geniculate circuit: R, relay neuron; TRN, thalamic reticular nucleus. In the collicular circuit: SO, stratum opticum; note that the exact circuitry has not been fully resolved, and that all layers send projections outside the SC. In the cochlear nuclei circuit: P, pyramidal (or fusiform); Gi, giant cell; B, bushy cell; TS, T-stellate cell; ds, d-stellate cell; tv, tubercoloventral (or vertical) cell; c, cartwheel cell; Gr, granule cell with its axon called parallel fiber.

While we acknowledge the extensive literature on sensory deprivation in diverse animal models, especially primates and carnivores (King and Parsons, 1999; Kupers and Ptito, 2014), our systematic search was tailored to rodents. This choice allowed us to maintain an ethologically focused approach while still ensuring the inclusivity necessary to capture a significant number of articles across all sensory modalities. To disentangle early developmental plasticity from activity-dependent plasticity in self-sufficient animals, we exclusively considered in vivo deprivation studies in postweaning rodents (i.e., juveniles and adults).

Our systematic search across two databases returned 91 articles which employed visual, auditory, or olfactory deprivations spanning durations from 30 min to over a year and utilized a range of experimental techniques from transcriptomics to behavioral assays. This meta-research study pursued two primary objectives. First, we aimed to elucidate the characteristics of the literature and provide recommendations for designing, executing, and reporting sensory deprivation experimental approaches in rodent models. Second, we sought to identify commonalities in precortical plasticity across the three senses, facilitating the synthesis of generalizable principles. Such insights can inform analogous approaches in other systems (e.g., in cortex, following sensory enrichment), as well as translational research on recovery from sudden sensory loss.

Materials and Methods

Inclusion criteria

We included papers which met all of the following criteria: (a) rodent animal models (mouse, rat, guinea pig, hamsters, gerbils) of either sex, (b) primary research, (c) English language, (d) sensory deprivation performed (d′) in vivo (d″) in animals which had been weaned for the entire duration of the deprivation, and (e) plasticity investigated at the location of first central synapse after cranial nerve (i.e., olfactory bulb, lateral geniculate nucleus, superior colliculus, dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei, trigeminal nucleus). The choice of a weaning as a cutoff was driven by the absence of information about full-adulthood milestones (e.g., puberty and sexual maturity) and the predominant use of juvenile animals in ex vivo electrophysiological studies.

Search strategy

Two separate searches, without any time constraints on publication date, were performed on 22 November 2021 in PubMed and Scopus using the Boolean search strings detailed below:“((mouse) OR (rat) OR (gerbil) OR (guinea pig) OR (rodent))

NOT (cross-modal)”

AND ((sensory deprivation) OR (auditory deprivation) OR (auditory deafferentation) OR (visual deprivation) OR (dark exposure) OR (enucleation) OR (retinal lesions) OR (olfactory deprivation) OR (odor deprivation) OR (naris occlusion) OR (nostril occlusion) OR ((trimming) AND (whiskers)) OR ((plucking) AND (whiskers)) OR ((pruning) AND whiskers)) OR (ear plug) OR ((cauterised) AND (naris)) OR ((cauterised) AND (nose)) OR ((cauterised) AND (olfactory)) OR (nose plug) OR ((naris) AND (closure)) OR ((whisker) AND (deprivation)))

AND ((superior colliculus) OR (lateral geniculate nucleus) OR (visual thalamus) OR (cochlear nucleus) OR ((trigeminal nucleus) AND (whisker)) OR (olfactory bulb))

AND ((plasticity) OR (adaptation) OR (adaptive) OR (experience-dependent) OR (homeostatic) OR (synaptic scaling) OR (compensatory) OR (activity-dependent plasticity) OR (firing-rate homeostasis) OR (intrinsic excitability))

For Scopus, the string was modified by adding “TITLE-ABS-KEY [“ at the start, and brace {} instead of brackets ()].

Study selection

In the first screening stage, the title and abstract of the research papers from both databases were scrutinized by two independent reviewers (L.H. and F.H.) to ensure compliance with the inclusion criteria a–c. Duplicates (research papers found in both databases) were removed. Disagreements were resolved by a third reviewer (E.G.). In the second screening stage, the full text of shortlisted studies was considered against the inclusion criteria d and e (see Inclusion criteria and Fig. 2).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Strategy for literature search and papers selection. Flowchart indicating the number of articles returned by the parallel searches in the databases PubMed and Scopus using the search terms detailed in the text and their subsequent selection by two independent scrutineers.

Data extraction and availability

Prior to the search, a list of relevant information to be extracted from each selected paper was agreed upon. These included the following: (1) publication year, (2) rodent species, (3) sex, (4) age at deprivation onset, (5) deprivation type and its possible reversibility, (6) method of deprivation, (7) duration of deprivation, (8) experimental method used to probe plasticity, (9) cell type in which the plasticity was investigated, and (10) main findings. Papers meeting inclusion criteria were then carefully screened for this information, which was collated into a master spreadsheet. The dataset is available under a CC BY copyright at https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0435-23.2023.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed in Prism (GraphPad) and R. Data were checked for normality, one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post hoc correction for multiple comparisons was used to assess differences in deprivation duration, and chi-squared tests were used to compare proportions. Significance was set to p < 0.05. For the meta-analysis, mean and SD as well as n number were extracted manually from the relevant papers and submitted to meta-analysis in R using the “metafor” and “meta” packages.

Results

Summary of included papers

Two search strategies were employed to screen articles (Fig. 2). PubMed yielded 579 articles, while Scopus returned 219. The inclusion of tactile sensing (whisking) was initially considered but later discarded due to the limited number of relevant papers (only five). After screening the abstracts of the 798 retrieved research papers to remove duplicates, both independent reviewers identified 87 articles that met the inclusion criteria. The 56 papers selected by only one reviewer underwent a third-party review, resulting in the exclusion of 35 papers. The remaining 108 papers underwent a full-text review, and 17 of them did not meet the inclusion criteria. Ultimately, 91 papers were included in the analysis.

Literature characteristics: publication trends over time

First, we analyzed the publication trends over time across vision, audition, and olfaction in the qualifying papers which investigated deprivation-induced plasticity in cranial nerve receiving areas. We found no significant trend in the number of publications over time (Fig. 3A, simple linear regression; R2 = 0.06; F(1,36) = 2.34; p = 0.14). The mean publications per year was 2.4 papers (SD = 1.7). The number of studies investigating the visual system was higher than in olfaction and audition (Fig. 3B, cumulative distributions). The median publication year was calculated for each sense, and vision studies appear to be older than olfaction and audition (Fig. 3C, vision, n = 39, median, 2002; audition, n = 22, median, 2009; olfaction, n = 30, median, 2009). In summary, we found that the field of deprivation-induced plasticity is steadily productive, with studies focusing on vision being more numerous.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Publication trends over time. A, No significant trend in the volume of published primary research articles over the 1984–2021 time period. Dotted line represents mean number of publications per year (2.4 mean, 1.7 standard deviation). B, Cumulative distribution of papers published over the 1984–2021 period for each investigated sensory modality. C, Distribution of papers over time for each investigated sensory modality. Dots are individual studies, line indicates median year. Purple, vision; orange, audition; yellow, olfaction.

Features of the experimental models

Next, we analyzed the use of animal models over years and across senses. Most studies used either rats (n = 54, of which 35% Wistar and 30% Sprague Dawley) or mice (n = 33, of which 48% C57Bl6 wild-type, 15% wild-type animals in other genetic background, and 30% genetically modified mice). In line with general trends in the field and the advent of numerous commercially available genetically modified lines, the use of mice has significantly increased since the early 2000s (mouse median publication year, 2012; rat median publication year, 2001). Only three studies used other rodent models, namely, gerbils (n = 1), guinea pigs (n = 1), and hamsters (n = 2; Fig. 4A). In terms of age, most studies reported using fully adult animals (i.e., animals reported as older than 40 d or “adult”; 65/91 papers). Some studies used exclusively juvenile rodents aged between weaning and postnatal day 40 (26 papers in total, 10 vision, 7 audition, 9 olfaction). These potentially fall within critical windows of heightened plasticity (Hensch, 2005; Andrade-Talavera et al., 2023), but they are a minority and equally distributed across the three senses (Fig. 4B). Regarding the rodents’ sex, studies most commonly used exclusively male rodents (41%), and unfortunately many articles did not report the animals’ sex (33%). However, recent years saw an increase of studies using female rodents (female only, n = 10, median publication year, 2014; male only, n = 37, median publication year, 2005; both sexes, n = 14, median publication year, 2003; not reported, n = 30, median publication year, 2005; Fig. 4C). Finally, we found no significant differences in the proportion of studies using both sexes among the three sensory modalities, with similar proportions of papers using both sexes (vision, 13%; audition, 18%; olfaction, 17%; chi-squared test; χ2(2) = 1.04; p = 0.59; Fig. 4D).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Animal models. A, Distribution of rodent species used over time (1984–2021). Dots are individual studies; line indicates median publication year. Inset: pie chart reporting the percentages of the 91 selected studies using rats (59%), mice (36%), and other rodents (5% split among hamsters, guinea pigs, and gerbils). B, Number of papers using only juvenile animals (postweaning to P40; 26 papers), both juvenile and adult animals (5 papers), and only adult animals older than 40 postnatal days (filled rectangles) or stated “adult” without specifying the age (striped rectangles) across vision (purple), audition (orange), and olfaction (yellow). C, Distribution of rodent sex used over time (1984–2021). Dots are individual studies; line indicates median publication year. Inset: pie chart reporting the percentages of the 91 selected studies using females only (11%), males only (41%), both sexes (15%), or failing to report the sex of the used animals (33%). D, Proportion of papers using both sexes (filled rectangles) across the three sensory modalities: purple, vision, 13%; orange, audition, 18%; yellow, olfaction, 17%. Striped rectangles include studies which used only one sex or failed to report the sex used.

Features of the experimental paradigm: diverse methods to induce sensory deprivation

Next, we focused on how visual, auditory, and olfactory deprivations were induced. The most common method to induce sensory deprivation involves lesioning the peripheral sensory organ via surgical or chemical approaches (surgical lesion, n = 63; chemical lesion, n = 13; lesion methods combined, 82%; Fig. 5A). Other less invasive methods were sense-specific and included using nose and ear plugs for, respectively, olfactory and auditory deprivation and dark rearing for visual deprivation (other methods, n = 17, 18%; Fig. 5A). The minimum duration of deprivation significantly differed among sensory modalities (one-way ANOVA; F(2,87) = 5.13; p = 0.0078), with the audition field adopting significantly shorter deprivation durations (4.9 d ± 5.7 d) compared with olfaction (18.05 d ± 15.65 d) and a trend of shorter duration compared with vision (13.16 d ± 2.791 d; Tukey’s post hoc audition vs olfaction, p < 0.01; audition vs vision, p = 0.09; vision vs olfaction, p = 0.36; Fig. 5B).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Deprivation method. A, Number of papers using surgical, chemical, or other (plugs, patches) deprivation methods. B, Minimum deprivation duration in days used in each study (individual dots) and mean duration (black line). C, Proportion of papers using reversible deprivation methods (filled rectangles) across the three sensory modalities: purple, vision, 26%; orange, audition, 18%; yellow, olfaction, 90%. D, Proportion of papers which used a reversible method to induce deprivation and investigated recovery (filled rectangles) across the three sensory modalities: purple, vision, 3%; orange, audition, 18%; yellow, olfaction, 17%. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

We also assessed how many studies used a reversible deprivation method which allows investigating the circuit recovery while leaving it anatomically intact. Such methods include nose and ear plugs and eye patches or dark rearing. We found that, compared with vision and audition, a larger fraction of olfactory papers used reversible methods, chiefly the insertion and removal of a nose plug (vision, 26%; audition, 18%; olfaction, 90%: chi-squared test; χ2(2) = 126; p < 0.0001; Fig. 5C). Consequently, olfactory and auditory studies often investigated the functional recovery after cessation of the sensory deprivation (vision, 3%; audition, 18%; olfaction, 17%: chi-squared test χ2(2) = 12.72; p = 0.0017; Fig. 5D). It is noteworthy that while not every olfactory study using nose plugs investigated functional recovery, every auditory paper employing reversible deprivation techniques, that is, ear plugs, studied the circuit recovery after plug removal.

Features of experimental paradigm: deprivation-induced plasticity readouts

After confirming that sensory deprivation is induced in two broadly similar ways across the three sensory modalities—permanent lesions or reversible removal of the sensory stimuli—we proceeded to assess how the consequences of such sensory deprivations were investigated. Across all sensory modalities, the predominantly employed experimental technique was histology (n = 75), followed by molecular biology assays (n = 14), electrophysiological recordings (n = 14), and functional imaging in live tissue (n = 13). Although histology was the most commonly used technique, the proportion of studies employing each technique is different across the senses (chi-squared test; χ2(8) = 38.34; p < 0.0001). Notably, autoradiography is used almost exclusively by vision researchers (except for 1 auditory study; n = 7; Fig. 6A). We observed more pronounced differences when we analyzed the number of studies employing more than one method to probe deprivation-induced plasticity in the target circuits. While approximately a quarter of papers focusing on visual and auditory early brain areas used more than one technique, over half of olfactory papers investigated the consequences of smell deprivation in the olfactory bulb using multiple methods (vision, 26%; audition, 23%; olfaction, 60%: chi-squared test; χ2(2) = 36.51; p < 0.0001; Fig. 6B).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Plasticity readouts. A, Number of papers across the three sensory modalities using each investigative methodology. B, Proportion of papers using more than one method to investigate the effects of deprivation (filled rectangles) across the three sensory modalities: purple, vision, 26%; orange, audition, 23%; yellow, olfaction, 60%. ***p < 0.001.

Features of experimental paradigm: definition of cell types

As experience-dependent plasticity is cell type specific (Pozo and Goda, 2010; Fox and Stryker, 2017), we next assessed on which cell types the 91 selected studies focused their investigation on. We broadly grouped cell types into glia and neurons and further subdivided the neuronal class into principal neurons, whose axon projects out of the circuits where their soma resides (i.e., relay neurons in the lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus, bushy and T-stellate cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus, fusiform and giant cells in dorsal cochlear nucleus, mitral/tufted cells in the olfactory bulb), and excitatory or inhibitory interneurons, whose processes are fully contained in the local circuit. We found that a significant number of studies across all three sensory modalities, but more pronouncedly in vision, did not define the cell types that they investigated, either because they looked at area-wide measures or because they did not classify the types/subtypes of cells that they assessed (number of studies defining cell types: vision, 13%; audition, 59%; olfaction, 80%: chi-squared test; χ2(2) = 93.96; p < 0.0001; Fig. 7A). Given the historical popularity of visual deprivation (Fig. 3C; mean publication year, 2002), we analyzed the trend over time for studies to define cell type (Fig. 7B). As the median year for cell type definition is 2009, we confirmed that defining cell types has become more routine in recent years (e.g., the two most recent papers in vision both defined cell types; Jaepel et al., 2017; Huh et al., 2020), but not fully penetrant since long-established practices in each field seem to somewhat linger.⇓

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Definition of cell types. A, Proportion of papers defining the cell types where the deprivation-induced plasticity was investigated (filled rectangles). ***p < 0.001. B, Distribution of papers over time split by the lack/presence of cell type definition. Dots are individual studies; purple, vision; orange, audition; yellow, olfaction. C, Among the studies which defined cell types, number of papers investigating deprivation-induced plasticity in projection/principal neurons, excitatory (+) or inhibitory (−) interneurons, and glia across the three sensory modalities. Note the lack of vision papers focusing on interneurons.

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Meta-analysis of TH expression after olfactory deprivation of various durations. A, Effect size of olfactory deprivation on the number of TH-positive DA neurons in the OB. Note that 7/11 data points originated from the same study (Briñón et al., 2001). B, Effect size of olfactory deprivation duration on the TH staining intensity in bulbar DA neurons. Note that 2/3 data points originated from the same study (Byrne et al., 2022).

When papers defined the cell types in which they investigated deprivation-induced plasticity, they did so in different proportions across the three sensory modalities. While principal neurons have been explicitly investigated in all circuits albeit with different proportions [2/39 papers in vision (Jaepel et al., 2017; Huh et al., 2020), 3/30 papers in olfaction (Henegar and Maruniak, 1991; Wilson and Sullivan, 1995; Galliano et al., 2021), and 7/22 papers in audition (Caicedo et al., 1997; Francis and Manis, 2000; Garcia et al., 2000; Asako et al., 2005; Whiting et al., 2009; Clarkson et al., 2016; Poveda et al., 2020)], a much more diverse picture emerges when one focuses on of interneurons. Notably, while none of the vision papers investigated interneurons explicitly, most of the olfactory studies did, both historically and currently (Fig. 7B,C). Studies investigating the olfactory bulb focused on both inhibitory interneurons (23 papers, 77% of all olfaction papers) and excitatory interneurons (5 papers, 17% of all olfaction papers; see Table 1 for details). Similarly, in audition, a fair percentage of papers investigated inhibitory interneurons (5 papers, 23%) and excitatory interneurons (2 papers, 9%). Such difference was expected given the different ratios of interneurons present in these circuits, with over 80% of OB neurons being GABAergic (Shepherd, 2004). Since the turn of the century, more attention has been devoted to glial cells in all circuits (median year of investigation: 2003).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Deprivation-induced plasticity

Effects of sensory deprivation-induced plasticity

Our objective was to assess the consistency, directionality, and comparability of the deprivation-induced effects across systems in a quantitative manner, performing a meta-analysis of the literature. Unfortunately, this proved impossible given the huge variability in the deprivation induction method (Fig. 5), experimental techniques employed to readout the deprivation-induced plasticity (Fig. 6), as well as severity of deprivation (i.e., lesion or reversible stimulus removal; Fig. 7) and completeness of the reporting.

Reliable activity-dependent molecular markers can be used to validate the success of a deprivation method. However, in vision and audition, such a pronounced and stereotypical change has not been systematically described, albeit calcium-binding proteins and immediate early genes are potential candidates (Philpot et al., 1997; Barmack and Qian, 2002; Gonzalez-Perez et al., 2018). In olfaction, reduced expression of tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) has been traditionally used to confirm that the nose plugging or cauterization had an effect (Baker et al., 1983). As such, TH RNA and/or protein expression is the most reported plasticity readout across the literature. Using the same search terms to extend the publication date to March 2023 to increase our paper pool to a statistically meaningful size (Andrade, 2020), we collated 11 papers reporting TH changes using immunofluorescence, and we attempted to conduct a meta-analysis to assess the overall effect of olfactory deprivation on TH expression. While all studies showed a decrease in TH, we wanted to see if the change was dependent or modulated by deprivation duration. Out of the 11 papers, the ones with sufficient statistical reporting allowing for meta-analysis (5 papers) were split into ones that used TH-positive cell density (9 experiments across 3 papers) versus those based on TH cell fluorescence (3 experiments across 2 papers) to report TH changes. The overall measurement of standardized mean difference was significant for both measurements, 3.24 (random-effects meta-analysis, p = 0.0001) for density and 0.74 (random-effects meta-analysis, p = 0.0004) for the staining intensity, demonstrating that olfactory deprivation is correlated with a decline in TH expression. However, upon further analysis, the test for heterogeneity in each case (I2 = 73.69%, p = 0.0003 and I2 = 95.68%, p < 0.0001) shows that we cannot assume that there is a singular effect size for the population represented by these studies. When occlusion duration was added as a modulator, the amount of heterogeneity unaccounted for by occlusion time was very high in studies investigating TH by fluorescence. This was lower for studies of TH density analysis (I2 = 23.07%, p = 0.2486), suggesting a more homogeneous underlying effect size once occlusion duration was accounted for. The low number of experiments and papers entered in the meta-analysis precludes any conclusions from being drawn as the number of studies falls below the threshold of minimum of 10 experiments used by meta-analyses in the field (Andrade, 2020).

Given the impossibility to perform quantitative meta-analysis on the entire dataset, we attempted to summarize and correlate these results qualitatively by grouping the findings using the only descriptor present in every study: the overall duration of the deprivation, which we divided in short term (1 d or less), medium term (up to a week), and long term (over a week). This grouping, albeit arbitrary, reflects physiologically relevant scenarios of changes in sensory inputs. Short-term deprivation reflected transient and mild diseases involving sensory organs, for example, temporary hearing loss from noise overexposure (Syka and Rybalko, 2000), loss of smell from mild colds (Fokkens et al., 2012), and transient visual loss (Pula et al., 2016). Medium-term deprivation reflects longer albeit still temporary diseases, such as acute ear infection (Worrall, 2007) and anosmia associated with COVID-19 (Santos et al., 2021). Long-term deprivation reflects severe, more permanent forms of sensory deprivation, such as presbycusis (age-related hearing loss; Gold and Bajo, 2014), long COVID-19 (Park et al., 2022), and diabetic retinopathy (Stitt et al., 2016). Where possible we tried to group findings by cell-type (Fig. 7) and broad subregion (e.g., dorsal LGN/CN vs ventral LGN/CN). When cell type specificity was not reported, the results were divided into a few broad themes, namely, macroscopic size, protein expression, activity, and proliferation. Using this structure, a few central commonalties emerged and are summarized below and in Table 1.

Common mechanisms of plasticity: short-term deprivation produces decreased activity and disinhibition

After short-term deprivation, defined as 24 h or less, the effect most consistently investigated across all three senses was changed in overall activity levels. Despite using different proxies, a reduction in neuronal metabolic activity in the early brain circuits receiving input from the deprived eye/ear/nostril was found across all studies. In the visual system, this is manifested as a reduction in glucose uptake in the LGN and SC (Horsburgh and McCulloch, 1991; Wang et al., 2005). Similarly, a decrease in the immunoreactivity of the activity early gene c-fos was observed in the olfactory (Briñón et al., 2001; Galliano et al., 2021) and auditory systems (Luo et al., 1999, p. 199). This rapid decrease in activity is in alignment with that found in primary visual and auditory cortices (Feldman, 2009; Gold and Bajo, 2014).

Deprivation-induced homeostasis can be achieved, at least transiently, by balancing the changes in the excitatory and inhibitory pathways, and it has been proposed that rapid disinhibition mediated by downregulated inhibitory networks precedes excitatory plasticity (Barnes et al., 2015; Gainey and Feldman, 2017). The papers that defined cell types and specifically investigated inhibition reported findings which were consistent with decreased inhibition: GABAergic and glycinergic mechanisms are downregulated following brief sensory deprivation (see Table 1 for details). In the auditory system, GlyRα1 postsynaptic density in bushy and fusiform cells was decreased (Whiting et al., 2009). In the olfactory system, the dopaminergic inhibitory interneurons downregulate TH expression, reduce their intrinsic excitability, and shorten the axon initial segment (Galliano et al., 2021). In visual areas, immunoreactivity of GABA transporters (GAT-1 and GAT-3) in hypertrophied astrocytes in the deafferented SC was increased, suggesting an increased uptake of GABA (Yan and Ribak, 1998). This early disinhibition of the deprived system, achieved via different mechanisms in each circuit, also occurs in higher areas and has been extensively reviewed in the auditory, visual, and somatosensory cortices (Gold and Bajo, 2014; Li et al., 2014; Gainey and Feldman, 2017).

Conversely, alterations in excitatory signaling following 1-day-long deprivation were less clear cut. In olfaction and vision, studies which investigated glutamatergic function found no differences after brief deprivation. In vision, this was investigated using autoradiography, and no changes were found in the SC or dLGN for AMPA, NMDA, and kainate receptors expression (Chalmers and McCulloch, 1991a). In the olfactory system, while both the glutamatergic principal neurons and interneurons downregulated the expression of immediate early genes and activity markers, they did not modulate their intrinsic excitability or their axon initial segment morphology (Galliano et al., 2021). In the auditory system, however, there is a highly specific upregulation of AMPA receptor subunits at the auditory nerve to principal neuron synapse (GluR3 upregulated at bushy cell and fusiform synapse, GluR4 downregulated at fusiform synapse; Whiting et al., 2009). When combined with concomitant changes in inhibition, the overall functional effect of these changes on circuit homeostasis remains to be elucidated.

In contrast to the other two senses, papers in audition mostly focused on short deprivation, with almost half of all papers investigating 1 d or <1 d deprivations (9/22; Fig. 5B) and glial responses. An increase in glial activation was found across different studies using varied experimental methods and included increases in immune-related genes (Harris et al., 2008), activation (Harris et al., 2008; Janz and Illing, 2014), and proliferation (Illing et al., 2019) of microglia, as well as increases in staining for astrocyte markers (Janz and Illing, 2014). These findings are largely consistent with an increased glial activity in the cochlear nuclei, but given the lack of similar investigations in vision and olfaction, it remains unclear whether this is a unique feature of the early auditory circuits or, like decrease in activity and disinhibition, a common response to brief sensory deprivation.

Common mechanisms of plasticity: medium-term deprivation is reflected by an increased involvement of glia and synaptic remodeling

After mid-term deprivation, defined as lasting between 1 d and a week, we found activity reductions (e.g., in glucose uptake after visual deprivation) which were consistent between animals deprived at juvenile or adult stages (Zilles et al., 1989; Wang et al., 2005). Alongside continued decreases in activity, the most prominent changes were changes in synaptic properties and glial activation. Across all three sensory modalities, multiple studies reported functional and structural changes at the synapses formed by the cranial nerve axon terminals, suggesting an overall change to circuit-level excitation/inhibition (E/I) balance.

In the olfactory system studies, investigations did not focus on presynaptic remodeling but rather on the postsynaptic properties of the various interneuron subtypes innervated by the olfactory nerve directly or via multisynapse loops. Functional changes have been found in juvenile mice at the excitatory interneurons (external tufted cells, ETCs), where deprivation decreased the amplitude of spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents (Lau and Murthy, 2012), as well as increased quantal glutamatergic (AMPA-mediated) postsynaptic currents (Tyler et al., 2007). Structurally, a reduction of spine density was found in the inhibitory interneurons granule cells (Denizet et al., 2017) as well as GAD67 puncta in ETCs (Lau and Murthy, 2012). Since ETCs modulate both inhibitory interneurons and excitatory principal neurons in the OB, the overall effect of these changes on the whole-circuit output remains unclear but seems to suggest modifications in the E/I balance.

In the visual system, only synapses formed by the optic nerve onto principal neurons in the dorsal LGN were studied after medium-term sensory deprivation. Thalamocortical neurons in the dLGN of juvenile animals had smaller single-fiber AMPA-mediated postsynaptic currents at the retinogeniculate synapse (Hooks and Chen, 2008; Narushima et al., 2016), indicating weakening of individual glutamatergic afferents following deprivation. However, this was counterbalanced by a concomitant increase in excitatory postsynaptic current (EPSC) amplitudes, indicating an increase in the number and/or strength of retinogeniculate synapses (Hooks and Chen, 2008). Structurally, there was an increase in the phosphorylation of stargazin, a transmembrane AMPAR trafficking protein in the LGN involved in synaptic scaling (Louros et al., 2014). Overall, the evidence indicates that visual deprivation induces synaptic remodeling at the excitatory retinogeniculate synapse in the dLGN of juvenile rodents after medium-term deprivation.

In the auditory system, the most prominent synaptic effect of 1 week deprivation was the overall macroscopic decrease in the number of synaptic contact zones in the anterior VCN (Hildebrandt et al., 2011). Along similar lines, a significant reduction in VGLUT1 staining was found in the VCN (Heusinger et al., 2019). Interestingly, inhibitory synapses decreased in number less markedly than excitatory ones (Hildebrandt et al., 2011). Taken together with the findings of rapid disinhibition described above, this could suggest a period of transient overinhibition following an early disinhibition phase of the VCN after cochlear deafferentation.

In addition to changes in synaptic transmission, an increase in glial proliferation and activation was found consistently across all senses. In the olfactory system, significant increases in the density and activation of microglia were seen in all papers which investigated this phenomenon (Grier et al., 2016; Denizet et al., 2017). The increased activation of microglia wasevident both in the morphology (shown as fewer primary microglial processes and shift toward hypertrophied morphology; Grier et al., 2016) and immunoreactivity staining with specific marker for activated microglia CD68 (Denizet et al., 2017). Moreover, a significant increase in reactive astrocytes was reported in all OB layers (Veyrac et al., 2005), as well as in the visual system (although here it was not a formal strand of investigation; Gonzalez et al., 2005). In the auditory system in addition to increases in microglial and astrocytic immunoreactivity in both juvenile and adult animals (Förster and Illing, 2000; Janz and Illing, 2014), glial activation has been linked to synaptogenesis in both the VCN and DCN (Fredrich et al., 2013; Heusinger et al., 2019). During the first 3 d of deprivation, astrocytes increased production of neurocan, aggrecan, and MMP9, which are key for synapse stabilization (neurocan and aggrecan; Asher et al., 2000; Rowlands et al., 2018) and the induction of synaptic plasticity via ECM remodeling (MMP9; Huntley, 2012). Then, between 3 and 7 d of deprivation, expression of these proteins decreased and was replaced by PSA-NCAM and MMP2. Both are strongly associated with structural plasticity, MMP2 via neurocan digestion and PSA-NCAM by mediating synaptogenic interactions between neurons and astrocytes (Hillen et al., 2018; Akol et al., 2022). This astrocytic biphasic response and its potential role in synaptic remodeling warrants further research in the visual and olfactory regions.

Common mechanisms of plasticity: long-term deprivation

Given the adopted definition of long-term deprivation as any period spanning between a week and a year, we found that the effect on experience-dependent plasticity on the different cell types was more heterogeneous than for shorter-lasting deprivations. Variability notwithstanding, remarkably consistent macroscopic changes and apoptosis were found across all three senses in both juvenile and fully adult animals. In olfaction, this was reflected by a smaller overall OB volume and weight (Maruniak et al., 1989; Hamilton et al., 2008; Pothayee et al., 2017) as well as in a reduction of external plexiform layer thickness (Hamilton et al., 2008) and glomeruli volume (Cummings and Belluscio, 2010) and in increased cell death in the mitral and granule cell layers (Fiske and Brunjes, 2001). Similarly, increased apoptosis was seen in the dLGN (Sukekawa, 1987) and reduction in volume was seen in the stratum zonale, stratum griseum superficiale, and stratum opticum regions of the SC (García del Caño et al., 2002; Gerrikagoitia et al., 2002). In audition, there was a marked decrease in VCN area and neuron quantity (Yuan et al., 2014). This contrasts with short-term deprivation, where macroscopic structural changes were scarcely investigated. Among our included papers, only one study reported no difference in apoptotic neurons between the unperturbed and deprived side (Kawabata et al., 2003).

In the olfactory system, these macroscopic changes can manifest in a rather unique way, namely, in cellular turnover. Neural progenitors are produced in the subventricular zone and migrate along the rostral migratory stream (RMS). They arrive at the subependymal layer of the OB, where they differentiate into glomerular interneurons or granule cells (Carleton et al., 2003). The experience-dependent nature of adult neurogenesis has been widely researched (reviewed in Bonzano et al., 2016), and it is well known that olfactory deprivation and enrichment can decrease or increase the survival of adult-born neurons, respectively. The findings of this review were in alignment with the literature, where after medium- to long-term deprivation, neurogenesis was reduced at each level of the neurogenic process. After 2–3 weeks deprivation, there was a reduction in the integration of neuroblasts into the OB, as well as slower migration along the RMS in juvenile animals (Pothayee et al., 2017). In the glomerular layer of adult rodents, a decrease of newborn periglomerular cells was found after 28 d (Sawada et al., 2011) and 42 d deprivation (Bovetti et al., 2009). This is mirrored by an increase in cell apoptosis in both the periglomerular (Sawada et al., 2011) and granule cells (Yamaguchi and Mori, 2005). Remarkably, this experience-dependent neurogenesis is found to be very specific to the dopaminergic population at the glomerular layer, as the calbindin and calretinin glomerular interneuron populations remained unchanged after deprivation (Sawada et al., 2011). Unfortunately, no papers investigated neurogenesis in the context of shorter deprivations, so we cannot correlate these findings with the deprivation duration.

According to theoretical frameworks and working models in the homeostatic plasticity field, the expectation is to see prominent changes in excitatory networks with a long-lasting sensory deprivation as the circuit stabilizes to a new set point (Gainey and Feldman, 2017). While no papers focused on the early visual circuits’ principal neurons responses to long-term deprivation, changes at the excitatory synapse between incoming nerve and principal neurons were seen at bushy cells in the cochlear nuclei. Ten days of auditory deprivation using earplugs was correlated with a decrease of the presynaptic marker VGlut immunoreactivity and size of synaptic vesicles in juvenile rats (Clarkson et al., 2016). A similar VGlut decrease was observed in the VCN of adult rats 3–14 d after cochlear ablation, suggesting a robust effect in both manipulations (Heusinger et al., 2019). Postsynaptically, GluA2/3 expression was upregulated, while GluA2 and GluA4 remained unchanged (Clarkson et al., 2016)—an effect found also after 1 d deprivation in P30 rats (Whiting et al., 2009). In the olfactory system, the systematic search only returned one paper which investigated mitral/tufted cells after long-term deprivation in juvenile rats (Wilson and Sullivan, 1995). After 60 d deprivation, while there were no changes in their spontaneous activity rates, the number of mitral/tufted cells responding to more than one odor was increased. In addition, significantly more cells in the deprived bulb responded to specific odors presented at higher intensities, suggesting a possible decrease in odor discrimination coupled with increase in responsiveness. Conversely, in a very recent paper published after the cutoff date of our systematic search (George et al., 2022), the authors found a shortening in the mitral cell axon initial segment as well as spiking frequency in adult mice unilaterally occluded for 30 d compared with the un-occluded bulb, indicating potential for decrease in intrinsic excitability at OB principal neurons after longer deprivation times. The slight discrepancy in mitral/tufted cell firing rate after long deprivation found by Wilson and Sullivan (1995) and George et al. (2022) could be attributable to various methodological differences, such as occlusion times (30 d vs 60 d, respectively), animal model (P30 rats vs P60 mice, respectively), as well as experimental methodology (in vivo electrophysiology vs acute slice electrophysiology, respectively).

In addition to changes at the principal neurons, deprivation on a longer time scale also sees a decrease in glial activation, which was investigated in vision and audition. In vision, the number of immunoreactive astrocytes significantly decreased 12–48 weeks after enucleation (Gonzalez et al., 2006). In audition, the number of calbindin-positive astrocytes started to decrease 30 d after cochlear lesion (Förster and Illing, 2000).

Discussion

In this study we employed systematic and meta-analysis methods to investigate the effects of sensory deprivation of various durations at the homologous regions receiving the cranial nerve input of three sensory modalities—vision, olfaction, and audition—in rodents. Our analysis returned large disparities across sensory modalities in publication trends, experimental methodologies employed, as well as focus of the research. However, despite such methodological and reporting differences, a few shared findings describing adaptive responses and compensatory mechanisms to deprivation can be extrapolated.

Profound differences in micro circuitry, experimental protocols, plasticity readouts, and reporting make comparisons across the three sensory modalities challenging

The three senses differ in their micro circuitry, as well as in the degree to which it has been characterized (Fig. 1). The lack of studies defining cell types and investigating interneurons in vision could be partially attributed to the incomplete characterization of the LGN, where cell identification is challenging by its lack of overt lamination (Duménieu et al., 2021). Indeed, while the OB is a highly inhibitory circuit where the ratio of GABAergic interneurons to excitatory neurons is much higher than the other parts of the brain (Shepherd, 2004), visual structures such as the LGN are composed of mainly excitatory projection neurons (Leist et al., 2016).

The main experimental methods used to induce and interrogate plasticity also differed widely across the three senses. While all sensory deprivation approaches are long-established (Gudden, 1870; Coppola, 2012), visual deprivation via monocular enucleation and eyelid suturing garnered prominence and widespread adoption after the seminal work of Hubel and Wiesel (1963). Thus, many vision papers in our list were older and tended to use this surgical technique, while papers focusing on olfactory and audition deprivation were more recent and more commonly employ the well-established reversible procedure of nose and ear plugging.

Of note, contrary to rods, cones, and hair cells, the olfactory sensory neurons are capable of regenerating throughout the life of the animal (Cheetham et al., 2016). This poses an important difference between senses. While deprivation by deafferentation (direct lesion to the peripheral sensory neurons) and by sensory deprivation (removal of sensory stimulation while leaving the anatomy of the circuit intact) has largely similar effects in the visual and auditory systems as both are incapable of regenerating, the effects in the olfactory system can be divided into adaptive plasticity (elicited by sensory deprivation, e.g. nose plug, and within the remit of this study) and regenerative plasticity (elicited by ablation of OSNs using olfactotoxic drugs such as methimazole and dichlobenil; Bergman et al., 2002; Cummings and Belluscio, 2008).

Adding onto the anatomical and procedural discrepancies across the senses, a profound lack of consistency in reporting precluded the possibility of a systematic meta-analysis. Furthermore, the findings of this study are heavily subject to publication bias, as the lack of findings in a particular area does not necessarily denote a negative finding. For example, we found an abundance of studies demonstrating the role of glia in regulating the cochlear nuclei response to short auditory deprivation, but it remains unclear whether this is because glial plasticity was never investigated in olfaction and vision or rather that it was investigated, found absent, and not published.

Moreover, by focusing on postweaning sensory deprivation, our analysis included studies involving both juvenile rodents and animals reported either as older than 40 d or simply as “adult.” This potentially introduces confounding factors associated with developmentally regulated plasticity, and it is particularly noteworthy given the heterogeneity of “critical” or “sensitive” periods across different senses and regions. For example, rodents are born deaf and blind but with a functional sense of smell (Neff, 2019). The olfactory map is then shaped during early postnatal development, and the existence of a postweaning, late critical period is still debated (Poo and Isaacson, 2007; Inoue et al., 2021). Conversely, in rodents eye, opening and hearing onset only occur approximately postnatal day 10 and “late” critical periods, which have been extensively characterized in the respective cortices, and can extend as late as the fifth postnatal week (Hooks and Chen, 2007; Nakamura et al., 2020). Drawing direct comparisons between sensory modalities in juvenile animals is therefore complex. Just over a fourth of the studies in our dataset involved rodents younger than 40 d (Fig. 4B), and the overall trends of deprivation-induced plasticity reported in these studies align with the findings from adult-only studies (e.g., activity reduction, glia activation, early disinhibition, long-term apoptosis). Yet, our analysis may have overemphasized plasticity effects in vision and/or audition more than olfaction, especially regarding short- and medium-term remodeling of excitatory synapses. A dedicated study focusing on late critical period plasticity in precortical areas, along with more standardized reporting of the age and developmental milestones of the animals across all experiments, is needed.

Despite procedural differences, the three early sensory areas share deprivation-dependent plasticity motifs

With the caveat that the short–medium–long deprivation duration classification is partially arbitrary, albeit based on clinical evidence (Syka and Rybalko, 2000; Worrall, 2007; Fokkens et al., 2012; Gold and Bajo, 2014; Pula et al., 2016; Stitt et al., 2016; Santos et al., 2021; Park et al., 2022), we found four consistent plasticity responses across the senses (Fig. 9). First, for sensory deprivation lasting up to 24 h, the overall neuronal metabolic activity is consistently reduced. In addition, the dampening of inhibitory interneuron activity was consistently reported in olfaction and audition (but not investigated in vision), suggesting fast-acting circuit disinhibition. This is in line with data from sensory cortices and the working hypothesis that excitatory principal neuron plasticity is preceded by a depression of inhibitory interneurons (Barnes et al., 2015; Gainey and Feldman, 2017). Second, in these early sensory areas, long-term and/or permanent sensory deprivations result in more drastic changes involving macroscopic changes to the circuit architecture which manifest as tissue shrinkage, cell apoptosis, and reduced adult neurogenesis in the olfactory system. Third, there is consistent evidence across all three early sensory areas and all deprivation durations for glial activation and proliferation. This highlights the prominent role of glia in shaping neuronal plasticity (Sancho et al., 2021). Fourth, although less clear-cut, from medium-term sensory loss onward, there is a strong indication of synaptic remodeling and changes in E/I balance. The fact that these four effects were revealed from the systematic search despite profound differences in circuit architecture, methodology, and readouts is perhaps an indication that these plasticity motifs are robust and conserved.

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

Deprivation-induced plasticity. Graphical representation of the cellular and circuit effects of sensory deprivations of increasing durations in early visual, auditory, and olfactory areas. See Table 1 for details and references.

It remains unclear whether these findings can be generalized to other areas, though the time course and multiplicity of mechanisms of deprivation-induced plasticity in precortical regions found in this study align with those found in the cortex and fit with the canonical theories of homeostatic plasticity (Turrigiano, 2011; Keck et al., 2017). Notably, the phenomenon that the inhibitory cells are modulated rapidly after sensory deprivation and that such changes precede adaptations in excitatory neurons has been widely reported in sensory cortices. For example, after 1 d of monocular deprivation parvalbumin-positive GABAergic basket cells in the primary visual cortex significantly reduced their firing rate and receive less synaptic excitation, while pyramidal cells remained unchanged (Hengen et al., 2013; Kuhlman et al., 2013). This rapid functional reduction of inhibitory tone is also supported by structural changes (Keck et al., 2011), and similar dynamics have been described in primary auditory (Chambers et al., 2016; Henton et al., 2023; Kumar et al., 2023) and somatosensory (Li et al., 2014; Gainey et al., 2018) cortices. Interestingly, the opposite manipulation—6 h sensory enrichment via whisker stimulation—has been shown to induce rapid downregulation of barrel cortex pyramidal cells’ excitability (Jamann et al., 2021), perhaps a neuroprotective mechanism to combat runaway excitation. This highlights a possible asymmetry in plasticity induced by sensory deprivation versus enrichment, which warrants further investigation in both cortical and subcortical regions (Keck et al., 2017).

Finally, it has been suggested that rapid disinhibition restores the system to a “juvenile” state of plasticity (Syka, 2002; Chen et al., 2011) and facilitates functional recovery by creating an environment more permissive for the induction of synaptic potentiation through long-term potentiation or spike timing-dependent plasticity in excitatory neurons (Dan and Poo, 2004; Chen et al., 2011; Gambino and Holtmaat, 2012; Li et al., 2014). However, it remains unclear whether the subcortical disinhibition we described reflects the start of functional recovery or simply a response to reduced input (Keck et al., 2011). This is of particular relevance for those wishing to use data from animal model deprivation studies to inform translational interventions in patients suffering from sudden sensory loss. Further research is required to better dissociate the mechanisms of the two responses and to understand the relationship between subcortical plasticity and its feedforward consequences in cortex.

Recommendations

This work synthesized studies on the early stages of sensory processing in audition, vision, and olfaction, and the results revealed significant heterogeneity in deprivation method, experimental readouts, and reporting, including definition of investigated cell types. While biological restrictions of the circuits (e.g., accessibility, different animal models) may explain some of the heterogeneity, this review also highlights areas needing further research. To improve comparability within and between fields, the identification of a “gold-standard” deprivation methods and validation readouts, together with systematic reporting of statistics and deposition of raw data, are sorely needed. Furthermore, the field could better definine cell types, especially with increasing evidence, including from our results, that inhibition and excitation are differentially regulated after deprivation (Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2006; Turrigiano, 2011; Kullmann et al., 2012). Finally, since most published work focused on deprivation-induced plasticity in primary sensory cortices, it is essential to fully characterize potential effects in the precortical areas which link them to the periphery. As described above, these are not passive relays but active plasticity hotspots which send to the cortex already processed and modulated inputs. This early processing must be considered to depict the full picture of the final cortical computation and behavioral outputs.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this systematic review and literature meta-analysis found that, notwithstanding major experimental heterogeneity in inducing and assessing deprivation-induced plasticity, the early visual, auditory, and olfactory systems largely employ shared mechanisms to change their circuit processing. Future work should strive to standardize both experimental and reporting approaches and investigate how early circuits and higher cortical areas together coordinate an appropriate adaptive response to a lack of peripheral sensory inputs.

Data availability

https://doi.org/10.17863/CAM.104411.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • We thank Emma Cahill (University of Bristol) for the inspiration to tackle a meta-research study; Sarah Byford (King's College London) for methodological advice; Matthew Grubb, Ana Dorrego-Rivas (King's College London), Marcela Lipovsek (University College London), and Sue Jones (University of Cambridge) for comments on early drafts; and the participants of the Kavli Institute for Theoretical Physics “Statistical Learning in the Brain” Programme and the members of the Galliano laboratory for helpful discussions.

  • This research was supported in part by the National Science Foundation under Grant NSF PHY-1748958, a Royal Society Research Grant RGS\R1\19148, an UKRI BSRC grant BB/W014688/1 (E.G.), and by a Cambridge Trust PhD studentship (L.H.).

  • ↵* L.H. and F.H. contributed equally to this work.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Gudden (1870) Experimentaluntersuchungen über das peripherische und centrale Nervensystem. Archiv f Psychiatrie 2:693–723. doi:10.1007/BF02046772
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Akol I,
    2. Kalogeraki E,
    3. Pielecka-Fortuna J,
    4. Fricke M,
    5. Löwel S
    (2022) MMP2 and MMP9 activity is crucial for adult visual cortex plasticity in healthy and stroke-affected mice. J Neurosci 42:16–32. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0902-21.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Allen NJ,
    2. Lyons DA
    (2018) Glia as architects of central nervous system formation and function. Science 362:181–185. doi:10.1126/science.aat0473
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Andrade-Talavera Y,
    2. Pérez-Rodríguez M,
    3. Prius-Mengual J,
    4. Rodríguez-Moreno A
    (2023) Neuronal and astrocyte determinants of critical periods of plasticity. Trends Neurosci 46:566–580. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2023.04.005
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. ↵
    1. Andrade C
    (2020) Understanding the basics of meta-analysis and how to read a forest plot: as simple as it gets. J Clin Psychiatry 81:20f13698. doi:10.4088/JCP.20f13698
    OpenUrlCrossRef
    1. Arai M,
    2. Arai R,
    3. Sasamoto K,
    4. Kani K,
    5. Maeda T,
    6. Deura S,
    7. Jacobowitz DM
    (1993) Appearance of calretinin-immunoreactive neurons in the upper layers of the rat superior colliculus after eye enucleation. Brain Res 613:341–346. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(93)90924-c
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Asako M,
    2. Holt AG,
    3. Griffith RD,
    4. Buras ED,
    5. Altschuler RA
    (2005) Deafness-related decreases in glycine-immunoreactive labeling in the rat cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci Res 81:102–109. doi:10.1002/jnr.20542
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Asher RA,
    2. Morgenstern DA,
    3. Fidler PS,
    4. Adcock KH,
    5. Oohira A,
    6. Braistead JE,
    7. Levine JM,
    8. Margolis RU,
    9. Rogers JH,
    10. Fawcett JW
    (2000) Neurocan is upregulated in injured brain and in cytokine-treated astrocytes. J Neurosci 20:2427–2438. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02427.2000
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Baker H,
    2. Kawano T,
    3. Margolis FL,
    4. Joh TH
    (1983) Transneuronal regulation of tyrosine hydroxylase expression in olfactory bulb of mouse and rat. J Neurosci 3:69–78. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-01-00069.1983
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Barmack NH,
    2. Qian Z
    (2002) Activity-dependent expression of calbindin in rabbit floccular Purkinje cells modulated by optokinetic stimulation. Neuroscience 113:235–250. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00008-8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    1. Barnes SJ,
    2. Sammons RP,
    3. Jacobsen RI,
    4. Mackie J,
    5. Keller GB,
    6. Keck T
    (2015) Subnetwork-specific homeostatic plasticity in mouse visual cortex in vivo. Neuron 86:1290–1303. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Batista CMC,
    2. Carneiro K,
    3. de Bittencourt-Navarrete RE,
    4. Soares-Mota M,
    5. Cavalcante LA,
    6. Mendez-Otero R
    (2003) Nitrergic dendrites in the superficial layers of the rat superior colliculus: retinal afferents and alternatively spliced isoforms in normal and deafferented animals. J Neurosci Res 71:455–461. doi:10.1002/jnr.10494
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Bergman U,
    2. Ostergren A,
    3. Gustafson A-L,
    4. Brittebo B
    (2002) Differential effects of olfactory toxicants on olfactory regeneration. Arch Toxicol 76:104–112. doi:10.1007/s00204-002-0321-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Boesveldt S,
    2. Postma EM,
    3. Boak D,
    4. Welge-Luessen A,
    5. Schöpf V,
    6. Mainland JD,
    7. Martens J,
    8. Ngai J,
    9. Duffy VB
    (2017) Anosmia—a clinical review. Chem Senses 42:513–523. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjx025
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    1. Bonzano S,
    2. Bovetti S,
    3. Gendusa C,
    4. Peretto P,
    5. De Marchis S
    (2016) Adult born olfactory bulb dopaminergic interneurons: molecular determinants and experience-dependent plasticity. Front Neurosci 10:189. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00189
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Boulenguez P,
    2. Abdelkefi J,
    3. Pinard R,
    4. Christolomme A,
    5. Segu L
    (1993) Effects of retinal deafferentation on serotonin receptor types in the superficial grey layer of the superior colliculus of the rat. J Chem Neuroanat 6:167–175. doi:10.1016/0891-0618(93)90026-z
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Bovetti S,
    2. Bonzano S,
    3. Garzotto D,
    4. Giannelli SG,
    5. Iannielli A,
    6. Armentano M,
    7. Studer M,
    8. De Marchis S
    (2013) COUP-TFI controls activity-dependent tyrosine hydroxylase expression in adult dopaminergic olfactory bulb interneurons. Development 140:4850–4859. doi:10.1242/dev.089961
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Bovetti S,
    2. Veyrac A,
    3. Peretto P,
    4. Fasolo A,
    5. De Marchis S
    (2009) Olfactory enrichment influences adult neurogenesis modulating GAD67 and plasticity-related molecules expression in newborn cells of the olfactory bulb. PLoS One 4:e6359. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006359
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Briñón JG,
    2. Crespo C,
    3. Weruaga E,
    4. Martínez-Guijarro FJ,
    5. Aijón J,
    6. Alonso JR
    (2001) Bilateral olfactory deprivation reveals a selective noradrenergic regulatory input to the olfactory bulb. Neuroscience 102:1–10. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00443-7
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Byrne DJ,
    2. Lipovsek M,
    3. Crespo A,
    4. Grubb MS
    (2022) Brief sensory deprivation triggers plasticity of dopamine-synthesising enzyme expression in genetically labelled olfactory bulb dopaminergic neurons. Eur J Neurosci 56:3591–3612. doi:10.1111/ejn.15684
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  17. ↵
    1. Caicedo A,
    2. d’Aldin C,
    3. Eybalin M,
    4. Puel JL
    (1997) Temporary sensory deprivation changes calcium-binding proteins levels in the auditory brainstem. J Comp Neurol 378:1–15. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19970203)378:1<1::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Campagnola L,
    2. Manis PB
    (2014) A map of functional synaptic connectivity in the mouse anteroventral cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 34:2214–2230. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4669-13.2014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Canady KS,
    2. Olavarria JF,
    3. Rubel EW
    (1994) Reduced retinal activity increases GFAP immunoreactivity in rat lateral geniculate nucleus. Brain Res 663:206–214. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(94)91265-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Carleton A,
    2. Petreanu LT,
    3. Lansford R,
    4. Alvarez-Buylla A,
    5. Lledo P-M
    (2003) Becoming a new neuron in the adult olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 6:507–518. doi:10.1038/nn1048
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Carrasco MM,
    2. Pallas SL
    (2006) Early visual experience prevents but cannot reverse deprivation-induced loss of refinement in adult superior colliculus. Vis Neurosci 23:845–852. doi:10.1017/S0952523806230177
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Carulli D,
    2. Foscarin S,
    3. Rossi F
    (2011) Activity-dependent plasticity and gene expression modifications in the adult CNS. Front Mol Neurosci 4:50. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2011.00050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Chalmers DT,
    2. McCulloch J
    (1991a) Selective alterations in glutamate receptor subtypes after unilateral orbital enucleation. Brain Res 540:255–265. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(91)90515-w
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Chalmers DT,
    2. McCulloch J
    (1991b) Alterations in neurotransmitter receptors and glucose use after unilateral orbital enucleation. Brain Res 540:243–254. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(91)90514-v
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Chambers AR,
    2. Resnik J,
    3. Yuan Y,
    4. Whitton JP,
    5. Edge AS,
    6. Liberman MC,
    7. Polley DB
    (2016) Central gain restores auditory processing following near-complete cochlear denervation. Neuron 89:867–879. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.041
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Cheetham CEJ,
    2. Park U,
    3. Belluscio L
    (2016) Rapid and continuous activity-dependent plasticity of olfactory sensory input. Nat Commun 7:1–11. doi:10.1038/ncomms10729
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Chen JL,
    2. Lin WC,
    3. Cha JW,
    4. So PT,
    5. Kubota Y,
    6. Nedivi E
    (2011) Structural basis for the role of inhibition in facilitating adult brain plasticity. Nat Neurosci 14:587–594. doi:10.1038/nn.2799
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Clarkson C,
    2. Antunes FM,
    3. Rubio ME
    (2016) Conductive hearing loss has long-lasting structural and molecular effects on presynaptic and postsynaptic structures of auditory nerve synapses in the cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 36:10214–10227. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0226-16.2016
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Coppola DM
    (2012) Studies of olfactory system neural plasticity: the contribution of the unilateral naris occlusion technique. Neural Plast 2012:351752. doi:10.1155/2012/351752
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Cummings DM,
    2. Belluscio L
    (2008) Charting plasticity in the regenerating maps of the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuroscientist 14:251–263. doi:10.1177/1073858408315026
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Cummings DM,
    2. Belluscio L
    (2010) Continuous neural plasticity in the olfactory intrabulbar circuitry. J Neurosci 30:9172–9180. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1717-10.2010
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Dan Y,
    2. Poo M-M
    (2004) Spike timing-dependent plasticity of neural circuits. Neuron 44:23–30. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Denizet M,
    2. Cotter L,
    3. Lledo P-M,
    4. Lazarini F
    (2017) Sensory deprivation increases phagocytosis of adult-born neurons by activated microglia in the olfactory bulb. Brain Behav Immun 60:38–43. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2016.09.015
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Duménieu M,
    2. Marquèze-Pouey B,
    3. Russier M,
    4. Debanne D
    (2021) Mechanisms of plasticity in subcortical visual areas. Cells 10:3162. doi:10.3390/cells10113162
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  32. ↵
    1. Feldman DE
    (2009) Synaptic mechanisms for plasticity in neocortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 32:33–55. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135516
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Fiske BK,
    2. Brunjes PC
    (2001) Cell death in the developing and sensory-deprived rat olfactory bulb. J Comp Neurol 431:311–319. doi:10.1002/1096-9861(20010312)431:3<311::AID-CNE1072>3.0.CO;2-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Fokkens WJ
    , et al. (2012) European position paper on rhinosinusitis and nasal polyps 2012. Rhinology 50:329. doi:10.4193/Rhino50E2
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  35. ↵
    1. Förster CR,
    2. Illing RB
    (2000) Plasticity of the auditory brainstem: cochleostomy-induced changes of calbindin–D28k expression in the rat. J Comp Neurol 416:173–187. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000110)416:2<173::aid-cne4>3.0.co;2-v
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Fox K,
    2. Stryker M
    (2017) Integrating Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity: introduction. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci 372:20160413. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0413
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Francis HW,
    2. Manis PB
    (2000) Effects of deafferentation on the electrophysiology of ventral cochlear nucleus neurons. Hear Res 149:91–105. doi:10.1016/s0378-5955(00)00165-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Fredrich M,
    2. Zeber AC,
    3. Hildebrandt H,
    4. Illing R-B
    (2013) Differential molecular profiles of astrocytes in degeneration and re-innervation after sensory deafferentation of the adult rat cochlear nucleus. Eur J Neurosci 38:2041–2056. doi:10.1111/ejn.12200
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. Gainey MA,
    2. Aman JW,
    3. Feldman DE
    (2018) Rapid disinhibition by adjustment of PV intrinsic excitability during whisker map plasticity in mouse S1. J Neurosci 38:4749–4761. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3628-17.2018
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Gainey MA,
    2. Feldman DE
    (2017) Multiple shared mechanisms for homeostatic plasticity in rodent somatosensory and visual cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 372:20160157. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Galliano E,
    2. Hahn C,
    3. Browne LP,
    4. Villamayor PR,
    5. Tufo C,
    6. Crespo A,
    7. Grubb MS
    (2021) Brief sensory deprivation triggers cell type-specific structural and functional plasticity in olfactory bulb neurons. J Neurosci 41:2135–2151. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1606-20.2020
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    1. Gambino F,
    2. Holtmaat A
    (2012) Spike-timing-dependent potentiation of sensory surround in the somatosensory cortex is facilitated by deprivation-mediated disinhibition. Neuron 75:490–502. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. García-Hernández S,
    2. Abe M,
    3. Sakimura K,
    4. Rubio ME
    (2017) Impaired auditory processing and altered structure of the endbulb of Held synapse in mice lacking the GluA3 subunit of AMPA receptors. Hear Res 344:284–294. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2016.12.006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. García del Caño G,
    2. Gerrikagoitia I,
    3. Martínez-Millán L
    (2002) Plastic reaction of the rat visual corticocollicular connection after contralateral retinal deafferentiation at the neonatal or adult stage: axonal growth versus reactive synaptogenesis. J Comp Neurol 446:166–178. doi:10.1002/cne.10179
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Garcia MM,
    2. Edward R,
    3. Brennan GB,
    4. Harlan RE
    (2000) Deafferentation-induced changes in protein kinase C expression in the rat cochlear nucleus. Hear Res 147:113–124. doi:10.1016/s0378-5955(00)00125-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. George NM,
    2. Gentile Polese A,
    3. Merle L,
    4. Macklin WB,
    5. Restrepo D
    (2022) Excitable axonal domains adapt to sensory deprivation in the olfactory system. J Neurosci 42:1491–1509. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0305-21.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Gerrikagoitia I,
    2. García Del Caño G,
    3. Martínez-Millán L
    (2001) Changes of the cholinergic input to the superior colliculus following enucleation in neonatal and adult rats. Brain Res 898:61–72. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02142-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Gerrikagoitia I,
    2. García del Caño G,
    3. Martínez-Millán L
    (2002) Quantifying presynaptic terminals at the light microscope level in intact and deafferented central nervous structures. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 9:165–172. doi:10.1016/s1385-299x(02)00142-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Giraldi-Guimarães A,
    2. Mendez-Otero R
    (2005) Induction of the candidate-plasticity NGFI-A protein in the adult rat superior colliculus after visual stimulation. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 133:242–252. doi:10.1016/j.molbrainres.2004.10.020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Gold JR,
    2. Bajo VM
    (2014) Insult-induced adaptive plasticity of the auditory system. Front Neurosci 8:110. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00110
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Gonzalez-Perez O,
    2. López-Virgen V,
    3. Ibarra-Castaneda N
    (2018) Permanent whisker removal reduces the density of c-Fos+ cells and the expression of calbindin protein, disrupts hippocampal neurogenesis and affects spatial-memory-related tasks. Front Cell Neurosci 12:132. doi:10.3389/fncel.2018.00132
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Gonzalez D,
    2. Satriotomo I,
    3. Miki T,
    4. Lee K-Y,
    5. Yokoyama T,
    6. Touge T,
    7. Matsumoto Y,
    8. Li H-P,
    9. Kuriyama S,
    10. Takeuchi Y
    (2005) Effects of monocular enucleation on calbindin-D 28k and c-Fos expression in the lateral geniculate nucleus in rats. Okajimas Folia Anat Jpn 82:9–18. doi:10.2535/ofaj.82.9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Gonzalez D,
    2. Satriotomo I,
    3. Miki T,
    4. Lee K-Y,
    5. Yokoyama T,
    6. Touge T,
    7. Matsumoto Y,
    8. Li H-P,
    9. Kuriyama S,
    10. Takeuchi Y
    (2006) Changes of parvalbumin immunoreactive neurons and GFAP immunoreactive astrocytes in the rat lateral geniculate nucleus following monocular enucleation. Neurosci Lett 395:149–154. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.067
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Gribaudo S,
    2. Bovetti S,
    3. Friard O,
    4. Denorme M,
    5. Oboti L,
    6. Fasolo A,
    7. De Marchis S
    (2012) Transitory and activity-dependent expression of neurogranin in olfactory bulb tufted cells during mouse postnatal development. J Comp Neurol 520:3055–3069. doi:10.1002/cne.23150
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Grier BD,
    2. Belluscio L,
    3. Cheetham CEJ
    (2016) Olfactory sensory activity modulates microglial–neuronal interactions during dopaminergic cell loss in the olfactory bulb. Front Cell Neurosci 10:178. doi:10.3389/fncel.2016.00178
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Hamilton KA,
    2. Parrish-Aungst S,
    3. Margolis FL,
    4. Erdélyi F,
    5. Szabó G,
    6. Puche AC
    (2008) Sensory deafferentation transsynaptically alters neuronal GluR1 expression in the external plexiform layer of the adult mouse main olfactory bulb. Chem Senses 33:201–210. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjm079
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Harris JA,
    2. Iguchi F,
    3. Seidl AH,
    4. Lurie DI,
    5. Rubel EW
    (2008) Afferent deprivation elicits a transcriptional response associated with neuronal survival after a critical period in the mouse cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 28:10990–11002. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2697-08.2008
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Heeringa AN,
    2. Stefanescu RA,
    3. Raphael Y,
    4. Shore SE
    (2016) Altered vesicular glutamate transporter distributions in the mouse cochlear nucleus following cochlear insult. Neuroscience 315:114–124. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.009
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  54. ↵
    1. Henegar JR,
    2. Maruniak JA
    (1991) Quantification of the effects of long-term unilateral naris closure on the olfactory bulbs of adult mice. Brain Res 568:230–234. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(91)91402-m
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Hengen KB,
    2. Lambo ME,
    3. Van Hooser SD,
    4. Katz DB,
    5. Turrigiano GG
    (2013) Firing rate homeostasis in visual cortex of freely behaving rodents. Neuron 80:335–342. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.038
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Hensch TK
    (2005) Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:877–888. doi:10.1038/nrn1787
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Henton A,
    2. Zhao Y,
    3. Tzounopoulos T
    (2023) A role for KCNQ channels on cell type-specific plasticity in mouse auditory cortex after peripheral damage. J Neurosci 43:2277–2290. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1070-22.2023
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Hernandes MS,
    2. Britto LRG,
    3. Real CC,
    4. Martins DO,
    5. Lopes LR
    (2010) Reactive oxygen species and the structural remodeling of the visual system after ocular enucleation. Neuroscience 170:1249–1260. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.07.065
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Heusinger J,
    2. Hildebrandt H,
    3. Illing R-B
    (2019) Sensory deafferentation modulates and redistributes neurocan in the rat auditory brainstem. Brain Behav 9:e01353. doi:10.1002/brb3.1353
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  59. ↵
    1. Hildebrandt H,
    2. Hoffmann NA,
    3. Illing R-B
    (2011) Synaptic reorganization in the adult rat’s ventral cochlear nucleus following its total sensory deafferentation. PLoS One 6:e23686. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023686
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Hillen AEJ,
    2. Burbach JPH,
    3. Hol EM
    (2018) Cell adhesion and matricellular support by astrocytes of the tripartite synapse. Prog Neurobiol 165–167:66–86. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2018.02.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Hooks BM,
    2. Chen C
    (2007) Critical periods in the visual system: changing views for a model of experience-dependent plasticity. Neuron 56:312–326. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Hooks BM,
    2. Chen C
    (2008) Vision triggers an experience-dependent sensitive period at the retinogeniculate synapse. J Neurosci 28:4807–4817. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4667-07.2008
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Horsburgh K,
    2. McCulloch J
    (1991) Alterations of functional glucose use and ligand binding to second messenger systems following unilateral orbital enucleation. Brain Res 549:317–321. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(91)90474-a
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Howarth A,
    2. Shone GR
    (2006) Ageing and the auditory system. Postgrad Med J 82:166–171. doi:10.1136/pgmj.2005.039388
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Hubel DH,
    2. Wiesel TN
    (1963) Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young, visually inexperienced kittens. J Neurophysiol 26:994–1002. doi:10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.994
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Huh CYL,
    2. Abdelaal K,
    3. Salinas KJ,
    4. Gu D,
    5. Zeitoun J,
    6. Figueroa Velez DX,
    7. Peach JP,
    8. Fowlkes CC,
    9. Gandhi SP
    (2020) Long-term monocular deprivation during juvenile critical period disrupts binocular integration in mouse visual thalamus. J Neurosci 40:585–604. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1626-19.2019
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  67. ↵
    1. Huntley GW
    (2012) Synaptic circuit remodelling by matrix metalloproteinases in health and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:743–757. doi:10.1038/nrn3320
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Illing R-B,
    2. Buschky H,
    3. Tadic A
    (2019) Mitotic activity, modulation of DNA processing, and purinergic signalling in the adult rat auditory brainstem following sensory deafferentation. Eur J Neurosci 50:3985–4003. doi:10.1111/ejn.14521
    OpenUrlCrossRef
    1. Illing RB,
    2. Horváth M
    (1995) Re-emergence of GAP-43 in cochlear nucleus and superior olive following cochlear ablation in the rat. Neurosci Lett 194:9–12. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(95)11706-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Inoue N,
    2. Nishizumi H,
    3. Ooyama R,
    4. Mogi K,
    5. Nishimori K,
    6. Kikusui T,
    7. Sakano H
    (2021) The olfactory critical period is determined by activity-dependent Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling within glomeruli. eLife 10:e65078. doi:10.7554/eLife.65078
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  70. ↵
    1. Jaepel J,
    2. Hübener M,
    3. Bonhoeffer T,
    4. Rose T
    (2017) Lateral geniculate neurons projecting to primary visual cortex show ocular dominance plasticity in adult mice. Nat Neurosci 20:1708–1714. doi:10.1038/s41593-017-0021-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Jamann N,
    2. Dannehl D,
    3. Lehmann N,
    4. Wagener R,
    5. Thielemann C,
    6. Schultz C,
    7. Staiger J,
    8. Kole MHP,
    9. Engelhardt M
    (2021) Sensory input drives rapid homeostatic scaling of the axon initial segment in mouse barrel cortex. Nat Commun 12:23. doi:10.1038/s41467-020-20232-x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Janz P,
    2. Illing R-B
    (2014) A role for microglial cells in reshaping neuronal circuitry of the adult rat auditory brainstem after its sensory deafferentation. J Neurosci Res 92:432–445. doi:10.1002/jnr.23334
    OpenUrlCrossRef
    1. Jin Y-M,
    2. Godfrey DA,
    3. Sun Y
    (2005) Effects of cochlear ablation on choline acetyltransferase activity in the rat cochlear nucleus and superior olive. J Neurosci Res 81:91–101. doi:10.1002/jnr.20536
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Jones EG
    (2000) Cortical and subcortical contributions to activity-dependent plasticity in primate somatosensory cortex. Ann Rev Neurosci 23:1–37. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Karmarkar UR,
    2. Buonomano DV
    (2006) Different forms of homeostatic plasticity are engaged with distinct temporal profiles. Eur J Neurosci 23:1575–1584. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04692.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Kawabata K,
    2. Maeda S,
    3. Takanaga A,
    4. Ito H,
    5. Tanaka K,
    6. Hayakawa T,
    7. Seki M
    (2003) Apoptosis and retinal projections in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus after monocular deprivation during the later phase of the critical period in the rat. Anat Sci Int 78:104–110. doi:10.1046/j.0022-7722.2003.00047.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Keck T, et al.
    (2017) Integrating Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity: the current state of the field and future research directions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 372:20160158. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0158
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Keck T,
    2. Scheuss V,
    3. Jacobsen RI,
    4. Wierenga CJ,
    5. Eysel UT,
    6. Bonhoeffer T,
    7. Hübener M
    (2011) Loss of sensory input causes rapid structural changes of inhibitory neurons in adult mouse visual cortex. Neuron 71:869–882. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.034
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Kelsch W,
    2. Lin C-W,
    3. Mosley CP,
    4. Lois C
    (2009) A critical period for activity-dependent synaptic development during olfactory bulb adult neurogenesis. J Neurosci 29:11852–11858. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2406-09.2009
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Kikuta S,
    2. Sakamoto T,
    3. Nagayama S,
    4. Kanaya K,
    5. Kinoshita M,
    6. Kondo K,
    7. Tsunoda K,
    8. Mori K,
    9. Yamasoba T
    (2015) Sensory deprivation disrupts homeostatic regeneration of newly generated olfactory sensory neurons after injury in adult mice. J Neurosci 35:2657–2673. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2484-14.2015
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  78. ↵
    1. King AJ,
    2. Parsons CH
    (1999) Improved auditory spatial acuity in visually deprived ferrets. Eur J Neurosci 11:3945–3956. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00821.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Kosaka T,
    2. Katsumaru H,
    3. Hama K,
    4. Wu JY,
    5. Heizmann CW
    (1987) GABAergic neurons containing the Ca2+-binding protein parvalbumin in the rat hippocampus and dentate gyrus. Brain Res 419:119–130. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(87)90575-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Kosaka T,
    2. Kosaka K
    (2016) Neuronal organization of the main olfactory bulb revisited. Anat Sci Int 91:115–127. doi:10.1007/s12565-015-0309-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Kreutz MR,
    2. Weise J,
    3. Dieterich DC,
    4. Kreutz M,
    5. Balczarek P,
    6. Böckers TM,
    7. Wittkowski W,
    8. Gundelfinger ED,
    9. Sabel BA
    (2004) Rearrangement of the retino-collicular projection after partial optic nerve crush in the adult rat. Eur J Neurosci 19:247–257. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03087.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Kuhlman SJ,
    2. Olivas ND,
    3. Tring E,
    4. Ikrar T,
    5. Xu X,
    6. Trachtenberg JT
    (2013) A disinhibitory microcircuit initiates critical-period plasticity in the visual cortex. Nature 501:543–546. doi:10.1038/nature12485
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Kullmann DM,
    2. Moreau AW,
    3. Bakiri Y,
    4. Nicholson E
    (2012) Plasticity of inhibition. Neuron 75:951–962. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.030
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Kumar M,
    2. Handy G,
    3. Kouvaros S,
    4. Zhao Y,
    5. Brinson LL,
    6. Wei E,
    7. Bizup B,
    8. Doiron B,
    9. Tzounopoulos T
    (2023) Cell-type-specific plasticity of inhibitory interneurons in the rehabilitation of auditory cortex after peripheral damage. Nat Commun 14:4170. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-39732-7
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  83. ↵
    1. Kupers R,
    2. Ptito M
    (2014) Compensatory plasticity and cross-modal reorganization following early visual deprivation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 41:36–52. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Land PW
    (1987) Dependence of cytochrome oxidase activity in the rat lateral geniculate nucleus on retinal innervation. J Comp Neurol 262:78–89. doi:10.1002/cne.902620107
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Lane RD,
    2. Allan DM,
    3. Bennett-Clarke CA,
    4. Rhoades RW
    (1996) Differential age-dependent effects of retinal deafferentation upon calbindin- and parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the superficial layers of the rat’s superior colliculus. Brain Res 740:208–214. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(96)00867-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Lau CG,
    2. Murthy VN
    (2012) Activity-dependent regulation of inhibition via GAD67. J Neurosci 32:8521–8531. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1245-12.2012
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  85. ↵
    1. Leist M,
    2. Datunashvilli M,
    3. Kanyshkova T,
    4. Zobeiri M,
    5. Aissaoui A,
    6. Cerina M,
    7. Romanelli MN,
    8. Pape H-C,
    9. Budde T
    (2016) Two types of interneurons in the mouse lateral geniculate nucleus are characterized by different h-current density. Sci Rep 6:24904. doi:10.1038/srep24904
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Lepousez G,
    2. Valley MT,
    3. Lledo P-M
    (2013) The impact of adult neurogenesis on olfactory bulb circuits and computations. Ann Rev Physiol 75:339–363. doi:10.1146/annurev-physiol-030212-183731
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. Li L,
    2. Gainey MA,
    3. Goldbeck JE,
    4. Feldman DE
    (2014) Rapid homeostasis by disinhibition during whisker map plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:1616–1621. doi:10.1073/pnas.1312455111
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Linden ML,
    2. Heynen AJ,
    3. Haslinger RH,
    4. Bear MF
    (2009) Thalamic activity that drives visual cortical plasticity. Nat Neurosci 12:390–392. doi:10.1038/nn.2284
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. Liu X,
    2. Huang H,
    3. Snutch TP,
    4. Cao P,
    5. Wang L,
    6. Wang F
    (2022) The superior colliculus: cell types, connectivity, and behavior. Neurosci Bull 38:1519–1540. doi:10.1007/s12264-022-00858-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  89. ↵
    1. Louros SR,
    2. Hooks BM,
    3. Litvina L,
    4. Carvalho AL,
    5. Chen C
    (2014) A role for stargazin in experience-dependent plasticity. Cell Rep 7:1614–1625. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2014.04.054
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Luo L,
    2. Ryan AF,
    3. Saint Marie RL
    (1999) Cochlear ablation alters acoustically induced c-fos mRNA expression in the adult rat auditory brainstem. J Comp Neurol 404:271–283. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990208)404:2<271::aid-cne10>3.0.co;2-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Maffei A,
    2. Turrigiano GG
    (2008) Multiple modes of network homeostasis in visual cortical layer 2/3. J Neurosci 28:4377–4384. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5298-07.2008
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Mandairon N,
    2. Jourdan F,
    3. Didier A
    (2003) Deprivation of sensory inputs to the olfactory bulb up-regulates cell death and proliferation in the subventricular zone of adult mice. Neuroscience 119:507–516. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00172-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Marks CA,
    2. Cheng K,
    3. Cummings DM,
    4. Belluscio L
    (2006) Activity-dependent plasticity in the olfactory intrabulbar map. J Neurosci 26:11257–11266. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2805-06.2006
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  92. ↵
    1. Maruniak JA,
    2. Taylor JA,
    3. Henegar JR,
    4. Williams MB
    (1989) Unilateral naris closure in adult mice: atrophy of the deprived-side olfactory bulbs. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 47:27–33. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(89)90105-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Mast TG,
    2. Fadool DA
    (2012) Mature and precursor brain-derived neurotrophic factor have individual roles in the mouse olfactory bulb. PLoS One 7:e31978. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031978
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Mendonça HR,
    2. Araújo SES,
    3. Gomes ALT,
    4. Sholl-Franco A,
    5. da Cunha Faria Melibeu A,
    6. Serfaty CA,
    7. Campello-Costa P
    (2010) Expression of GAP-43 during development and after monocular enucleation in the rat superior colliculus. Neurosci Lett 477:23–27. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Miguel-Hidalgo JJ,
    2. Senba E,
    3. Takatsuji K,
    4. Tohyama M
    (1990) Substance P and enkephalins in the superficial layers of the rat superior colliculus: differential plastic effects of retinal deafferentation. J Comp Neurol 299:389–404. doi:10.1002/cne.902990402
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Miguel-Hidalgo JJ,
    2. Senba E,
    3. Takatsuji K,
    4. Tohyama M
    (1991) Ultrastructure and retinal innervation of deafferentation-induced enkephalin-immunoreactive elements in the superficial layers of the rat superior colliculus. Brain Res 556:175–179. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(91)90565-d
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Mostafapour SP,
    2. Del Puerto NM,
    3. Rubel EW
    (2002) bcl-2 overexpression eliminates deprivation-induced cell death of brainstem auditory neurons. J Neurosci 22:4670–4674. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04670.2002
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Muguruma K,
    2. Matsumura K,
    3. Watanabe Y,
    4. Shiomitsu T,
    5. Imamura K,
    6. Watanabe Y
    (1997) Effects of monocular enucleation on receptor binding and innervation pattern of the noradrenergic system in the superior colliculus of the pigmented rat. Neurosci Res 28:311–324. doi:10.1016/s0168-0102(97)00059-x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Mullol J,
    2. Alobid I,
    3. Mariño-Sánchez F,
    4. Izquierdo-Domínguez A,
    5. Marin C,
    6. Klimek L,
    7. Wang D-Y,
    8. Liu Z
    (2020) The loss of smell and taste in the COVID-19 outbreak: a tale of many countries. Curr Allergy Asthma Rep 20:61. doi:10.1007/s11882-020-00961-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  94. ↵
    1. Muth CC
    (2017) Sudden vision loss. JAMA 318:584. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.7950
    OpenUrlCrossRef
    1. Nakamura S,
    2. Shirokawa T,
    3. Sakaguchi T
    (1984) Increased projection from the locus coeruleus to the lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex in young adult rats following unilateral enucleation. Neurosci Lett 49:77–80. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(84)90139-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Nakamura M,
    2. Valerio P,
    3. Bhumika S,
    4. Barkat TR
    (2020) Sequential organization of critical periods in the mouse auditory system. Cell Rep 32:108070. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108070
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Narushima M
    , et al. (2016) The metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 1 mediates experience-dependent maintenance of mature synaptic connectivity in the visual thalamus. Neuron 91:1097–1109. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.07.035
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Neff EP
    (2019) The noses have it: murine brain development starts with a smell. Lab Anim 48:114–114. doi:10.1038/s41684-019-0272-3
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  98. ↵
    1. Oertel D,
    2. Young ED
    (2004) What’s a cerebellar circuit doing in the auditory system? Trends Neurosci 27:104–110. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2003.12.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Park JW,
    2. Wang X,
    3. Xu R-H
    (2022) Revealing the mystery of persistent smell loss in Long COVID patients. Int J Biol Sci 18:4795–4808. doi:10.7150/ijbs.73485
    OpenUrlCrossRef
    1. Parrish-Aungst S,
    2. Kiyokage E,
    3. Szabo G,
    4. Yanagawa Y,
    5. Shipley MT,
    6. Puche AC
    (2011) Sensory experience selectively regulates transmitter synthesis enzymes in interglomerular circuits. Brain Res 1382:70–76. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.01.068
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Paulussen M,
    2. Arckens L
    (2012) Striking neuronal thymosin beta 4 expression in the deep layers of the mouse superior colliculus after monocular deprivation. Brain Struct Funct 217:81–91. doi:10.1007/s00429-011-0330-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Pérez-Revuelta L,
    2. Téllez de Meneses PG,
    3. López M,
    4. Briñón JG,
    5. Weruaga E,
    6. Díaz D,
    7. Alonso JR
    (2020) Secretagogin expression in the mouse olfactory bulb under sensory impairments. Sci Rep 10:21533. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-78499-5
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  100. ↵
    1. Philpot BD,
    2. Lim JH,
    3. Brunjes PC
    (1997) Activity-dependent regulation of calcium-binding proteins in the developing rat olfactory bulb. J Comp Neurol 387:12–26. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971013)387:1<12::AID-CNE2>3.0.CO;2-Q
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  101. ↵
    1. Poo C,
    2. Isaacson JS
    (2007) An early critical period for long-term plasticity and structural modification of sensory synapses in olfactory cortex. J Neurosci 27:7553–7558. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-07.2007
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  102. ↵
    1. Pothayee N,
    2. Cummings DM,
    3. Schoenfeld TJ,
    4. Dodd S,
    5. Cameron HA,
    6. Belluscio L,
    7. Koretsky AP
    (2017) Magnetic resonance imaging of odorant activity-dependent migration of neural precursor cells and olfactory bulb growth. Neuroimage 158:232–241. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.060
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  103. ↵
    1. Poveda CM,
    2. Valero ML,
    3. Pernia M,
    4. Alvarado JC,
    5. Ryugo DK,
    6. Merchan MA,
    7. Juiz JM
    (2020) Expression and localization of Kv1.1 and Kv3.1b potassium channels in the cochlear nucleus and inferior colliculus after long-term auditory deafferentation. Brain Sci 10:35. doi:10.3390/brainsci10010035
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  104. ↵
    1. Pozo K,
    2. Goda Y
    (2010) Unraveling mechanisms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuron 66:337–351. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.028
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  105. ↵
    1. Pula JH,
    2. Kwan K,
    3. Yuen CA,
    4. Kattah JC
    (2016) Update on the evaluation of transient vision loss. Clin Ophthalmol 10:297–303. doi:10.2147/OPTH.S94971
    OpenUrlCrossRef
    1. Rhoades RW,
    2. Mooney RD,
    3. Chiaia NL,
    4. Bennett-Clarke CA
    (1990) Development and plasticity of the serotoninergic projection to the hamster’s superior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 299:151–166. doi:10.1002/cne.902990203
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  106. ↵
    1. Rowlands D,
    2. Lensjø KK,
    3. Dinh T,
    4. Yang S,
    5. Andrews MR,
    6. Hafting T,
    7. Fyhn M,
    8. Fawcett JW,
    9. Dick G
    (2018) Aggrecan directs extracellular matrix-mediated neuronal plasticity. J Neurosci 38:10102–10113. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1122-18.2018
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  107. ↵
    1. Ryan AF,
    2. Kujawa SG,
    3. Hammill T,
    4. Le Prell C,
    5. Kil J
    (2016) Temporary and permanent noise-induced threshold shifts: a review of basic and clinical observations. Otol Neurotol 37:e271–e275. doi:10.1097/MAO.0000000000001071
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  108. ↵
    1. Sancho L,
    2. Contreras M,
    3. Allen NJ
    (2021) Glia as sculptors of synaptic plasticity. Neurosci Res 167:17–29. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2020.11.005
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  109. ↵
    1. Santos REA,
    2. da Silva MG,
    3. do Monte Silva MCB,
    4. Barbosa DAM,
    5. Gomes ALdV,
    6. Galindo LCM,
    7. da Silva Aragão R,
    8. Ferraz-Pereira KN
    (2021) Onset and duration of symptoms of loss of smell/taste in patients with COVID-19: a systematic review. Am J Otolaryngol 42:102889. doi:10.1016/j.amjoto.2020.102889
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  110. ↵
    1. Sawada M,
    2. Kaneko N,
    3. Inada H,
    4. Wake H,
    5. Kato Y,
    6. Yanagawa Y,
    7. Kobayashi K,
    8. Nemoto T,
    9. Nabekura J,
    10. Sawamoto K
    (2011) Sensory input regulates spatial and subtype-specific patterns of neuronal turnover in the adult olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 31:11587–11596. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0614-11.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  111. ↵
    1. Schwob JE,
    2. Jang W,
    3. Holbrook EH,
    4. Lin B,
    5. Herrick DB,
    6. Peterson JN,
    7. Hewitt Coleman J
    (2017) Stem and progenitor cells of the mammalian olfactory epithelium: taking poietic license. J Comp Neurol 525:1034–1054. doi:10.1002/cne.24105
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Senba E,
    2. Miguell-Hidalgo JJ
    (1993) Substance P in the retina and primary visual centers: its projection and plasticity after deafferentation. Regul Pept 46:129–137. doi:10.1016/0167-0115(93)90023-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  112. ↵
    1. Shepherd GM
    (2004) The synaptic organization of the brain, Ed 5. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159561.001.1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  113. ↵
    1. Stitt AW
    , et al. (2016) The progress in understanding and treatment of diabetic retinopathy. Prog Retin Eye Res 51:156–186. doi:10.1016/j.preteyeres.2015.08.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  114. ↵
    1. Sukekawa K
    (1987) Changes of cytochrome oxidase activity in the rat subcortical visual centers after unilateral eye enucleation. Neurosci Lett 75:127–132. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(87)90285-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  115. ↵
    1. Syka J
    (2002) Plastic changes in the central auditory system after hearing loss, restoration of function, and during learning. Physiol Rev 82:601–636. doi:10.1152/physrev.00002.2002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  116. ↵
    1. Syka J,
    2. Rybalko N
    (2000) Threshold shifts and enhancement of cortical evoked responses after noise exposure in rats. Hear Res 139:59–68. doi:10.1016/s0378-5955(99)00175-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Thurlow GA,
    2. Cooper RM
    (1991) Activity-dependent changes in eye influence during monocular blockade: increases in the effects of visual stimulation on 2-DG uptake in the adult rat geniculostriate system. J Comp Neurol 306:697–707. doi:10.1002/cne.903060411
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Tucci DL,
    2. Cant NB,
    3. Durham D
    (2001) Effects of conductive hearing loss on gerbil central auditory system activity in silence. Hear Res 155:124–132. doi:10.1016/s0378-5955(01)00256-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  117. ↵
    1. Turrigiano G
    (2011) Too many cooks? Intrinsic and synaptic homeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinement. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:89–103. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153238
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  118. ↵
    1. Tyler WJ,
    2. Petzold GC,
    3. Pal SK,
    4. Murthy VN
    (2007) Experience-dependent modification of primary sensory synapses in the mammalian olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 27:9427–9438. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0664-07.2007
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  119. ↵
    1. Veyrac A,
    2. Didier A,
    3. Colpaert F,
    4. Jourdan F,
    5. Marien M
    (2005) Activation of noradrenergic transmission by alpha2-adrenoceptor antagonists counteracts deafferentation-induced neuronal death and cell proliferation in the adult mouse olfactory bulb. Exp Neurol 194:444–456. doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2005.03.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Vierci G,
    2. Oliveira CSD,
    3. Perera LR,
    4. Bornia N,
    5. Leal RB,
    6. Rossi FM
    (2013) Creb is modulated in the mouse superior colliculus in developmental and experimentally-induced models of plasticity. Int J Dev Neurosci 31:46–52. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2012.10.003
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  120. ↵
    1. Wang W-F,
    2. Kiyosawa M,
    3. Ishiwata K,
    4. Mochizuki M
    (2005) Glucose metabolism in the visual structures of rat monocularly deprived by eyelid suture after postnatal eye opening. Jpn J Ophthalmol 49:6–11. doi:10.1007/s10384-004-0146-z
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  121. ↵
    1. Wefelmeyer W,
    2. Puhl CJ,
    3. Burrone J
    (2016) Homeostatic plasticity of subcellular neuronal structures: from inputs to outputs. Trends Neurosci 39:656–667. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2016.08.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  122. ↵
    1. Weyand TG
    (2016) The multifunctional lateral geniculate nucleus. Rev Neurosci 27:135–157. doi:10.1515/revneuro-2015-0018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  123. ↵
    1. Whiting B,
    2. Moiseff A,
    3. Rubio ME
    (2009) Cochlear nucleus neurons redistribute synaptic AMPA and glycine receptors in response to monaural conductive hearing loss. Neuroscience 163:1264–1276. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.07.049
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  124. ↵
    1. Wilson D,
    2. Sullivan R
    (1995) The D2 antagonist spiperone mimics the effects of olfactory deprivation on mitral/tufted cell odor response patterns. J Neurosci 15:5574–5581. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-08-05574.1995
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Wilson DA,
    2. Wood JG
    (1992) Functional consequences of unilateral olfactory deprivation: time-course and age sensitivity. Neuroscience 49:183–192. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(92)90086-h
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  125. ↵
    1. Worrall G
    (2007) Acute otitis media. Can Fam Physician 53:2147–2148. doi:PMID: 18077757
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  126. ↵
    1. Yamaguchi M,
    2. Mori K
    (2005) Critical period for sensory experience-dependent survival of newly generated granule cells in the adult mouse olfactory bulb. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:9697–9702. doi:10.1073/pnas.0406082102
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  127. ↵
    1. Yan XX,
    2. Ribak CE
    (1998) Increased expression of GABA transporters, GAT-1 and GAT-3, in the deafferented superior colliculus of the rat. Brain Res 783:63–76. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01157-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Yan X-X,
    2. Xiong K,
    3. Luo X-G,
    4. Struble RG,
    5. Clough RW
    (2007) Beta-secretase expression in normal and functionally deprived rat olfactory bulbs: inverse correlation with oxidative metabolic activity. J Comp Neurol 501:52–69. doi:10.1002/cne.21239
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  128. ↵
    1. Yuan Y,
    2. Shi F,
    3. Yin Y,
    4. Tong M,
    5. Lang H,
    6. Polley DB,
    7. Liberman MC,
    8. Edge ASB
    (2014) Ouabain-induced cochlear nerve degeneration: synaptic loss and plasticity in a mouse model of auditory neuropathy. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 15:31–43. doi:10.1007/s10162-013-0419-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  129. ↵
    1. Zilles K,
    2. Wree A,
    3. Petrovic-Minic B,
    4. Schleicher A,
    5. Beck T
    (1989) Different metabolic changes in the lateral geniculate nucleus and the superior colliculus of adult rats after simultaneous or delayed double enucleation. Brain Res 488:14–21. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(89)90688-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Matthew Grubb, King's College London

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: C. Geoffrey Lau, Wei Wen.

Synthesis:

Both Reviewers are very positive about this manuscript. Reviewer 2 seeks clarification on an important point regarding timepoints. After consultation we agreed that this need not necessarily involve changing the search terms or the publications included in the review/analysis. Rather, there needs to be clarity about the use of the terms 'adult'/'developmental'/'critical period' etc, and the removal or explicit justification of any claims that the paper reviews exclusively adult plasticity. Essentially, more clarity is required on exactly what was reviewed/analysed, and why. Please ensure you have fully addressed both Reviewers' comments in any revised version.

Reviewer 1:

This is a timely meta-analytical review of the three major sensory modalities where scientists have worked on the sensory deprivation-dependent plasticity in these brain regions. Some findings are expectable e.g. the study of vision is older than audition and olfaction, increasing number of publications analyzing cell types, but it is still good to have this quantitatively documented. The community needs more papers like this. I am in support of publication of this article.

Minor comment

Lines 280-282: "In recent years more attention has been devoted to glial cells in all circuits (median year of investigation =2003)." I am not sure if 2003 should be classified as "recent" given the search was done in 2021.

Reviewer 2:

In this study, the authors summarize and compare past publications on circuit and neuronal plasticity induced by sensory deprivation across three unique modalities: visual, auditory, and olfactory, particularly focusing on subcortical regions implicated in these pathways. They assess the following aspects of the screened studies: publication trend over time, sex of animal used, deprivation paradigm, plasticity readouts, cell types of interest, and effects of deprivation. They then categorize studies based on the length of deprivation performed, namely short-term (<1 day), medium-term (< 1 week), and long-term (> 1 week), and show that, despite huge variabilities in experimental design, there are common features in the manifestation of neuronal/circuit plasticity under each category.

While this study has been submitted as new research, the paper reads like a miniature review. In this sense, the authors have truly done a remarkable job in providing the field with a comprehensive picture of deprivation-induced plasticity at the interface between PNS and CNS. They have cross-examined common deprivation paradigms regarding to the three different modalities, and have summarized both distinct molecular players involved in each sensory pathway and generalized mechanisms shared by all three. Based on their findings, they have also proposed recommendations for future experimental designs that could improve comparability and reproducibility. I believe the researchers in the sensory system field will find this information interesting and helpful.

My major concern for this study is whether the authors have underestimated the contribution of developmentally regulated changes to the cellular and network responses observed post sensory deprivation. The authors claim in the Introduction (line 99-101) that they have disentangled "developmentally regulated plasticity from activity-dependent plasticity in adulthood" by only including studies that use animals older than P21. First, P21 is hardly considered as entering rodent adulthood, in fact, explosive plasticity mechanisms occur during P21-45 across many brain regions. Second, this statement may be true for the olfactory system (critical period around the first postnatal week), but certainly not for the auditory and visual systems. The auditory system exhibits several sensitive developmental windows as late as P40, and the visual system critical period does not start until P21. Ample evidence in literature suggests that plasticity mechanisms are highly regulated by development during this time. Could the author explain why this factor has not been included in the meta-analysis?

Author Response

Synthesis of Reviews:

Both Reviewers are very positive about this manuscript. Reviewer 2 seeks clarification on an important point regarding timepoints. After consultation we agreed that this need not necessarily involve changing the search terms or the publications included in the review/analysis. Rather, there needs to be clarity about the use of the terms 'adult'/'developmental'/'critical period' etc, and the removal or explicit justification of any claims that the paper reviews exclusively adult plasticity. Essentially, more clarity is required on exactly what was reviewed/analysed, and why. Please ensure you have fully addressed both Reviewers' comments in any revised version.

We thank the reviewers for their thorough examination of our manuscript and for their comments. We are pleased that our effort to quantify and summarize deprivation data across multiple modalities was acknowledged and found valuable to the field.

Below, we outline our responses and the changes we have made to manuscript, figures, and table. We are confident that these changes have enhanced the clarity and quality of our manuscript.

Reviewer 1:

This is a timely meta-analytical review of the three major sensory modalities where scientists have worked on the sensory deprivation-dependent plasticity in these brain regions. Some findings are expectable e.g. the study of vision is older than audition and olfaction, increasing number of publications analyzing cell types, but it is still good to have this quantitatively documented. The community needs more papers like this. I am in support of publication of this article.

We thank the reviewer for their support. To facilitate follow-up studies, we are preparing the entire dataset for full release under a CC BY license. To maintain anonymity, we have indicated the doi of the chosen repository in the title page, and put a placeholder in the methods section, page 5. The data will be made available as soon as possible after publication.

Minor comment

Lines 280-282: "In recent years more attention has been devoted to glial cells in all circuits (median year of investigation =2003)." I am not sure if 2003 should be classified as "recent" given the search was done in 2021.

We apologize for losing track of time and adverbs, by working with so many manuscripts from the 1980s and 1990s we mis-identified 2003 as relatively recent. We have now amended the sentence on page 10.

Reviewer 2:

In this study, the authors summarize and compare past publications on circuit and neuronal plasticity induced by sensory deprivation across three unique modalities: visual, auditory, and olfactory, particularly focusing on subcortical regions implicated in these pathways. They assess the following aspects of the screened studies: publication trend over time, sex of animal used, deprivation paradigm, plasticity readouts, cell types of interest, and effects of deprivation. They then categorize studies based on the length of deprivation performed, namely short-term (<1 day), medium-term (< 1 week), and long-term (> 1 week), and show that, despite huge variabilities in experimental design, there are common features in the manifestation of neuronal/circuit plasticity under each category.

While this study has been submitted as new research, the paper reads like a miniature review. In this sense, the authors have truly done a remarkable job in providing the field with a comprehensive picture of deprivation-induced plasticity at the interface between PNS and CNS. They have cross-examined common deprivation paradigms regarding to the three different modalities, and have summarized both distinct molecular players involved in each sensory pathway and generalized mechanisms shared by all three. Based on their findings, they have also proposed recommendations for future experimental designs that could improve comparability and reproducibility. I believe the researchers in the sensory system field will find this information interesting and helpful.

We thank the reviewer for their supportive comments.

My major concern for this study is whether the authors have underestimated the contribution of developmentally regulated changes to the cellular and network responses observed post sensory deprivation. The authors claim in the Introduction (line 99-101) that they have disentangled "developmentally regulated plasticity from activity-dependent plasticity in adulthood" by only including studies that use animals older than P21. First, P21 is hardly considered as entering rodent adulthood, in fact, explosive plasticity mechanisms occur during P21-45 across many brain regions. Second, this statement may be true for the olfactory system (critical period around the first postnatal week), but certainly not for the auditory and visual systems. The auditory system exhibits several sensitive developmental windows as late as P40, and the visual system critical period does not start until P21. Ample evidence in literature suggests that plasticity mechanisms are highly regulated by development during this time. Could the author explain why this factor has not been included in the meta-analysis?

We agree with the reviewer, and we apologize for the lack of clarity on this essential point.

First, we decided not to include studies in which deprivation was induced in pre-weaning animals to make the work manageable and focussed. The literature on developmental plasticity is vast and extremely diverse and while we believe that developmental plasticity is critical, we feel that its investigation warrants a dedicated study.

Second, as the reviewer indicates, we also appreciate that plasticity is heightened in juvenile animals, and happening at different rates across the three sensory systems. By weaning age an animal can live independently from its mother, but it is by no means fully mature. In fact, independently of critical period windows which change for different modalities and species, one could argue that until it reaches puberty and sexual maturity it is not. However, the systematic recording of sexual maturity is notably lacking in most studies, and worse many lack the exact age of the animal and just indicate "adult". This is in fact why we originally avoided graphing these data and why we chose weaning as cutoff since it was the only milestone that we could clearly identify across all papers. Besides providing a graph, we now acknowledge more explicitly the limitations of this choice.

Changes made:

1. We have rephrased the introduction to better capture the fact that we included studies done in juvenile animals and not only adults, pages 3-4

2. An explanation of why weaning was chosen as a cutoff age is now included in the methods on page 4 (inclusion criteria)

3. A quantification of the number of studies using post-weaning rodents younger than P40 (= juvenile) is now included on page 7 and in Figure 4B. They are a minority, and they are not skewed towards a specific sense.

4. In the result sections on common mechanisms of plasticity (pages 13-17), we have highlighted the studies performed in juvenile animals. When the methodologies and reporting variability allowed, we have drawn direct comparisons with data from fully-adult animals.

5. We have extended the discussion to overtly state the limitations of our study due to age selection, and to advocate the need of future studies fully focussed on the developmental aspects of deprivation-induced plasticity (pre-weaning vs juvenile vs fully mature) specifically in pre-cortical areas - page 19.

6. We have indicated studies using juvenile animals by starring the reference in the summary table, which we have also edited to mention all 91 studies at least once. In this way readers can immediately differentiate the findings from adults (non-starred, the majority) vs juvenile (starred) animals.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 11 (2)
eNeuro
Vol. 11, Issue 2
February 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Deprivation-Induced Plasticity in the Early Central Circuits of the Rodent Visual, Auditory, and Olfactory Systems
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Deprivation-Induced Plasticity in the Early Central Circuits of the Rodent Visual, Auditory, and Olfactory Systems
Li Huang, Francesca Hardyman, Megan Edwards, Elisa Galliano
eNeuro 9 January 2024, 11 (2) ENEURO.0435-23.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0435-23.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Deprivation-Induced Plasticity in the Early Central Circuits of the Rodent Visual, Auditory, and Olfactory Systems
Li Huang, Francesca Hardyman, Megan Edwards, Elisa Galliano
eNeuro 9 January 2024, 11 (2) ENEURO.0435-23.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0435-23.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusions
    • Data availability
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
    • Author Response
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • audition
  • olfaction
  • plasticity
  • rodent
  • sensory deprivation
  • vision

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Deletion of endocannabinoid synthesizing enzyme DAGLα in Pcp2-positive cerebellar Purkinje cells decreases depolarization-induced short-term synaptic plasticity, reduces social preference, and heightens anxiety
  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
Show more Research Article: New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Combinatorial Approaches to Restore Corticospinal Function after Spinal Cord Injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
  • Interference underlies attenuation upon relearning in sensorimotor adaptation
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.