Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by deficits in social interaction and communication, cognitive rigidity, and atypical sensory processing. Recent studies suggest that the basal ganglia, specifically the striatum (NSt), plays an important role in ASD. While striatal interneurons, including cholinergic (ChAT+) and parvalbumin-positive (PV+) GABAergic neurons, have been described to be altered in animal models of ASD, their specific contribution remains elusive. Here, we combined behavioral, anatomical, and electrophysiological quantifications to explore if interneuron balance could be implicated in atypical sensory processing in cortical and striatal somatosensory regions of rats subjected to a valproic acid (VPA) model of ASD. We found that VPA animals showed a significant decrease in the number of ChAT+ and PV+ cells in multiple regions (including the sensorimotor region) of the NSt. We also observed significantly different sensory-evoked responses at the single-neuron and population levels in both striatal and cortical regions, as well as corticostriatal interactions. Therefore, selective elimination of striatal PV+ neurons only partially recapitulated the effects of VPA, indicating that the mechanisms behind the VPA phenotype are much more complex than the elimination of a particular neural subpopulation. Our results indicate that VPA exposure induced significant histological changes in ChAT+ and PV+ cells accompanied by atypical sensory-evoked corticostriatal population dynamics that could partially explain the sensory processing differences associated with ASD.
Significance Statement
One of the main characteristics of patients with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is the hypo- or hyper-responses to sensory stimuli. Various studies indicate that a possible explanation for these atypical responses is an imbalance in excitation–inhibition modulated by different types of interneurons. In the present work, we provide evidence that a striatal imbalance in ChAT+ and PV+ levels could partly explain behavioral and somatosensory processing differences associated with the valproic acid-induced ASD model.
Introduction
Within autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), three important characteristics have been described: (1) differences in social interaction, (2) increased repetitive movements, and (3) cognitive rigidity. Patients with ASD also exhibit sensory dysfunction, characterized by hypersensibility to sound and tactile or visual stimuli (DSM-V; Green et al., 2015; Mikkelsen et al., 2018; Siemann et al., 2020). Although the fundamental mechanisms of sensory hypersensibility are unknown, it has been suggested that circuit-related alterations in the sensory cortex, and particularly a dysfunction of the GABAergic elements of this region (Gogolla et al., 2014; Stoner et al., 2014; Canetta et al., 2016; Q. Chen et al., 2020), play a key role in these differences. Likewise, another area that participates in sensory processing is the striatum (NSt), the main nucleus of the basal ganglia that has been previously suggested as a key participant in ASD (Fuccillo, 2016; Rapanelli et al., 2017a). The NSt processes and integrates inputs from almost the entire cerebral cortex and multiple thalamic regions, including information from sensory and motor areas. Synaptic inputs are processed in medium spiny neurons (MSNs) and different types of GABAergic and cholinergic interneurons (Kawaguchi et al., 1995; Tepper and Bolam, 2004; Tepper et al., 2010, 2018). It has been proposed that the dysfunction of any of these components would lead to poor processing of synaptic inputs and, therefore, aberrant behaviors such as those observed in ASD (Marín, 2012). In this context, previous studies using rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid (VPA-exposed) as a model of ASD have reported anatomical and physiological alterations of MSNs, for example, an increase in both dendritic arborization and dendritic spines in direct pathway MSNs and the opposite in indirect pathway MSNs (Bringas et al., 2013; H. Kim et al., 2022). It has been suggested that these anatomical alterations may be related to changes in MSN excitability (Di et al., 2022; Iezzi et al., 2022). On the other hand, other studies have shown alterations in cholinergic transmission at the cortical (rats/mice VPA-exposed to VPA) and striatal (BTBR mice) levels in different animal models of ASD (J. W. Kim et al., 2014; Karvat and Kimchi, 2014) and a decrease in the number of both cholinergic and parvalbumin-positive (PV+) cells in the NSt in the shank mouse ASD model (Filice et al., 2016). Likewise, the depletion of both cholinergic (ChAT) and PV interneurons (Rapanelli et al., 2017b) mimics some autistic features. Research has found that PV+ interneurons are fundamental to sculpt MSN ensemble activity and facilitate movement (Gernert et al., 2000; Gittis et al., 2010), and cholinergic interneurons participate in movement control, decision-making, and cognitive processes (Hasselmo and Sarter, 2011; Apicella, 2017; Prado et al., 2017; Gritton et al., 2019). Hence, decreased or dysfunctional activity of these elements could underlie repetitive movements and cognitive rigidity (Xu et al., 2015; Matamales et al., 2016b). To explore these possibilities and the potential contribution of ChAT+ and PV+ interneurons to sensory hypersensitivity, we studied absolute changes in the number of these cells, as well as somatosensory processing throughout corticostriatal sensory networks associated with a widely used rodent model of ASD by prenatal exposure to VPA (Rodier et al., 1997; J. W. Kim et al., 2014; Maisterrena et al., 2022). We found a significant decrease in both ChAT+ and PV+ striatal interneuron quantities compared with those of control animals. Moreover, for both cell types, we observed that this decrease was more noticeable in the dorsolateral striatum (DLSt). On the other hand, extracellular recordings of the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and DLSt in anesthetized animals exhibited exacerbated somatosensory-evoked responses at the single-neuron and population levels. Our data suggest that unbalanced levels of striatal interneurons may be instrumental in sensory process differences associated with autism.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Sprague Dawley rats were obtained from the animal facility of the Universidad Autónoma de San Luis Potosí, Mexico. Pregnant rats were housed individually in a temperature-controlled room (24°C) under a 12 h light/dark cycle with food and water ad libitum. Long–Evans rats [LE-Tg [Pvalb-iCre] 2Ottc; (n = 4); 250–350 g; PV + Cre-Casp] were obtained from the Rat Resource and Research Center at the University of Missouri (MTA-TO; NIMH Ref. No. 2018-0136; Transgenic line 773) and reproduced in our animal facility. To express Caspase 3, the adeno-associated virus pAAV-flex-taCasp3-TEVp was unilaterally injected (1 µl per site of injection) into the DLSt (in mm, AP = 0.6; ML = ±3.5; DV = −4.4). pAAV-flex-taCasp3-TEVp was a gift from Nirao Shah and Jim Wells (Addgene viral prep # 45580-AAV5; http://n2t.net/addgene:45580; RRID: Addgene_45580). All procedures described in the present study were in agreement with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the Mexican Council for Animal Care (Norma Oficial Mexicana NOM-062-ZOO-1999), the internal committee (BGFMUASLP-08-20), and the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. All efforts were made to minimize the number of rats used and any possible discomfort.
Model of ASD in rats
Because autism occurs with a ratio of 3:1 higher incidence in males than that in females (Loomes et al., 2017), the present study was conducted only in male rats. The prenatal exposure to valproic acid (VPA) method was used as described previously (Rodier et al., 1996; Schneider and Przewłocki, 2005; Rinaldi et al., 2008; Markram and Markram, 2010; K. C. Kim et al., 2011; Bringas et al., 2013). In brief, pregnant rats were intraperitoneally injected with VPA (500 mg/kg, single dose; Sigma-Aldrich) or physiological solution at 0.9% of NaCl (PSS), on gestational day 12.5. VPA was dissolved in PSS at a 250 mg/ml concentration. Male offspring were weaned on postnatal day (P) 23. Both groups of animals (i.e., control and VPA-exposed) were kept in separate cages, with a maximum of three animals per cage and in the same conditions as the pregnant rats until their use at the age of P60 for behavior tests or in vivo recordings.
Behavior tests
Three behavioral paradigms were used to evaluate the ASD model. All behavior protocols were video recorded and analyzed offline, and the “activity test counter” software was used to analyze behavior (Velázquez, 2020). To prevent olfactory cue bias, before each behavioral assay, devices were thoroughly cleaned with a solution containing alcohol 5%, ammonia, and multipurpose cleaner at 5%.
Social interaction test
The behavioral apparatus consisted of a transparent acrylic box (in cm, 84 long × 35 wide × 25 high) divided into three 28 × 35 cm compartments interconnected by 9 × 7 cm gates (P. Kim et al., 2013; Karvat and Kimchi, 2014). For habituation to experimental conditions, animals (control or VPA) were placed in the central compartment and allowed to freely explore the three compartments for 5 min. Subsequently, a “novel rat” (i.e., a rat that had not been exposed to the experimental animals) was randomly placed in one of the lateral compartments. The “novel rat” was constrained to a transparent Plexiglas chamber (17.5 × 13.5 × 14 cm). We evaluated the following: (1) how much time the animal spent in the novel animal's compartment; (2) the number of contacts with the novel animal, defined as the times that the animal touched or smelled the novel animal; and (3) the interaction time, defined as the amount of time that the animal interacted with the novel animal. The test duration was 10 min.
Open field test
Individual animals from both groups (control and VPA-exposed) were placed in the center of an acrylic box (70 × 70 × 70 cm) for 20 min (Prut and Belzung, 2003), and the following parameters were analyzed: (1) distance traveled, (2) number of grooming events, and (3) time spent grooming. The last two parameters were indicative of stereotyped behavior (Hartgraves and Randall, 1986; Wu et al., 2019; Li et al., 2024).
Hole-board test
Animals were placed individually into a black acrylic box (40 × 40 × 30 cm). One of the walls contained a 3 cm diameter hole through which a light stimulus was provided to attract the animal's attention (Rodriguiz and Wetsel, 2006; Bringas et al., 2013). Animals were monitored for 10 min, and the following parameters were evaluated: (1) latency to the first time that the animal passed the head through the hole (head-dipping behavior), (2) frequency of head-dipping, and (3) cumulative head-dipping time.
Immunohistochemistry
Immunohistochemistry for choline acetyltransferase (ChAT+, cholinergic interneurons) or parvalbumin (PV+ interneurons) was developed using the peroxidase-diaminobenzidine reaction in samples collected from control and VPA-exposed animals. At the end of each recording session, animals were administered a lethal dose of pentobarbital and transcardially perfused with phosphate buffer saline (PBS, 0.01 M), pH 7.6, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA; in M): 0.08 Na2HPO4 and 0.0225 NaH2PO4. Then, the brains were extracted and kept in PFA overnight before immersion in sucrose solution (30–0.04% NaN3 in PBS) to preserve samples. Brains were later sliced (50 μm) with a vibratome (Leica VT1200S) and prepared for immunohistochemical procedures. First, to remove remains of the red globules from the tissue, samples were washed and cleaned with PBS and incubated in a solution with methanol 10%, H2O2 0.5% in PBS for 15 min. Then, slices were incubated for 72 h at 44°C with ChAT (anti-Choline Acetyltransferase polyclonal, goat Merck Millipore: AB144P200UL;1:400) or PV (Rabbit Anti-Parvalbumin antibody ab11427; 1:1,200) antibodies in a solution containing 0.01 M Tris-buffered saline (TBS), Triton X-100 0.5% (Sigma-Aldrich), and gelatin 0.25% (Merck KGaA). After this period, the tissue was washed with PBS and incubated with a secondary antibody (ChAT, 1:500, Anti Goat IgG/Donkey, Jackson ImmunoResearch: 705-065-003, biotinylated; PV, 1:1,200, Goat Anti-Rabbit IgG H&L ab207995, biotinylated) for 2 h at room temperature. After that, the slices were washed with PBS and the avidin–biotin complex for 2 h (Kit Vectastain Elite ABC – peroxidase; 1:500). Subsequently, slices were revealed with a solution containing 3,39-diaminobenzidine 0.25% with H2O2 0.01% (both Sigma-Aldrich) and nickel ammonium sulfate 10% in TBS for 5 min. Finally, sections were mounted on gelatinized glass slides, dried for 8 h, and dehydrated through a graded series of ethanol, followed by xylene, and covered with epoxy resin in xylol 60% (Hycel lot: 304883). Special care was taken to always process the same number of saline and VPA sections simultaneously, with the same solutions, and in the same multiple 24-well plates.
Cell counting
Four representative slices containing the NSt (∼1.80 to −0.12 mm with respect to the bregma) were selected for each animal. Sections were examined under a light microscope (Leica DM500) at 10× magnification, and images were captured with a digital camera (Leica ICC50 HD). The perimeter of the NSt was delimitated digitally, and ChAT+ and PV+ neurons were marked and represented with colored circles. Then, the ventral striatum (VSt), dorsomedial striatum (DMSt), and DLSt were delimitated manually based on anatomical landmarks with reference to the Paxinos and Watson (2014) rat brain atlas. Finally, for each slide and NSt subdivision, the total area was calculated, ChAT+ and PV+ neurons were counted, and the number of cells per square millimeter was calculated. Digital procedures and quantifications were achieved with Adobe Illustrator CS6 and ImageJ 1.52 K software.
Anesthetized experiments
Recordings were performed in control, VPA, and PV + Cre-Casp animals anesthetized with urethane (1 g/kg) and mounted on a stereotaxic frame; to maintain optimal anesthetic levels, supplemental doses of urethane (0.15 g/kg) were given when necessary. Silicon probe recordings were performed through a craniotomy (2 × 2 mm) centered at 0.6 mm anterior and 3.7 mm lateral to the bregma. Coordinates were selected based on previous reports demonstrating robust forelimb representations in these S1 and DLSt regions. Neural signals were collected at two depths in S1 (between 0.9 and 1.5 mm below the surface of the brain, corresponding to layers IV and V) and at two to four depths in the DLSt (between 3 and 5 mm below the surface of the brain). Probes were slowly lowered into the recording sites (2 mm/h), and recordings were performed at depths where visual inspection of the signals indicated spiking activity in multiple group channels (probe shanks). Histological confirmation of silicon probe positions in the brain was achieved by applying DiI lipophilic carbocyanine dye (1%; Sigma) to the back of the probes. Somatosensory stimulations (5 ms pulses) were performed on the pads of the forelimb contralateral to the recording site with a bipolar electrode located on the medial (+) and lateral pads (−). Stimulation was given every 5 s as trains of five stimuli (3.3 Hz). Stimulation intensity (∼0.4 mA) was selected based on previous reports (Hidalgo-Balbuena et al., 2019; Peña-Rangel et al., 2021) and set for all animals as the minimal current that produced similar deflections in the local field potential of S1 to a brief tapping on the contralateral forepaw pads with a cotton tip. Immediately after the experiments, animals were injected with a lethal dose of pentobarbital and transcardially perfused; their brains were extracted and processed for histological confirmation or immunohistochemical procedures. Under these conditions, no statistical differences were found in the total number of cells recorded at each depth between control or experimental groups (cells recorded per depth in S1 and striatum: control group, 55 depths, median = 9, 75th = 16, 25th = 5; VPA group, 70 depths, median = 8, 75th = 13, 25th = 5; PV + Cre-Casp group, 27 depths, median = 9, 75th = 15, 25th = 6; K–W, X2 = 0.97; p = 0.61).
Electrophysiological data acquisition and processing
Sixty-four-channel silicon probe arrays (NeuroNexus, Buzsaki64) were used in all recordings. Wideband (0.1–8,000 Hz) neurophysiological signals were amplified 1,000 times via the Intan RHD2000-series amplifier evaluation system. Spike sorting was performed semiautomatically using the clustering software KlustaKwik (http://klustakwik.sourceforge.net) and the graphical spike sorting application Klusters (http://klusters.sourceforge.net; Hazan et al., 2006).
Analysis of neural data
Sensory-evoked patterns
The principal component analysis (PCA)/silhouette-based method was used as reported in Peña-Rangel et al. (2021) and Pimentel-Farfan et al. (2022). In brief, PCA was applied to the z-scored cutaneous stimulation-evoked patterns of each recorded neuron in a time window of 1.5 s aligned to the first stimulus of the train. To assign cells to specific clusters with similar characteristics, k-means was applied to the first three principal components (PCs). To obtain the best classification, the process was repeated 1,000 times with projections ranging from three to eight clusters. Each projection was scored with the silhouette method, and the projections with the highest silhouettes scores were selected. This procedure was also applied to extract autocorrelogram shapes in the S1 and DLSt and for spike-wave shapes in the DLSt.
Statistics
For behavioral statistical analysis, the Shapiro–Wilk test was applied to assess normal distribution, and an unpaired t test was used for group comparisons, and the data is reported as the mean ± SEM. For anatomical data, groups were compared using an unpaired t test. In general, non-normal distributions were observed in electrophysiological data; hence, nonparametric statistics were used for this type of data. For most of the cases, Kruskal–Wallis (K–W) and Bonferroni’s post hoc test were utilized for multiple comparisons. The Wilcoxon test was applied as indicated in the text.
Results
Behavioral and cognitive assessments in VPA-exposed animals
One of the main characteristics of ASD patients is the lack of social interaction (Boraston et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2019; Bos et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020), which has also been reported in different rodent models of ASD, such as the VPA-based model or genetic-based models (BTBR; P. Kim et al., 2013; J. W. Kim et al., 2014, 2017; Karvat and Kimchi, 2014). Here we evaluated social interaction at P56–60 (control offspring n = 18 of N = 8 pregnant nontreated rats; VPA offspring n = 18 of N = 10 pregnant VPA-exposed rats). Our data show that the VPA animals spent significantly less time in the compartment with the “novel rat” than the control animals (control 352.8 ± 12.26 s vs VPA 314.1 ± 10.68 s; p = 0.0231; Fig. 1A2). Moreover, compared with the control group, VPA animals showed significantly fewer contacts (control 10.67 ± 0.46 vs VPA 7.33 ± 0.57; p < 0.0001; Fig. 1A3) and a significantly shorter cumulative time of interaction (control 49.31 ± 2.78 s vs VPA 37.15 ± 1.56 s; p = 0.0006; Fig. 1A4) with the novel rat. Importantly, both groups of animals exhibited similar times in each compartment during the habituation period, discarding potential differences related to the baseline levels of exploration (Fig. 1A1).
Another principal feature of ASD is the presence of stereotyped behaviors. To explore whether this behavior was present in our ASD model, VPA and control animals were evaluated in an open field test (Fig. 1B; Prut and Belzung, 2003; D. Chen et al., 2019). Consistent with our previous quantification (Fig. 1A4), both groups of animals showed similar traveled distances (control 2,308 ± 266.1 cm vs VPA 2,563 ± 285 cm, ns; Fig. 1B1). However, compared with the control group, VPA animals exhibited a significant increase in the frequency (control 7.86 ± 0.6218 vs VPA 12.43 ± 1.142; p = 0.0014; Fig. 1B2) and duration of grooming behavior (control 84.96 ± 9.957 vs VPA 170.7 ± 12.21; p = 0.0001; Fig. 1B3), a commonly accepted indicator of increased stereotyped behavior.
Finally, another trait associated with ASD is cognitive rigidity. It has been proposed that one possible way to assess cognitive rigidity in rodents is by evaluating the amount of time that animals spend responding to a salient stimulus. Increased times may be indicative of exacerbated or obsessive attention, also known as attention reorienting deficit (Markram and Markram, 2010; Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014). To evaluate this behavior, we used a variation of the hole-board test (modified from Rodriguiz and Wetsel, 2006; Fig. 1C). Our data show that control and VPA animals exhibited similar latencies to the first head-dipping behavior (Fig. 1C1). However, VPA animals displayed significantly fewer events (control 44.33 ± 1.35 vs VPA 34.83 ± 1.09; p < 0.0001; Fig. 1C2) and accumulated more time with the head hole than control animals (control 99.77 ± 2.74 vs VPA 144 ± 3.41; p < 0.0001; Fig. 1C3), suggesting that the VPA-exposed animals maintained a higher level of attention to the light stimulus.
These results show that our VPA model produced behavioral traits consistent with those previously reported in children with ASD and environmental and genetic animal models, such as impaired social interaction (Karvat and Kimchi, 2014; Franchini et al., 2017; Supekar et al., 2018), increased behavioral stereotypy (J. W. Kim et al., 2017; Hirsch et al., 2018), and attention re-reorienting deficit (Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014). It should be noted that all behavioral tests by themselves present limitations. For example, in our version of the social interaction test, the results may be interpreted as a general lack of interest in exploration and not as a specific effect in the social domain. On the other hand, the results are limited to the interaction with a novel rat and not to social preference, as could also be measured by introducing a familiar versus a novel animal (J. W. Kim et al., 2017).
Decrease of striatal ChAT+ and PV+ cells in VPA-exposed animals
Previous reports in both, shank mice and rats prenatally exposed to VPA, have shown that animal models of ASD exhibit a decrease in the number of striatal PV+ cells (Filice et al., 2016; Lauber et al., 2016; Rapanelli et al., 2017a) and that the depletion of both cholinergic and PV+ interneurons in the NSt of male mice (Rapanelli et al., 2017b) mimics some characteristics of ASD. To confirm these possibilities and explore the significance of these interneurons in striatal functionality, first we developed immunohistochemical quantifications for both types of interneurons in control and VPA animals.
To analyze the striatal cholinergic system, we used antibodies against the enzyme choline acetyltransferase (ChAT+) that is present in all cholinergic cells (Bolam et al., 1984). A detailed inspection of the brain slices showed a homogeneous distribution of ChAT+ interneurons throughout the entire NSt of control animals (Fig. 2A1–3). In contrast, ChAT+ interneurons in VPA animals were scarce, and a formal statistical comparison revealed a general significant decrease of 38.2% (control, 2,335 ± 93.98 cells vs VPA, 1,443 ± 187.1 cells; p = 0.0011; Fig. 2B1–3). Furthermore, VPA sections showed a fainter mark in neuropil than the control sections (Fig. 2, compare A1,2, B1,2). Then we explored whether the decrease was a general phenomenon throughout the NSt or if it was exacerbated in specific regions. We observed that the most important decrease was localized at the level of the DLSt (56.9%), followed by the VSt (36.81%) and DMSt (27.84%; Fig. 2C, Table 1). In addition, we observed that the decrease of ChAT+ cells in the NSt was more pronounced as we moved through the slices from rostral to caudal. Finally, in the control animals, we also noticed that the density of ChAT+ cells in the DMSt decreased in the caudal direction, whereas in the DLSt it followed the opposite pattern (Fig. 2C, Table 2).
On the other hand, another important element of the NSt microcircuit that has been reported to be affected in neurodevelopmental disorders is the PV+ interneurons (Kataoka et al., 2010; Filice et al., 2016; Lauber et al., 2016; Rapanelli et al., 2017a). According to our immunohistochemical quantifications, VPA animals exhibited a 42.5% decrease in the total number of PV+ interneurons along the NSt (control, 3,617 ± 207.3 vs VPA, 2,080 ± 88.67; p = 0.0024; Fig. 3A,B). Similar to the observations in ChAT+ cells, the decrease was greater in the DLSt (50.8%) than in the DMSt (38%) or VSt (28.9%; Table 1). When analyzing PV+ interneurons in the rostrocaudal axis, we found that the three subdivisions (VSt, DMSt, and DLSt) presented the lowest quantities in the medial regions (Table 3, Fig. 3C).
Baseline neuronal dynamics in S1 and DLSt
The decrease in striatal ChAT+ and PV+ cells associated with the ASD model suggests an important disturbance in the striatal microcircuit functionality and, hence, inadequate processing of both motor and somatosensory cortical inputs. To explore this possibility, we performed extracellular recordings on urethane-anesthetized animals (Fig. 4A), a preparation in which somatosensory-evoked signals can be reliably recorded in S1 and DLSt (Hidalgo-Balbuena et al., 2019; Báez-Cordero et al., 2020; Peña-Rangel et al., 2021). Neural recordings were performed in VPA and control adult rats (P70–90) in S1, where a decrease in GABAergic interneurons has been reported in ASD models (Lauber et al., 2016; Q. Chen et al., 2020) and in the DLSt, where we observed the most important decrease in ChAT+ and PV+ interneurons (Fig. 4A,B). Sensory-evoked responses were obtained by applying trains of five stimuli (3.3 Hz, 5 ms/stimulus) to the palm of the forelimb contralateral to the recording site (Fig. 4A; see Materials and Methods).
We analyzed the activity of 610 well-isolated neurons from S1 and 945 from the DLSt of 26 animals (control n = 12; VPA n = 10; and PV + Cree-Casp n = 4), 295 and 340 neurons were collected from S1 and the DLSt, respectively. From the VPA animals, 315 and 388 neurons were obtained from the S1 and DLSt, respectively. And from the PV + Cre-Casp animals, 217 neurons were recorded in the DLSt. Neurons recorded from VPA animals presented slightly but significantly higher baseline firing rates than neurons recorded from control animals in both structures. These differences were observed when comparing all individual neurons or divided by recorded animal (Fig. 4D). To explore possible differences in intrinsic properties between groups, we analyzed spontaneous oscillatory patterns in the spiking activity of individual neurons. By using a PCA-based method to classify the shape of individual autocorrelograms (Peña-Rangel et al., 2021), we identified four prevalent patterns in both groups in the S1 (Fig. 4E) and DLSt (Fig. 4H). These patterns were similarly distributed in both groups in the S1, but with a higher prevalence of pattern type 4 for the VPA group (Fig. 4F; K–W, X2 = 37.1; DF = 7; p < 0.001). On the other hand, a higher representation of pattern type 1 was observed in the DLSt of VPA animals (Fig. 4I; K–W, X2 = 19.02; DF = 7; p = 0.008). This pattern corresponds to neurons with tonic firing rates. Then, for each neuron we analyzed the relative oscillatory power of the autocorrelogram at different frequencies and found no significant differences between groups in either structure (Fig. 4G,J).
The slight increase in baseline firing rates suggests a greater excitability in the cortical and striatal networks of VPA animals (Fig. 4D). To explore the possible consequences of these differences, we evaluated somatosensory processing evoked by cutaneous stimulation of the forelimb contralateral to the recording site (Fig. 4A). S1 and DLSt neurons in both control and VPA animals showed different patterns of reliable sensory-evoked responses, as depicted in the raster plots of multiple representative neurons (Fig. 5A,B). Consistent with previous reports (Hidalgo-Balbuena et al., 2019), in all conditions, many neurons presented complex patterns of activation, being the most salient sharp and short-latency responses to each stimulus of the train. Other neurons presented a complex response, which consisted of a short-latency increase in the activity followed by a transient pause followed by a rebound. Finally, some neurons presented a brief and short-latency pause in their activity.
Somatosensory-evoked neural activity in S1
PV+ cell levels in control and VPA animals did not differ significantly, which is inconsistent with our anatomical observations in the NSt (Fig. 6A,B). To further analyze the network dynamics of control and VPA networks before, during, and after the somatosensory stimulation train (SST), we calculated the Z-score values of the evoked spiking activity (Fig. 5A,D) aligned to the first stimulus of the SST. Following the trend observed in the representative neurons (Fig. 5), both groups presented a complex population response consisting of subpopulation neurons that increased or decreased their spiking activity in a transitory manner (Fig. 6C). A visual inspection of the data revealed that both experimental groups (control and VPA) had a similar general average response shape consisting of a short-latency increase in activity, followed by a transitory pause, and finally a second long-latency increase or “rebound” in activity. This pattern was repeated for each stimulus of the train (Fig. 6C). However, the reduced activity component was stronger in the VPA population than in the control population, and as the stimulus train progressed, the short-latency increases appeared to be smaller (Fig. 6C, bottom panel). To confirm these observations, we first analyzed the latency of the increased activity and found no differences between groups (Fig. 6D). Then, we aimed to analyze the amplitude of the rebound phase of the response; for this, we averaged the activity of the 200 ms windows starting 80 ms after the onset of each stimulus of the train, rendering five values (one for each stimulus) per cell (Fig. 6C, white square). Because the train stimulation could induce facilitation or depression on different neurons, we first calculated the linear growth (or decay) rate of the response amplitude (200 ms window starting 80 ms after stimulus onset) for each neuron and divided the population according to units that presented facilitation or depression. We found that VPA animals had a higher proportion of facilitating units (Fig. 6E), but both groups displayed similar values of linear growth or decay (growth: control, 25th = 0.03, 50th = 0.11, 75th = 0.23; VPA, 25th = 0.04, 50th = 0.11, 75th = 0.23; K–W, X2 = 2.41, p = 0.29; decay: control, 25th = −0.33, 50th = −0.19, 75th = −0.08; VPA, 25th = −0.28, 50th = −0.17, 75th = −0.07; K–W, X2 = 0.47, p = 0.78). Then we calculated the amplitude of the responses of facilitating and depressing units. We found clear facilitating and depressing dynamics in both groups. We identified these dynamics by the significant differences between the amplitude of the first stimulus of the train and the following amplitudes (Fig. 6F, top asterisk and lines). However, when we compared the amplitudes of the control and VPA animals, we found that for some train stimuli, the VPA group displayed lower amplitudes (Fig. 6F, bottom asterisks), suggesting slightly different population dynamics (facilitating units K–W, X2 = 13.83, DF = 9; p = 0.017; inhibiting units, K–W, X2 = 29.75, DF = 9; p < 0.001). These were more evident when we averaged all stimulus responses and found that VPA animals presented a significantly larger inhibitory component than the control group (Fig. 6G; Wilcoxon test, p = 0.01).
Then, to further explore potential evoked response differences between groups, we aimed to classify the response patterns of individual neurons and assess their prevalence in the population. As previously reported (Peña-Rangel et al., 2021), we applied a PCA/silhouette-based method to classify the average response shape of each neuron as part of clusters with similar characteristics (see Materials and Methods). We tested multiple k-means projections ranging from three to eight cluster shapes and found that the projection with the highest silhouette values (indicating higher intra-cluster cohesion and inter-cluster separation; Fig. 6H; K–W, X2 = 4,375, DF = 5; p < 0.001) consisted of four clusters (Fig. 6I) corresponding to four distinct S1 cortical patterns (Fig. 6J) expressed in both control and VPA animals. However, when we analyzed the prevalence of each pattern, we found that VPA animals presented a significantly lower prevalence of patterns type 1 and 4 than the control group (Fig. 6K; K–W, X2 = 17.56, DF = 5; p = 0.014). These patterns were characterized by sustained and sharp sensory stimulation-evoked increases in spiking activity, respectively (Fig. 6J). On the other hand, VPA animals also showed a higher prevalence of pattern type 3 (Fig. 6K), which is characterized by deep pauses induced by each stimulus of the train (Fig. 6J). Altogether, these data confirm that S1 sensory-evoked responses were dominated by an inhibitory component in VPA animals.
The previous data suggest that, as a population, cortical VPA networks may be representing sensory stimuli in a different way than control networks. To further scrutinize this possibility, we applied PCA to the sensory-evoked responses over the whole neuronal population and compared population trajectories between VPA and control animals (see Materials and Methods). First, when plotting the first two PCs against time, we found that population dynamics from the control group were organized as rotational trajectories that followed the same path started by each stimulus of the train (Fig. 6L, black trace). However, population trajectories from VPA animals followed a disorganized path clearly different from that of the control group (Fig. 6L, blue trace). To formally quantify differences between groups, first we calculated the confidence interval for each PC by generating surrogate spike trains by randomly shifting the spike times in a range of ±5 s and plotting them against each component from the control and VPA groups (Fig. 6M–O, gray traces). In the control group, the three components presented an oscillatory shape with five cycles starting and corresponding to each stimulus of the train (Fig. 6M–O, black dots and trajectories); these cycles were well differentiated from the surrogate trajectories. On the contrary, VPA trajectories (Fig. 6M–O, blue dots and trajectories) were clearly oscillatory for PC1 but to a lesser extent for PC2 and PC3 (the latter was completely embedded in the surrogate trajectories). Then, we calculated the average speed and amplitude of each component for the three groups (surrogate, control, and VPA) and performed statistical comparisons. We found no differences in speed or amplitude for PC1, but significant differences between control and VPA animals for PC2 and PC3, being the former significantly faster and wider (Fig. 6P; K–W, X2 = 248.3, DF = 5; p < 0.001). On the other hand, the amplitude of the first component was significantly higher, while the amplitudes of components two and three were significantly lower in VPA animals (Fig. 6Q; K–W, X2 = 337.1, DF = 5; p < 0.001). In both groups, PC1 accounted for ∼25% of the population variability, and PC1 trajectories were characterized by a slow oscillatory component triggered by each stimulus of the train, suggesting that this component is associated with the large drops in activity triggered by each stimulus of the train (Fig. 6C). Hence, changes in the amplitude of PC1 reflect shifts in the general architecture of sensory-evoked responses. Moreover, the higher amplitudes observed in the VPA group are consistent with the previous quantifications, indicating that sensory-evoked activity is dominated by a decrease in activity in the S1 of VPA animals.
Atypical somatosensory stimulus train-evoked neuronal activity in the DLSt of VPA-exposed animals
After analyzing S1 sensory-evoked activity, we proceeded to explore striatal responses to the same stimulation protocol. Consistent with previous reports (Hidalgo-Balbuena et al., 2019; Peña-Rangel et al., 2021), the averaged population responses of the DLSt in control animals displayed a transient increase in activity after the first stimulation pulse, followed by attenuated responses to the subsequent stimuli of the train (Fig. 7A). Neurons that presented short-latency responses to the first stimulus of the train (<30 ms) exhibited no significant differences among experimental conditions (Fig. 7B; Wilcoxon test, p = 0.121). Next, we analyzed the amplitude of the evoked responses. We divided the data considering the five-stimulus average amplitude of short-latency (<80 ms) and long-latency responses (200 ms windows starting 80 ms after the onset of each stimulus of the train). According to our findings, the short-latency responses did not differ significantly among groups (Fig. 7C; Wilcoxon test, p = 0.56). However, when analyzing the long-latency responses, we found that the VPA group presented significantly smaller amplitudes than the control group (Fig. 7D; Wilcoxon test, p = 0.04). As for S1 data, we identified neurons with facilitated or depressed responses by calculating the linear growth (or decay) rate of their response amplitudes to the progression of the somatosensory stimulation train. Striatal neurons in both groups presented similar growth and decay rates (Fig. 7E; growth: Wilcoxon test, p = 0.68; decay: Wilcoxon test, p = 0.87), with similar prevalences in their populations (Fig. 7F). We also calculated the amplitude of the responses to each stimulus of the train for the facilitating and depressing classified units. Similar to our findings in S1, we identified clear facilitating and depressing dynamics in both groups, as indicated by the significant differences between the amplitude of the first stimulus of the train and the following ones (Fig. 7G, top asterisk and lines; facilitating units K–W, X2 = 38.39, DF = 9; p < 0.001; inhibiting units, K–W, X2 = 27.44, DF = 9; p < 0.01). However, when we analyzed potential differences between the groups, we found that for the facilitating neurons, all train stimuli in control animals were significantly higher than those in the VPA group (Fig. 7G). On the other hand, for the depressing neurons, the VPA animals displayed significantly lower amplitudes than the control group for all stimuli of the train (Fig. 7G). These results indicate that VPA animals presented diminished responses to somatosensory stimulation.
We next applied our pattern classification method and found that, in the DLSt, the projection with the significantly highest silhouette values corresponded to six clusters (Fig. 7H,I; K–W, X2 = 3,159.2, DF = 5; p < 0.001) with clearly different shapes in response to somatosensory stimulation. These clusters were identified in the three experimental groups (Fig. 7J). When analyzing the pervasiveness of each pattern (K–W, X2 = 27.94, DF = 11; p = 0.003), we found that patterns 3 and 4 were significantly under- and over-represented in the VPA animals, respectively (Fig. 7K). On the other hand, patterns 1 and 2 were under-represented and pattern 3 was over-represented in PV + Cre-Casp animals when compared with the control group (Fig. 7K). Pattern type 4, overexpressed in VPA animals, consisted of sharp, short-latency responses that increased with each stimulus of the train, which would explain the short-latency facilitation observed in the average population responses (Fig. 7A, bottom panel). Next, we analyzed the population dynamics with PCA and discovered that, in control conditions, contrary to what was observed in S1, DLSt presented more discrete oscillatory dynamics in PC1 and PC2, with PC3 almost completely embedded in the surrogate data (Fig. 7L–O). In contrast, VPA striatal dynamics had wide oscillatory components in PC1 and PC2, which was reflected in significantly faster speeds (Fig. 7P; K–W, X2 = 340.3, DF = 5, p < 0.001) and wider amplitudes (Fig. 7Q; K–W, X2 = 543.5, DF = 5, p < 0.001) when compared with the control group.
Our electrophysiological observations indicate that striatal activity in the VPA animals presented slightly higher baseline firing frequencies than those of control animals, accompanied by evoked responses with lower amplitudes in the increased activity component. This resulted in average population dynamics dominated by strong oscillatory pauses triggered by each stimulus of the train. But an important question is: what relationship is there between striatal and cortical dynamics? While our cortical and striatal recordings were not performed simultaneously, they were performed in the same session and animals, allowing us to calculate corticostriatal correlations of sensory-evoked data. We compared the amplitude of the short-latency increase (Fig. 8, left column) and decrease activity (Fig. 8, middle column) and the amplitude of the long-latency increase component (Fig. 8, right column) of the sensory-evoked responses in the control (Fig. 8, top row) and VPA animals (Fig. 8, bottom row). In control animals, the short-latency decrease and long-latency rebound presented significant positive correlations between the S1 and DLSt. This relationship was maintained in VPA animals. However, contrary to what happened in the control group, the amplitude of the short-latency increase in VPA animals exhibited a significant negative correlation. These results suggest that, in addition to the intrastructural changes, corticostriatal communications may also be altered in VPA animals.
On the other hand, our anatomical data suggest that striatal dynamics may be affected by the changes in interneuron proportions or functionality (Figs. 2, 3). To further scrutinize in the mechanism of these general population changes, we aimed to classify striatal neurons based on their spike shapes. However, studies using immunoreactivity methods have reported that PV + neuron hypoactivity may be related to a decrease in PV protein cell levels (Filice et al., 2016; Donato et al., 2013). Hence, we complemented our study by also investigating whether PV-specific depletion could have any electrophysiological effects in this region. To do so, we used a genetically targeted lesioning approach with a new group of animals (n = 4). We injected the adeno-associated virus pAAV-flex-taCasp3-TEVp into the DLSt to induce Caspase 3 expression and apoptosis in PV+ neurons from transgenic rats, where Cre-recombinase is expressed under the PV promoter (PV + Cre-Casp; Fig. 9A). Then, we applied the same PCA/silhouette-based method of classification used to identify sensory-evoked response patterns, but in this case, we classified spike-wave shapes and estimated the absolute proportion of putative MSNs, putative fast-spiking interneurons, and others (Peña-Rangel et al., 2021). By applying this method, we found that in this striatal dataset from the three experimental groups, spike shapes could be grouped into a three-cluster projection (Fig. 9B–D; K–W, X2 = 859.8, DF = 3, p < 0.001). Most cells recorded corresponded to a wider shape and the lowest firing rates, consistent with MSNs (Fig. 9D,E, control 55.5%; VPA 61.5%; PV + Cre-Casp 52.9%; K–W X2 = 37.26, DF = 8, p < 0.001). However, this type of neurons presented significantly higher firing frequencies in VPA and PV + Cre-Casp animals than in control animals (Fig. 9E; K–W, X2 = 21.16, DF = 8, p = 0.006). We did not find significant differences in the proportion or baseline firing rates of the type 2 units. However, type 3 units, whose firing rates and spike-wave shapes are consistent with fast-spiking interneurons, presented significantly lower prevalence (Fig. 9E) and significantly lower firing rates (Fig. 9F) for PV + Cre-Casp animals.
The previous results suggest that even when the histological data show a decrease in the PV+ and ChAT+ signals (Figs. 2, 3), the effects observed in the striatal population dynamics of VPA animals are not explained by the specific loss of PV+ interneurons. Instead, they are most likely explained by a general striatal imbalance. But would these altered neural dynamics explain the behavioral effects observed in VPA subjects? To answer this question, we tried to capitalize on the fact that most VPA-treated animals (n = 10) and some control animals (n = 5) used for behavioral confirmation of the VPA model were also recorded under anesthetized conditions after behavioral assessments. Here, for each animal, we intended to correlate the number of neurons classified as putative MSNs (type 1), others (type 2), or putative fast-spiking interneurons (type 3) with their corresponding behavioral values (total time of interaction with the stranger, Fig. 1A; accumulated time fixed on the light, Fig. 1C; and time spent on self-grooming, Fig. 1B). We found that VPA animals presented a robust and significant negative correlation between the total number of neurons classified as putative MSNs and the grooming time in the open field; that is, the more putative the MSNs, the less time spent on grooming. On the other hand, in the control group, neurons classified as “others” presented a robust positive correlation with fixation time in the hole-board test, and this correlation was lost in the VPA animals (Fig. 10B). Finally, neurons classified as putative fast-spiking interneurons displayed no significant correlations in either group. While these experiments are substantially limited by the lack of simultaneous electrophysiological and behavioral observations, it suggests that the general imbalance in neural dynamics identified in the NSt may partially explain some of the behavioral features observed in the VPA model.
Discussion
In the present work, we used animals prenatally exposed to VPA as an ASD model. Consistent with previous reports, VPA animals showed deficits in social behavior, difficulty in reorienting their attention to a particular sensory stimulus, and increased stereotyped behavior, indicating a reliable recapitulation of the ASD model. These behavioral alterations were accompanied by a decrease in both ChAT+ and PV+ signals in the NSt, with a more significant reduction observed at the DLSt level. A limitation of this observation is that, while a decrease in ChAT+ and PV+ signals could be interpreted as a decrease in total number of neurons, it is possible that these reduced signals could make it more difficult to identify interneurons without a real decrease in the total number of units. Nevertheless, a decrease in these signals together with a consistent behavioral outcome suggest that information processing in the DLSt may be altered in VPA animals. To functionally study this possibility, we performed high-density electrophysiological recordings in anesthetized animals. In the control animals, S1 cortical population dynamics presented clear rotational trajectories (Fig. 6L–O), while striatal dynamics presented smaller amplitudes in the rotational component (Fig. 7L–O). Being a primary somatosensory region, the high amplitude in the rotational trajectories observed in S1 is explained by the robustness in representing each stimulus of the train, with virtually all neurons expressing one of the four oscillatory response patterns depicted in Figure 6J,K. On the other hand, the lower amplitude in DLS is explained by the fact that in this region an important percentage on neurons (∼30%) did not express a clear response to the somatosensory stimulation (Pattern type 6; Fig. 7J,K). When comparing control and VPA-exposed animals, we found that the later ones presented altered neuronal dynamics in both the S1 and DLSt in response to the cutaneous somatosensory stimulation, with the DLSt area having a more exacerbated response, resembling the cortical rotational population trajectories (Figs. 6L–O, 7L–O). To determine whether these differences were explained by alterations in specific interneuron subpopulations, we performed targeted elimination of striatal PV+ neurons and observed that these animals also exhibited strong alterations in sensory-evoked neural dynamics but only partially recapitulated the VPA effects, indicating a higher complexity in the VPA phenotype. Furthermore, altered neural dynamics characterized by fastest and wider population trajectories were observed in the DLSt of VPA animals. Interestingly, these dynamics were similar to those observed in the S1 of control animals, suggesting the loss of a hypothetical filtering function in the DLSt of VPA animals. In this study, we focused the histological quantifications on interneuron populations. This could be a limitation, considering that MSNs are the most prevalent cell type in the DLSt. However, our electrophysiological quantifications included all possible cells recorded. Moreover, when subdividing the populations based on the spike waveform analysis, we found that in VPA and PV + Cre-Casp animals, units with spike shapes that were consistent with MSNs significantly increased their firing rates compared with control animals. This finding indicates that the VPA model affects the general dynamic of the NSt. A controversial observation is that, in the VPA animals, we did not find changes in the gamma frequency or in any other oscillatory frequency. However, this could be explained by the fact that we did not measure gamma directly in the local field potential but in the oscillatory component of the autocorrelogram of individual neurons. On the other hand, we did not study the potential effects of this model in striatal non-neural populations, but other studies have researched these effects in other brain regions using the VPA exposure model and have shown that the model induces alterations in the glial system. These alterations can lead to various developmental, anatomical, and physiological effects and have been attributed to disturbances in various glial elements, such as S100b, GABAA-ρ3, and glial fibrillary acidic protein in astrocytes, Olig2 in oligodendrocytes, and CD11B and Iba1 in microglia (Codagnone et al., 2015; Bronzuoli et al., 2018; Gąssowska-Dobrowolska et al., 2020; Traetta et al., 2021; Saavedra-Bonilla et al., 2024). These findings suggest that this model may also affect the striatal glial system, but more research is needed to confirm this hypothesis.
ASD model
As demonstrated in previous studies validating different models of ASD (Rodier et al., 1997; Schneider and Przewłocki, 2005; K. C. Kim et al., 2011; Schiavi et al., 2019; Elnahas et al., 2020), we observed an important reduction in the social behavior of VPA animals, which displayed fewer contacts and interactions with a novel rat (K. C. Kim et al., 2011; Foley et al., 2014). Furthermore, animals also exhibited aberrantly long periods of attention engagement to a visual stimulus (Fig. 1C). These behaviors are typically observed in ASD patients, with reduced social interaction and communication, resulting in poor social performance in their environment (Franchini et al., 2017; Supekar et al., 2018). Likewise, these patients also display hyper- or hypo-responses to sensory stimuli (Fontes-Dutra et al., 2018; Siemann et al., 2020), as well as low attentional levels to the general environment while being hyper-engaged to a specific stimulus (Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014). Finally, VPA animals also displayed an increase in stereotypical behaviors, as exemplified by the exacerbated frequency and duration of grooming (Hartgraves and Randall, 1986; Wu et al., 2019; Li et al., 2024). This finding is consistent with previous reports (Langen et al., 2014; J. W. Kim et al., 2017; Hirsch et al., 2018). Altogether, our model successfully mimics important features of autistic behavior.
Decrease in ChAT+ and PV+ cells in the NSt
Although interneurons are a small percentage of cells in the NSt (∼5%), they play an important role in the processing of different synaptic inputs reaching the NSt (Tepper and Bolam, 2004; Tepper et al., 2010, 2018) and in the control of synaptic output and the inhibition–excitation balance of neuronal circuits. In this sense, ChAT+ and PV+ cells are two central elements for the proper functioning of the NSt. ChAT+ interneurons provide the main source of acetylcholine (Ach) to the NSt (Zhou et al., 2002; Shen et al., 2005; Abudukeyoumu et al., 2019), while PV+ interneurons control NSt synaptic output by inhibiting MSNs (Tepper et al., 2018).
Diverse studies show that a deregulation in the cholinergic system can generate differences in social and cognitive behavior, increasing stereotypic patterns in patients with ASD and Tourette syndrome, as well as in animal models (Kataoka et al., 2010; Oginsky et al., 2014; Rapanelli et al., 2017b). In this sense, our results showed a significant reduction of ChAT+ levels in the NSt of VPA animals (Fig. 2). This reduction suggests a weak cholinergic modulation in the NSt, which would partly explain the increased grooming and social deficits in our VPA animals (Cubo et al., 2008; Karvat and Kimchi, 2014). Interestingly, the spatial configuration of the ChAT+ distribution was altered along the NSt, with the DLSt being the most affected, losing the rostral-caudal and medial-lateral gradient observed in control animals (Fig. 2; Meredith et al., 1989; Matamales et al., 2016a). Similar to ChAT levels, PV levels were significantly reduced (Fig. 3). These results are in agreement with previous reports using genetic and pharmacological models of ASD (Peñagarikano et al., 2011; Filice et al., 2016; Lauber et al., 2016). Furthermore, Orduz and colleagues (Orduz et al., 2013) found that low PV expression led to electrophysiological alterations in fast-spiking neurons and, therefore, to a deficit in the modulation of afferents reaching the NSt, particularly the MSN (Monterio et al., 2018). Likewise, Rapanelli and collaborators (2017) showed that a joint depletion of ChAT+ and PV+ cells in the NSt emulates the main characteristics of ASD. However, previous studies have reported that PV+ neuron hypoactivity may be related to a decrease in PV protein expression and may be related with ASD (Filice et al., 2016; Donato et al., 2013; Contractor et al., 2021). In our data, the decreased levels in PV+ (Fig. 3) were not accompanied by a decrease in the prevalence of neurons with sharp spike waves shapes, typically associated to FSI PV+ interneurons (Fig. 9), opening the possibility that our data could be better explained by a decreased in the levels of PV protein and not an absolute decrease in the number pf PV+ neurons. This possibility was explored by experimentally depleting PV+ neurons in a transgenic rat model (Fig. 9). This manipulation induced decrease in both, the levels of PV+ neurons and the prevalence of neurons expressing sharp spike waves. This data suggests that in the VPA model an imbalance in the basal ganglia interneuron functionality is related to atypical somatosensory processing. In this context, striatal interneurons exert considerable control over MSNs and help in the processing of cortical and thalamic afferents and regulation of their synaptic output (Tepper and Bolam, 2004; Tepper et al., 2010, 2018; Marín, 2012; Kepecs and Fishell, 2014; Poppi et al., 2021). Our data show a heterogeneous decrease in ChAT+ and PV levels in the NSt of VPA animals. This reduction was accompanied by changes in the cortical and striatal network dynamics. Notably, in the NSt, VPA animals expressed a modest increase in baseline firing rates (Fig. 4D) that appeared to be associated with the activity of putative MSNs (Fig. 9) and, most importantly, different somatosensory-evoked responses to each of the sensory stimuli of the train (Fig. 7). When compared with the control animals, VPA animals presented different adaptation profiles, with lower amplitudes in the “rebound” long-latency increase component of the response. These changes produced a net imbalance in the responses, which were dominated by stronger pauses in activity, as seen in the average population response (Fig. 7A, bottom panel) and PC1 amplitude (Fig. 7A,Q). A possible explanation for these different responses would implicate an imbalance in ChAT+ and PV levels within the NSt, affecting their ability to adequately filter and process synaptic commands from the S1. It is important to highlight that the selective ablation of PV+ cells did not fully recapitulate the VPA phenotype. This finding indicates that the VPA model does not affect a particular neural population but causes a generalized imbalance. In this context, our data suggest that the interneuron imbalance would impact on sensory-evoked network activity in the dorsolateral striatum. Since striatal interneurons will not project outside the striatum, the behavioral outcome associated to these effects must implicate the main striatal neurons, MSNs, and their canonical connectivity within the BG (i.e., direct and indirect pathways). Here, our correlation analysis between the proportion of subpopulation of striatal neurons and behavioral parameters (Fig. 10) suggests a relationship between the number of putative MSNs (Type 1) and grooming behavior. However, while in our dataset we did not evaluate the levels of MSNs, diminishing the scope of our interpretations, these data confirm that linking neural dynamics and behavioral outcomes associated to the VPA model would require a wider approach where all the elements of striatal microcircuit are taken into consideration. To understand this imbalance, further research is needed, as well as experimental designs specifically targeting different elements of the striatal and cortical circuits. These data are in agreement with other studies showing that developmental differences or interneurons would lead to a series of electrophysiological and behavioral differences in sensory processing (Rapanelli et al., 2017b; Filice et al., 2020; Q. Chen et al., 2020).
The decrease in ChAT+ cells within the NSt would lead to a decrease in ACh concentration in VPA animals, which is consistent with the study by Karvat and Kimchi (2014), where the authors used an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor directly infused in the NSt and demonstrated cognitive and social interaction improvements in a mouse model of ASD. According to previous literature, a decrease in striatal ACh levels would lead to impaired modulation of corticostriatal afferents through type 2 muscarinic acetylcholine receptors that negatively control cortical glutamate release to the NSt (Calabresi et al., 2000). Likewise, feedback and feedforward inhibition would also be affected, since these processes have been linked to the activation of nicotinic receptors expressed in the GABAergic interneurons of the NSt (Koós and Tepper, 2002; English et al., 2011; Faust et al., 2016; Assous et al., 2017). Furthermore, it has recently been proposed that cholinergic interneurons (CINs) can indirectly modulate cortical MSN communication through PV+ interneurons (Matityahu et al., 2022). This effect that can be directly altered by CIN distribution across the entire NSt (Fig. 2; Dorst et al., 2020). Moreover, our data also indicate that CIN modulation directly over MSNs would also be affected, since MSNs express muscarinic receptors at pre- and postsynaptic levels (Goldberg et al., 2012; Assous, 2021). While further experiments would be necessary to causally link a decrease in ChAT+ cells (and therefore in Ach) within the NSt with the atypical sensory-evoked responses observed in our data, the previous literature will favor this interpretation. Additionally, the observed decrease in PV+ cells would reinforce the higher baseline frequencies and different sensory-evoked responses at the DLSt level in VPA animals due to a deficit in the inhibitory elements of the striatal circuit (Marín, 2012; Tepper et al., 2010, 2018). A decrease in PV+ interneurons may also impact on general striatal processing, especially in the DLSt, since it has been proposed that these interneurons are organized and communicate through gap junctions (Koós and Tepper, 1999). Altogether, our results may help to understand the exacerbated behaviors that ASD patients express in response to sensory stimulation, which can vary in intensity and frequency and that on many occasions may even generate crises (Buckley and Holmes, 2016).
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
We thank Alma E. Aguilar and Claudia I. Pérez Díaz for their technical assistance. This research was supported in part by FAI Grants: C14-FAI-04-02-02; C15-FAI-04-47.4, C16-FAI-09-31.31 (O.I.-S.), C20-FAI-10-54.54FAI 2020 (N.S.); UNAM-PAPIIT, IN200822 and IG200424; CONAHCyT, CF-2023-I-7 (P.E.R.-O.); and CONAHCYT-Mexico doctoral fellowships (D.N.I.-S.; CVU 627069).
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.