Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

Bilateral Retinofugal Pathfinding Impairments Limit Behavioral Compensation in Near-Congenital One-Eyed Xenopus laevis

Michael Forsthofer, Clayton Gordy, Meghna Kolluri and Hans Straka
eNeuro 20 December 2023, 11 (1) ENEURO.0371-23.2023; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0371-23.2023
Michael Forsthofer
1Faculty of Biology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
2Graduate School of Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Clayton Gordy
1Faculty of Biology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
2Graduate School of Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Meghna Kolluri
1Faculty of Biology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hans Straka
1Faculty of Biology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hans Straka
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

To generate a coherent visual percept, information from both eyes must be appropriately transmitted into the brain, where binocular integration forms the substrate for visuomotor behaviors. To establish the anatomical substrate for binocular integration, the presence of bilateral eyes and interaction of both optic nerves during retinotectal development play a key role. However, the extent to which embryonic monocularly derived visual circuits can convey visuomotor behaviors is unknown. In this study, we assessed the retinotectal anatomy and visuomotor performance of embryonically generated one-eyed tadpoles. In one-eyed animals, the axons of retinal ganglion cells from the singular remaining eye exhibited striking irregularities in their central projections in the brain, generating a noncanonical ipsilateral retinotectal projection. This data is indicative of impaired pathfinding abilities. We further show that these novel projections are correlated with an impairment of behavioral compensation for the loss of one eye.

  • eye extirpation
  • frog
  • OKR
  • optokinetic reflex
  • plasticity
  • retinotectal

Significance Statement

This study suggests that embryonic sensory imbalance has more far-reaching effects on function compared with acute loss in already established circuits. More generally, bilaterality of sensory organs is the norm across all vertebrates as well as many invertebrates, and our results contribute to a general understanding of how developmental programs, as well as behaviors reliant on them, pivotally depend on the presence of sensory inputs from both body sides.

Introduction

Integration of bilateral sensory information is crucial for detailed and environmentally relevant processing of the world. Such processing aids in the extraction of behaviorally important sensory features. In the visual system for example, vision with two eyes, or stereopsis, contributes to distance estimation (Poggio and Poggio, 1984) and motion detection (Van Hof-van Duin and Mohn, 1986). In order to accomplish this, anatomical pathways conveying separate information from the left and right eye must be integrated in the central nervous system to form a coherent visual percept (McLaughlin and O'Leary, 2005).

In some animals, such as mice, this combination of bilateral visual information happens at the level of retinal ganglion cell (RGC) projections: axons project into the brain either ipsi- or contralaterally depending on their topographical origin in the retina. In other animals, such as zebrafish or larval Xenopus tadpoles, all axons cross to the contralateral side (for review, see Petros et al., 2008). In this latter case, binocular information must still converge, which occurs anatomically via interhemispheric commissures, to form a binocular visual percept and facilitate binocular-dependent behaviors (Keating and Feldman, 1975; Grant and Keating, 1989; Gambrill et al., 2018; Gebhardt et al., 2019).

Following loss or obstruction of one eye in mature organisms, these established circuits display an impressive capacity to compensate for loss of input, on both a functional and behavioral level. This even includes recovery of visual associated behaviors to certain extents (Dews and Wiesel, 1970; Prusky et al., 2006; Steeves et al., 2008; Rose et al., 2016). In frogs, for example, acute monocular deprivation due to eyelid suture normally leads to direction-selective impairment of the optokinetic reflex (OKR). However, within 8 d this impairment is remedied, and the OKR responds equally to all directions even under monocular stimulation conditions (Jardon and Bonaventure, 1992a). To enable this plasticity in mature stages, circuits must be correctly assembled during embryonic development (Huang et al., 2006). Such canonical assembly occurs with contribution from both eyes, with genetic and molecular factors playing a key role for pathfinding of bilateral RGCs, and neural activity serving to fine-tune morphological arborizations and other aspects of neuronal connectivity (Holt, 1989; Oster et al., 2004; Hua et al., 2005; Niell and Smith, 2005). Genetically induced deviations in chiasmatic crossing have a direct impact on visual behaviors, such as the complete ipsilateral RGC projections in the zebrafish belladonna mutant that cause a left–right inversion of eye-following motion in the OKR (Karlstrom et al., 1996; Huang et al., 2006).

A large body of research has been conducted on the genetic basis of the establishment of retinotectal connections and their specific functions. Less is known about the role of molecular or functional interactions of bilateral visual pathways during embryogenesis, and how these contribute to establishment of visual circuits and their functional output. In animals where each eye possesses native RGC projections to both the ipsi- and contralateral brain hemispheres, such as ferrets and mice, early embryonic ablation of an optic vesicle and subsequent one-eyed embryonic development results in reduced ipsilateral projections from the remaining eye (Taylor and Guillery, 1995; Chan et al., 1999). This suggests that binocular RGC axons are involved in the chiasmatic-crossing decision, specifically in contributing to the ipsilateral projection of the contralateral eye.

In animals with exclusively contralateral RGC projections, similar experiments yielded less unanimous results. In chick embryos, removal of one optic vesicle induces an ipsilateral RGC projection from the remaining eye. However, these are lost within few days of development before any visual behavior in this species and do not allow conclusions about anatomical or functional impact on postembryonic behavior (Thanos et al., 1984). Unilateral embryonic eye removals in Xenopus on the other hand show no sign of an aberrant ipsilateral projection following metamorphosis (Taylor and Gaze, 1990). However, these optic vesicle removals were performed after onset of RGC differentiation, and mammalian studies showed a large variability in phenotypes depending on timing of optic vesicle removal (Chan and Guillery, 1993). Additionally, neither study assessed function of circuitry formed under embryonic monocular conditions, leaving a potential beneficial or detrimental role of embryonic plasticity open. While a detrimental effect poses the more likely outcome based on previous genetic studies on pathfinding errors (Huang et al., 2006), the complete absence of visual input to half of the circuit, rather than miswiring to both, may allow for stronger functional connections of the remaining eye and to functional plasticity (Straznicky et al., 1980). Indeed, Xenopus embryos show a large degree of plasticity during embryonic development, allowing for anatomical integration of, for example, a third eye into midbrain circuits (Constantine-Paton and Law, 1978). More recent studies reveal that supernumerary or displaced eyes and ears can be anatomically and functionally connected to their central targets and facilitate behavior to certain extents and under certain conditions (Cline, 1991; Blackiston and Levin, 2013; Elliott and Fritzsch, 2018; Gordy et al., 2018).

In this study, we revisit embryonic visual circuit formation in one-eyed Xenopus laevis, to answer the question whether visual circuits can develop appropriately in the absence of one eye during embryogenesis and whether this developmental plasticity conveys a compensatory or detrimental effect on visuomotor behavior. In contrast to previous studies in this species, we performed embryonic enucleations before differentiation of RGCs (Holt, 1989). Following development into late larval stages, we expect exclusively contralateral RGC projections in two-eyed animals. In monocular animals, we unexpectedly found an ipsilateral RGC projection. We then quantified eye motion elicited by the OKR, to assess the extent of functional plasticity of this monocular visual circuit. The results reveal that embryonically generated one-eyed Xenopus tadpoles with aberrant RGC projections exhibit more impaired visuomotor behaviors, highlighting a critical limitation of visuomotor plasticity in retinotectal development.

Material and Methods

Animals

Experiments were conducted on wild-type X. laevis embryos and tadpoles obtained from the animal breeding facility of the Biomedical Center at the Ludwig-Maximilians-University (LMU) Munich. Animals at early embryonic stages (stages 4–16) had their jelly coat removed with 2% cysteine and were incubated in 0.1× Marc's modified Ringer’s solution [MMR, diluted from a 10× stock solution (in mM): 1 NaCl, 18 KCl, 20 CaCl2, 10 MgCl2, 150 HEPES, pH 7.6–7.8] at 17°C under a 12/12 h dark/light cycle regime. Animals were then reared until stage 46 either as control group or with prior surgical removal of the embryonic optic vesicle at stages 26–28 (described below). At stage 46, a total of 83 manipulated animals were screened for successful removal of one eye (n = 22) and either killed with 3-aminobenzoic acid ethyl ester methanesulfonate (MS-222; Parmaq) and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) for immunohistochemistry or were transferred into de-chlorinated water and further reared in standing tanks at 17–19°C at the animal facility of the Biocenter of the LMU Munich. Animals were kept on a 12/12 h dark/light cycle, fed daily with powdered Spirulina (Algova) suspended in tank water, with a daily 50% water exchange. At stages 53–54, behavioral and anatomical experiments (described below) were performed on the animals in accordance with the “Principles of animal care” publication no. 86–23, revised 1985, of the National Institutes of Health and in accordance with the ARRIVE guidelines and regulations. Permission for the experiments was granted by the Government of Upper Bavaria (Regierung von Oberbayern) under the license codes ROB-55.2.2532.Vet_03-17-24, ROB-55.2.2532.Vet_02-19-146 and ROB-55.2.2532.Vet_02-22-54. All experiments were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations of the Biocenter of the LMU Munich.

Optic vesicle extirpation

Extirpation of the optic vesicle on the left side was performed at developmental stages 26–28 (Fig. 1a, Movie 1) with tungsten needles (0.125 mm, Fine Science Tools, 10130-05). Embryos were transferred into 1× MMR at 22°C and anaesthetized with 0.02% benzocaine (Sigma-Aldrich, E1501; Gordy and Straka, 2022) prior to the surgical manipulations. The optic vesicle was visually identified, the overlaying ectoderm layer removed, and the optic vesicle excised along with the underlying optic stalk. Following this manipulation, animals were left in 1× MMR for 30 min to promote healing of the excision site and subsequently returned to 0.1× MMR for further rearing until stage 46. At this stage, animals were anaesthetized in 0.02% benzocaine for visual inspection and sorted for successful eye removals under a stereo microscope (SteREO Discovery.V20, Axiocam 305 color camera, Carl Zeiss Microscopy). Successfully extirpated, one-eyed, tadpoles were then transferred to the animal facility for rearing until stages 53–54, when eye motion recordings were executed. Similar removals of sensory organs have been performed successfully, with no further detrimental effect on development (Blackiston and Levin, 2013; Gordy et al., 2018; Gordy and Straka, 2022). A representative video (Movie 1) was taken with a color camera (Axiocam 405, Carl Zeiss Microscopy) under a stereo microscope (SteREO Discovery.V20, Carl Zeiss Microscopy) at 30 FPS (ZEN software 3.4.91, Carl Zeiss Microscopy).

Movie 1

Extirpation of the optic vesicle. Representative video of the removal of the left optic vesicle of a Xenopus laevis embryo at developmental Stages 26-28. Download Movie 1, MP4 file.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Methodology of eye removals and behavioral tracking. a, Schematic of the experimental timeline, consisting of embryonic removal of the left optic vesicle at stages 26–28 (left, total of 83 animals, 22 successful) and behavioral tracking in a visual stimulation paradigm following maturation at stages 52–54 (right). Insets depict representative immunohistochemical staining of the eye region in a one-eyed (left) and a control (right) tadpole at stage 46, showing nerves (tubulin, green), muscles (MyoVI, red), and a nuclear counter-stain (DAPI, blue). Arrows indicate the trajectory of the oculomotor nerve. b, Representative images of 1-week-old one-eyed (top) tadpoles in comparison to controls (bottom). c, Representative traces of right eye motion in response to sinusoidal motion stimulation (top, 6,28°/s peak velocity at 0.1 Hz) or constant velocity unidirectional motion (bottom, 6.28°/s) in a single control animal. Eye motion depicted in blue, stimulus motion in black. Scale bars: a, 200 µm; b, 500 µm. II, optic nerve; III, oculomotor nerve; IV, trochlear nerve; V, trigeminal nerve; VII, facial nerve; C, caudal; cg, cement gland; D, dorsal; Le, leftward; M, medial; op, otic placode; R, rostral; Ri, rightward; V, ventral.

Immunohistochemistry

After sorting of phenotypes at stage 46, prototypic one-eyed tadpoles were prepared for the immunohistochemical analysis of nerve and muscle tissue around the site of the extirpated eye for further verification of a successful removal (Fig. 1b). To this end, animals were deeply anesthetized in 0.05% MS-222 and immersion-fixed in 4% PFA in 0.1× phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; in mM, 0.3 Na3PO4, 15 NaCl, 0.105 K3PO4) for 3–6 h at 4°C. Tadpoles were then transferred into 0.1× PBS, and the lower jaws, viscera, tail, dorsal skull cartilage, and brain were removed. The residual tissue was then dehydrated in 70% ethanol for 3–12 h at 36°C, washed 3× in 0.1× PBS, and blocked in 5% normal goat serum in 0.1× PBS with 0.1% Triton X-100 at 22–24°C for 1 h. Incubation with the primary antibodies against muscle (MyosinVI, 1:400, Proteus Biosciences, 25-6791) and neuronal (acetylated tubulin, 1:800, Sigma-Aldrich, T7451) tissue was performed for 14 h at 22–24°C. Subsequently, washing and blocking steps were performed as described above, followed by an incubation in secondary fluorescent antibodies (1:500, Alexa Goat anti-Mouse IgG2b, A-21141, Alexa Goat anti-Rabbit IgG, A32733) and DAPI (Thermo Fisher Scientific; 62248, 1:500) in 0.1× PBS with 0.1% Triton X-100 for 1 h at 22–24°C. Tissue was then washed 6× for 15 min each in 0.1× PBS, mounted on microscope slides with spacers on each side, and coverslipped with Aqua Polymount (PolyScience, 18606). Imaging of the tissue was performed on a Leica SP5-2 confocal microscope (Center for Advanced Light Microscopy).

In vitro preparations

Activation and tracking of eye movements was performed in semi-intact in vitro preparations of either control or one-eyed tadpoles at stages 53–54. The generation of such isolated preparations occurred as described in literature (Forsthofer and Straka, 2022). Animals were deeply anaesthetized in 0.05% MS-222 at 22–24°C for 2–5 min and were then transferred into ice-cold Ringer’s solution (in mM, 75 NaCl, 25 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 2 KCl, 0.1 MgCl2, and 11 glucose, pH 7.4). Following decapitation, the lower jaws, viscera, and skin above the skull were removed, the skull was opened, and the choroid plexus above the fourth ventricle was taken off. Preparations were then allowed to recover in 200 ml Ringer’s solution for ∼2 h at 17°C before commencing with the eye motion recording session (Gordy and Straka, 2022). Unilateral optic nerve transections were done, when performed, directly before behavioral assessments. The optic nerve was located from dorsally through the opened skull and cut proximally to its exit point at the diencephalon with scissors under visual control.

Visual motion (optokinetic) stimulation

Optokinetic stimuli consisted of horizontally moving vertical black and white bars (subtending 16° of visual angle each), projected with three orthogonally oriented digital light processing video projectors (Aiptek V60) onto a cylindrical screen covering 275° of the horizontal visual field of the tadpole. Stimulus motion consisted of either sinusoidal left–right oscillations or unidirectional constant velocity stimulus motion either to the left or to the right. Sinusoidal stimuli were presented at different frequencies (0.1, 0.2, 0.5 Hz; ±10° magnitude), presented in two trials of 15 cycles each, with a 30 s inter-trial interval. Constant velocity visual stimuli consisted of a 30 s stimulus motion at a velocity of 6.25°/s, with 30 s in either direction, with alternating starts to the left or to the right, with a stationary pattern for 30 s between the two motion directions. Stimulus motion was recorded and synchronized with the Spike2 software (v7.04) and a CED 1401 A/D interface (Cambridge Electronic Design) at 50 Hz.

Eye motion tracking

Preparations were recorded from above with a digital CCD camera (Grasshopper Mono, Point Grey Research) equipped with an infrared high-pass filter and a zoom objective (Optem Zoom 70XL, Qioptiq Photonics) using infrared illumination of the preparation. Eye movements were tracked in real-time with a custom-written program (Knorr et al., 2017), measured as deflection angle between the vertical image axis and the long axis of an ellipse fitted around the two eyes which were detected by a manually adjusted darkness threshold. Eye positions were recorded in Spike2 at 30 Hz (Fig. 1c). In both control and one-eyed tadpoles, only data from the right eye was used for analysis.

Retinal ganglion cell projections

RGC projections into the brain of control and one-eyed animals were fluorescently labeled following completion of the eye motion recording protocol with tetramethylrhodamine, conjugated to 3,000 MW dextran (Invitrogen, D3308). The fluorescent dye was dissolved in ddH2O before crystallization onto 0.1 mm minutiae pins. In vitro preparations were mounted right lateral-side up, the cornea was incised, and the lens was removed. Following a transient removal of the Ringer’s solution and insertion of a needle with crystallized dye into the eye cup, the cornea was resealed with superglue (UHU). Thereafter, in vitro preparations were incubated for 14–24 h at 14°C and, following visual assessment of labeling quality and extent, immersion-fixed in 4% PFA at 4°C for 24 h. Subsequently, the brain was extracted, DAPI-stained for 2 h (1:500), and cleared with the uDISCO protocol (Pan et al., 2016), with 2 h per butanol step and clearing in BABB D-15. Cleared brains were mounted using custom-built metal spacers, sealed with Roti Histokitt II (Carl Roth, T160.1), and imaged on a Leica SP5-2 confocal microscope at an optical section spacing of 2.4 µm.

Data analysis

Eye motion tracking

Eye movement data obtained in Spike2 was further processed in Python following established standards (Forsthofer and Straka, 2022). Sinusoidal and unidirectional eye and stimulus motion data was converted into the Matlab (MathWorks) file format, nonuniformally sampled data resampled at 200 Hz and filtered with a 4 Hz low-pass filter. Sinusoidal eye movements were segmented into individual cycles, sorted for cycles that exclusively contained slow-following eye movements, and averaged. The amplitude and phase of the cyclic responses were measured based on magnitude and temporal occurrence of the response peaks, relative to the stimulus cycle. Eye movements evoked by constant velocity motion stimuli were similarly cut into individual episodes for each direction. The initial 0.75 s of each stimulus episode were discarded due to the response latency of motion onset. The following 2 s of each stimulus episode was then screened manually for resetting fast phases. If fast phases occurred prior to 1.5 s after stimulus onset, the episode was discarded; if fast phases occurred after 1.5 s following stimulus onset, eye movements were included until fast-phase onset. Eye motion velocity was calculated as the mean derivative of the eye position and averaged across either leftward or rightward episodes per animal. In addition, an average leftward and rightward position trace was generated per animal for presentation purposes.

Neuronal tracing

Image stacks acquired from cleared brains were loaded into Fiji as 8 bit images (Schindelin et al., 2012) and thresholded at an intensity of 3–4 depending on background fluorescence. Subsequently, a region of interest (ROI) was manually selected at the rostral boundary of the optic tectum across the whole mediolateral extent of the brain and an intensity profile was generated for the ROI in each image of the stack. The ROI was limited to the caudal diencephalon and the rostral midbrain to measure intensity specifically in the approximate area of the pretectum. Intensity profiles were then summed up across the z-stack, normalized, and plotted, to generate a plot showcasing the amount of above-threshold labeled neuronal fibers within the ROI across the entire mediolateral and dorsoventral aspect of the brain. These plots were then split into left and right hemispherical halves, and the integral of each plot was calculated, approximating the total amount of fibers in the right and left (ipsi- and contralateral with respect to the right eye, respectively) rostral tectal region. The calculations served as proxy for the fraction of RGC axons projecting into the ipsi- and contralateral pretectum and optic tectum, respectively.

Statistical analysis

Statistical comparisons between independent (one-eyed animals, control animals, left optic nerve-transected animals) for sinusoidal data were performed with a Kruskal–Wallis test for unpaired nonparametric data, followed by a Dunn's multiple-comparisons test to find group differences in Prism 9 (GraphPad Software). Statistical comparisons between and within animals (leftward/rightward eye movements for different groups) were tested with a two-way ANOVA followed by a Bonferroni multiple-comparisons test for group differences. The ROUT outlier test in Prism (maximum FDR = 0.1%) was performed to identify outliers in all plots, with 1 data point removed from all plots in Figure 4 due to being an outlier in T/N asymmetry at a value of 2.36. To calculate motion direction preference for the left and right eye in control animals, we calculated the preference of rightward/leftward eye movements for either unidirectional or oscillating eye movements as the slope obtained by fitting the eye positions of both eyes plotted against each other, with the r2 indicating the quality of the fit (Python 3.7). Dots that are connected to each other with lines indicate paired data. Circular statistics were performed in Oriana (version 4.02; Kovach Computing Services; Gordy and Straka, 2022). A mean vector was calculated from phase values yielding the mean direction as well as the strength of the vector as an indication of data clustering (scale 0–1). Differences in phase values were identified with a Watson–Williams F test.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup

Data and code availability

All data reported in this paper will be shared by the lead contact on request. All code used for analysis of acquired behavioral data, as well as per-cycle segmented data, is publicly available on GitHub through Zenodo under http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7781042.

Results

Generation of one-eyed tadpoles

One-eyed tadpoles were generated by removal of the left optic vesicle in early embryonic stages, similar to embryonic removal of the otic placode in previous studies (Fig. 1a; stages 26–28; Nieuwkoop and Faber, 1994; Gordy and Straka, 2022).

Removals were validated 1 week post-surgery at stage 46 by visual inspection (Fig. 1b; 26.5% success rate calculated from n = 22, 54 and 7 for monocular, regrowth, and deformed tadpoles) in comparison with unmanipulated controls (Fig. 1b, bottom). To further visualize the observed absence of gross anatomical optic structures, we performed immunohistochemical labeling of cranial nerves (acetylated tubulin, green) and extraocular musculature (myosin VI, red). On the unmanipulated side in these animals (Fig. 1a, right inset), and in unmanipulated controls (Fig. 1a, right inset), the extraocular muscles insert themselves in a stereotyped fashion around the eye and are innervated by their respective extraocular motoneurons (Fig. 1a, right inset), consistent with features for X. laevis (Branoner and Straka, 2018). In embryonic one-eyed animals, however, such an organizational scheme was not observed (Fig. 1a, left inset), given the absence of an eye in this region. Lacking individual eye muscles to innervate, the suspected extraocular motor neurons in the optic area of these animals, identified based on their exit sites from the skull, failed to innervate in canonical fashion (Fig. 1a, insets, arrowheads). This immunohistochemical data validated the absence of eye-associated neural and muscular structures in embryonically manipulated animals (Schönenberger et al., 1983). Monocular animals were then further reared until stages 52–54 (Nieuwkoop and Faber, 1994), where visuomotor behaviors and central retinotectal circuits are readily profiled (Knorr et al., 2017), in order to anatomically and behaviorally assess the effects of embryonic removal of the optic vesicle. In vitro preparations (Fig. 1a, right) of control and one-eyed animals were generated and subsequently subjected to functional assessment paradigms which have been shown to induce quantifiable optokinetic eye movements (Fig. 1c; Soupiadou et al., 2020).

OKR performance in bidirectional stimulation

In control animals with two bilateral eyes (hereafter, “binocular”), visuomotor behaviors were profiled following visual stimulation. For this, we presented a sinusoidal horizontally moving black–white striped pattern, which readily induced reflexive eye movements that followed the motion cyclically (Figs. 1a,c, 2a). In binocularly stimulated controls, eye motions were conjugated between both eyes (Fig. 2a,b; slope = 0.92 ± 0.01, mean ± SD) and showed a gain of 0.28 ± 0.11 (Fig. 2a,b, blue, eye motion amplitude/stimulus motion amplitude) in line with previous studies in this species (Gordy and Straka, 2022). To better understand the individual contributions of each eye to these bilateral control visuomotor behaviors, we next investigated OKR performance following acute loss of visual input on one side. This was induced by selective lesioning of the left optic nerve (hereafter, “Lesioned”) close to the brain, similarly to lesions of the statoacoustic nerve in such preparations (Soupiadou et al., 2020), which induced an acute monocular condition. Such a condition allowed us to assess the extent to which mature and entrained visuomotor circuits are affected by sudden loss of input from one side. In lesioned animals, the OKR gain decreased to 0.18 ± 0.05 (Fig. 2a,b, green; p = 0.041a) indicating an impairment after acute unilateral eye loss. Coverage of the left visual field instead of surgical intervention yielded the same effect of a decrease of the OKR gain from 0.34 ± 0.06 to 0.21 ± 0.04 (data not visualized; p = 0.015b). As impairments from surgical lesions and restrictions of the visual field yielded similar results, the OKR impairment likely originated from visual signal loss rather than detrimental effects of the surgical procedure itself.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Responses of the right eye to sinusoidal motion stimulation. a, Eye motion traces (colored lines) of the right eye of binocular (n = 12), left eye lesioned (n = 12), and monocular tadpoles (n = 12) in response to sinusoidal stimulus motion (dotted lines). Thin lines represent average responses per animal (8–30 cycles per animal); thick lines in plots and in the inset represent the average response across animals. Schematics depict stimulation paradigm. b, Gain values (eye motion amplitude/stimulus amplitude) for the binocular, lesioned, and monocular paradigms. c, Average phase values of the eye following motion respective stimulus motion in degrees and seconds. Data points represent animals, with standard deviation bars outside the circle. 0° represents the peak at the leftward to rightward direction change, 180° the reverse. d, Top, Mean vectors (length, vector strength) of the data plotted in c; bottom, timing values are given in relation to the stimulus motion peak corresponding to the leftward or rightward eye motion peak. Data re-used from panel c. L, left to right peak; R, right to left peak.

With reference values for OKR performance in binocular and left eye lesioned tadpoles, we next examined whether embryonically one-eyed tadpoles (Fig. 1a,b, hereafter, “monocular”) were able to compensate for impairment of the OKR in monocular stimulation conditions. These tadpoles displayed eye movements in response to sinusoidal stimuli at a gain of 0.22 ± 0.09, which placed them in between binocular and lesioned tadpole performances, with no significant difference to either group (Fig. 2a,b, magenta; p = 0.36 and p > 0.99, respectivelya). Accordingly, the OKR in these animals was not strongly impaired during exclusive right eye stimulation and suggests a degree of compensation for the lack of one eye.

In acute optic nerve lesions, a change in the timing of the OKR response peak from a mostly in-phase response for both leftward and rightward motion (Fig. 2c,d, blue; 3.8 ± 8.0°, 1.3° ± 11.7°, respectively) to a phase lead in the leftward (Fig. 2c,d, upper half, green; −11.0 ± 11.8°, p = 0.002c) but not the rightward direction (Fig. 2c,d, lower half; −7.9 ± 10.8°, p = 0.1c; Fig. 2c, bottom, d) indicate that not just response strength but also timing of OKR is impaired upon acute loss of one eye. Response latencies of embryonic one-eyed animals were clearly improved from lesioned controls in both leftward and rightward direction (Fig. 2d, magenta; −0.97 ± 5.3°, p = 0.017; 5 ± 9.5°, p = 0.012c) and were similar to binocular animals (p = 0.117; p = 0.103c).

Overall, sinusoidal visuomotor stimulation reveals that tadpole OKR requires binocular visual input to maintain strength and timing of the response, and loss of one eye impairs both factors. Tadpoles which only developed with one eye, however, were able to partially compensate for these impairments.

Direction-specific impairment of OKR

As our manipulation induced an asymmetry on bilateral peripheral sensory detection, we next investigated if the impairment also affected the OKR in a direction-specific and therefore asymmetric manner. In many lateral eyed animals, each eye in isolation preferentially responds to motion in the nasal (N) direction (Masseck and Hoffmann, 2009). We therefore tested whether the amplitude impairment observed in sinusoidal stimulation was caused by a reduction of the temporal (T) motion component, in other words, a rightward movement in case of the singular right eye. To assess this, we measured eye velocity in the first 2 s during unidirectional motion stimulation at a constant velocity. In binocular control tadpoles, during binocular stimulation, the right eye responded more strongly to temporal motion at a gain of 0.39 ± 0.12 (eye velocity/stimulus velocity) versus 0.24 ± 0.07 for nasal motion (Fig. 4a,b, blue; p < 0.001d). Therefore, under binocular viewing conditions, the right eye responds well in both motion directions with a preference for temporal motion. This temporal preference was present in both eyes, as confirmed by comparative conjugation assessment (Fig. 3a) and only in unidirectional but not sinusoidal stimulation (Fig. 3b). This indicates a control condition whereby the OKR exhibits an asymmetry which favors temporal motion detection.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Left–right asymmetries in binocular and monocular tadpoles. a, Conjugations of the left and right eye for leftward (blue) and rightward (red) unidirectional motion and for sinusoidal motion (black) in control animals (n = 12). A slope of 1 represents conjugate eye motion, >1 stronger motion of the left, and <1 of the right eye. Slope of leftward and rightward motion are 1.12 ± 0.005 and 0.69 ± 0.005, respectively, while sinusoidal conjugation is intermediate at 0.92 ± 0.06. b, Conjugations of the left and right eye for leftward (blue) and rightward (red) components of sinusoidal motion. Slope of leftward and rightward motion are 0.95 ± 0.01 and 0.93 ± 0.01, respectively. c, Plot taken from b on nasal and temporal gains during unidirectional stimuli of binocular and lesioned tadpoles. Added are gray dots which represent a summation of corresponding nasal and temporal values of lesioned animals (mean ± SD, 0.31 ± 0.08; p = 0.08; two-way ANOVA; Bonferroni's multiple comparison). d, Plot from c, with added gray dots which represent symmetry values of the lesioned nasal component, with the temporal component substituted with summed nasal and temporal gains calculated in panel (mean ± SD, 1.64 ± 0.54; p > 0.99; Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn's multiple comparison). e, Correlation of nasal (red) and temporal (light blue) gains of embryonic monocular tadpoles (same individuals from Fig. 5g) with the amount of ipsilateral fraction of RGC projections. Nasal, r2 = 0.002; fit, y = −0.04y + 0.2. Temporal, r2 = 0.67; fit, y = −0.87y + 0.23.

Acute optic nerve transection of one eye selectively impaired OKR of the remaining eye in the temporal direction. Under such acute monocular viewing conditions, gain of the right eye in the temporal direction was reduced to 0.10 ± 0.05 and therefore lower than in binocular tadpoles (Fig. 4b, green; p < 0.001d), while nasal motion was unaffected at a gain of 0.19 ± 0.05. Therefore, under acute monocular conditions, the remaining eye shows a direction preference for nasal stimulus motion (Fig. 4b, green, p < 0.001d; Fig. 4c, 0.48 ± 0.2°, p < 0.001f), in line with other lateral-eyed animals (Masseck and Hoffmann, 2009). We hypothesize that this is due to a cooperative processing of temporal motion of one eye with nasal motion of the other one, as an addition of nasal and temporal gain yields similar gain values as temporal motion in binocular conditions (Fig. 3c,d, gray dots). Overall, these lesions confirm that acutely induced asymmetry in the visual sensory periphery imposes an asymmetry on visuomotor transformation.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Responses of the right eye to unidirectional motion stimulation. a, Motion traces (colored lines) of the right eye of binocular (n = 12), left eye lesioned (n = 11), and monocular tadpoles (n = 12) in response to unidirectional stimulus motion (dotted lines). Thin lines represent average responses per animal (1–4 stimulus bouts per direction); thick lines in plots and in the inset represent the average response across animals. b, Gain values (eye velocity/stimulus velocity) within the first 2 s of motion onset for the three paradigms. Connected dots represent nasal (N) and temporal (T) motion of the same eye. c, Symmetry indices (temporal gain/nasal gain) for the three paradigms. Values >1 indicate stronger motion toward the right and <1 motion toward the left. 1 (gray dotted line) indicates equal motion to the left and the right and a symmetric response. Arrowheads indicate low and high representative data points corresponding to anatomical values in Figure 5f and comparisons of anatomy and function in Figure 5g. d, Histograms of the distribution of symmetries in binocular and lesioned (top) versus monocular (bottom) animals. Data taken from panel c. Bin width on the x-axis is 0.25. Green shading highlights the distribution of lesioned animals for easier comparison with monocular animals.

Having established a direction-specific motor impairment of unilateral loss of visual input in postembryonic tadpoles, we next tested whether tadpoles that were reared from early embryo stages with only one eye compensated for such a sensory asymmetry during development. In contrast with the compensation shown by monocular tadpoles during sinusoidal stimulation, temporal eye motion during unidirectional constant velocity stimulation exhibited a gain of 0.14 ± 0.1 which was still impaired compared with binocular animals (p < 0.001e). Rather, this gain level was comparable to animals with an acute loss of one optic nerve (Fig. 4a,b, magenta). Moreover, motion in the nasal direction was not different than controls at 0.24 ± 0.08 (p = 0.58e), and as a result, the OKR was asymmetric toward the nasal direction (Fig. 4c, magenta; 0.65 ± 0.42; p = 0.0007f). Therefore, on a population level embryonic one-eyed animals appear to not exhibit behavioral plasticity sufficient to overcome an impairment in OKR directional perception. However, within this cohort of embryonic monocular tadpoles, left–right asymmetries were more heterogeneous in comparison with lesioned tadpoles. Indeed, the symmetry index (temporal gain/nasal gain; Fig. 4c) of monocular tadpoles did not distribute in a lognormal fashion (Fig. 4c,d, magenta; Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; p = 0.0038; data taken from c.), as opposed to both binocular and lesioned tadpoles (Fig. 4c,d, blue and green). This was likely caused by individual monocular tadpoles with an equal leftward and rightward, and therefore symmetric, OKR response (Fig. 4c, gray line; Fig. 4d, bottom). Such behavioral variability following embryonic unilateral sensory deprivation has been previously reported (Gordy and Straka, 2022), and we next investigated whether this functional variability had anatomical correlates following monocular embryonic development.

Anatomical correlates—RGC projections

As the OKR in Xenopus is mediated by the pretectum (Cochran et al., 1984; Kubo et al., 2014), a direct target of retinofugal fibers, we directly investigated the connectivity of RGC central projections to this area in binocular and monocular (embryonically manipulated) tadpoles. Application of dextran-conjugated dyes into the eyes of in vitro preparations (Fig. 5a,b, insets) revealed differences in RGC axon projections between one-eyed and control tadpoles. Control tadpoles showed exclusively contralateral RGC projections, reaching targets in the pretectum optic tectum (Fig. 5a,c) and thalamus (Fig. 3f, left; Beazley, 1975; Ruthazer et al., 2003). In monocular tadpoles, a subset of RGC axons innervated ipsilateral thalamic, pretectal, and tectal targets (Figs. 3f, right, 5b,d) instead of, or in addition to, contralateral ones. We quantified the number of labeled processes in the anterior midbrain across the mediolateral aspect of the brain (Fig. 5c,d) for multiple animals (controls, n = 5; monocular, n = 9), to approximate the amount of erroneously noncrossing RGCs (Fig. 5e). While in both the control and experimental conditions, most projections terminated in the contralateral anterior tectum (binocular, 98.74 ± 0.4%, mean ± SD; monocular, 83 ± 21.6%, mean ± SD), only monocular animals showed a fraction of RGC projections to the ipsilateral anterior tectum (Fig. 5e,f; 16.82 ± 21.62%, mean ± SD; p = 0.03h; data in 5f taken from 5e). The number of ipsilateral projections was variable and heterogeneous within the monocular group, particularly compared with binocular controls (Fig. 5e,f). Overall, we found that RGC pathfinding in one-eyed tadpoles is mainly impaired at the optic chiasm, with fibers projecting into the ipsilateral or contralateral pretectum and tectum. Further, the extent of chiasmatic pathfinding errors is highly variable between individuals and reminiscent of the behavioral variability found within monocular tadpoles.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

RGC projections in control and one-eyed tadpoles at stages 52–54. Representative confocal z-stacks of cleared whole brains of one control (a) and monocular (b) tadpole with RGC axons labeled. White background insets indicate dye application site (colored arrowheads), and colored boxes indicate the region of interest used for quantification of projections. c, d, Histograms of the amount of above threshold labeling within ROIs in a and b across the mediolateral axis of the brain at the anterior tectum for all control (n = 5) and monocular (n = 9) preparations. e, Cumulative sum per animal of the sections of the histograms corresponding to the contralateral or ipsilateral part of the ROI in control (blue) and monocular (magenta) animals. f, Statistical comparison of the amount of ipsilateral signal in control and monocular animals. Arrows indicate data points corresponding to two representative data points for low and high ipsilateral projection values as found in panel g and Figure 4c. g, Correlation of the fraction of ipsilateral RGC projections and symmetry of OKR in animals with anatomical and behavioral data (n = 6). Negative slope indicates a negative correlation between the amount of ipsilateral RGC projections and OKR symmetry. r2 = 0.884; fit, y = −3.734y + 1.049. Inset shows comparison of symmetry values between two groups of animals, grouped based on an anatomical threshold (>5% ipsilateral projections). Th, thalamus; PT, pretectum; OT, optic tectum; Di, diencephalon; OT, optic tectum; R, rostral; L, lateral; ROI, region of interest. Scale bar, 200 µm.

We thus asked if this variability correlated to the previously described variability in OKR behavior. We compared anatomical and behavioral data in animals in which anatomical dye tracings were successfully performed following behavioral assessment (n = 6). Plotting of the fraction of ipsilateral projections versus the symmetry index of the OKR response was used to approximate such a correlation. Linear fits through this relation revealed a significant negative correlation (Fig. 5g, red; r2 = 0.88; p = 0.005i compared with a slope of 0). Representative high (filled arrow) and low (empty arrow) OKR symmetry index animals (marked through Figs. 4c, 5f) suggest that an increased amount of erroneous ipsilateral RGC projections had a detrimental effect on functional compensation of the OKR. As nasal motion of these animals is unimpaired (Fig. 4b) and does not correlate with ipsilateral fibers (Fig. 3e, light blue) whereas the temporal direction does (Fig. 3e, red), it is likely that miswiring-caused impairments are either selective for temporal motion or can be compensated for in the nasal direction. Accordingly, animals with mostly or exclusively contralateral RGC projections partially compensated for the temporal OKR impairment following loss of one eye. While this is not a full restoration to the temporally asymmetric responses of binocular control tadpoles, it provides evidence for compensation of the temporal impairment observed in acute left eye lesioned tadpoles as observed in adult frogs (Jardon and Bonaventure, 1992b).

Overall, our data show that embryonic development of the visual system under monocular conditions causes aberrations in retinotectal pathfinding of the remaining eye. These anatomical aberrations directly correlate with functional impairment of OKR symmetry and an impairment of compensatory plasticity to a symmetric OKR, as shown by monocular tadpoles with little RGC pathfinding errors.

Discussion

The results of this paper demonstrate the requirement of bilateral eyes for appropriate establishment of contralaterally projecting RGCs in Xenopus tadpoles. They further show the detrimental effect of monocularity-derived retinopretectal pathfinding errors on compensatory plasticity of behavior.

OKR circuit development

Our results reported on the influence of binocularity on complete RGC axon decussation at the optic chiasm during embryonic development of Xenopus. While induction of ipsilateral retinotectal fibers has been demonstrated in this species previously, such results were observed in adult animals and resulted from regenerative, rather than embryonic, axonal growth (Glastonbury and Straznicky, 1978; Gaze and Straznicky, 1980). Due to different molecular environments in embryonic versus adult tissue (Jeffery, 2001), this regenerative effect does not necessarily translate to embryonic axonal pathfinding (Gaze and Straznicky, 1980). In fact, studies on embryonic axonal pathfinding in Xenopus were thus far unable to show a role of binocular interaction to establish the exclusive contralateral retinofugal projection during embryo development (Taylor and Gaze, 1990). The discrepancy of our data with this study can be explained by two differences in study design: First, extirpations in our study are performed earlier in embryonic development at stages 26–28, as opposed to stage 30 in previous studies. Since RGCs already start differentiating at stage 28 with their axons being detectable in the optic stalk (Cima and Grant, 1980), our removals at earlier stages of optic development may be more effective at preventing induction of an environment at the optic chiasm or in the optic stalk that facilitates contralateral or prevents ipsilateral guidance. Second, as anatomical data in the earlier study was gathered in adult frogs, it is possible that the ipsilateral fibers we observe in larval stages disappear during metamorphosis. Indeed, a transient, noncanonical ipsilateral RGC innervation in the absence of a contralateral eye has been shown previously in chick embryos (Thanos et al., 1984); however, aberrant projections are lost again during embryonic development in this species, before potential manifestation of behavioral relevance of this pathway. The role of the contralateral eye in RGC pathfinding is further corroborated by studies in mice and ferrets. These studies, however, show an opposite effect to ours, where rather than inducing a noncanonical ipsilateral projection, a canonical ipsilateral projection is reduced (Taylor and Guillery, 1995; Chan et al., 1999). Overall, while the importance of bilateral eyes for facilitation of appropriate decussation at the optic chiasm has been known, our study is the first to our knowledge to show this in Xenopus, while also highlighting an interspecies difference in the role of the contralateral eye as an inhibitor, rather than a promoter of ipsilateral RGC guidance.

Our study suggests the existence of mechanisms in chiasmatic pathfinding that remain thus far unidentified. Molecularly, ephrin B is implicated in the formation of ipsilaterally crossing fibers in this species (Nakagawa et al., 2000). In fact, adult Xenopus display ipsilaterally projecting RGC axons, which are established during metamorphosis and can be induced pre-metamorphically in the presence of ephrin B (Hoskins and Grobstein, 1985; Nakagawa et al., 2000). However, these axons target the thalamus rather than the tectum, and our results suggest that other mechanisms are relevant for axonal crossing in this species, either of a chemical or mechanical nature (Koser et al., 2016; Murcia-Belmonte and Erskine, 2019). Even though we are unable to verify whether individual fibers target the ipsilateral equivalent of their canonical contralateral target area, overall pathfinding to gross pretectal and tectal areas appears intact (Dingwell et al., 2000).

Directionality in Xenopus OKR

We showed that anatomical deviations from canonical, all-contralateral RGC fibers are negatively correlated with functional compensation for monocularity in the OKR. In our tadpoles, compensation manifests as an improvement of the temporal motion direction in some embryonically monocular tadpoles. Curiously, we also observe an asymmetry under control binocular conditions in the temporal direction. In zebrafish, such asymmetries are associated with higher spatial frequency of stimulus patterns than the ones employed in our study, suggesting a different spatial frequency preference in Xenopus (Qian et al., 2005). The stronger temporal component could originate from a summation of a small, temporal contribution from the respective eye with a stronger, nasal contribution of the contralateral eye during initial motion detection. This speculation is based on the symmetry and gain values we obtained by summing the temporal and nasal gain under monocular stimulation (Fig. 3c,d, gray dots). This may be mediated via intratectal and pretectal commissural connections, as in zebrafish (Portugues et al., 2014).

Preference for nasal motion in monocular stimulation is expected and fits data from other species (Masseck and Hoffmann, 2009; Portugues et al., 2014; Naumann et al., 2016). In our experiments, acute loss of one eye leads to an impaired OKR in the remaining eye as expected. When such acute condition responses were compared with embryonically generated one-eyed animals, we found that behavioral compensation was possible but correlated with retinotectal connectivity phenotypes. Behavioral compensation for loss of one eye was observed predominantly in animals with near-canonical, contralateral retinofugal pathways, and less in animals with ipsilateral retinopretectal fibers. Compensation, however, does not restore OKR back to binocular levels with a temporal motion preference but rather increases temporal motion to be equivalent with the nasal component, to facilitate symmetric OKR responses. Functionally, this could have two possible reasons: (1) Reaching maximum temporal motion requires bilateral eyes, and plasticity mechanisms are insufficient to compensate for the loss of the entire visual input from one. Or, (2) while each eye shows asymmetric OKR for unidirectional motion in binocular animals, the left and right eyes cooperate to ensure an equal leftward and rightward response across the entire visual system, comprising bilateral, complementary eyes. In monocular animals with control-like projections of the remaining eye, the single eye represents the entire visual system, and the only way to facilitate a symmetric OKR response is a symmetric response of this singular eye. Therefore, symmetry of the overall system, rather than a maximized motor response, may be desirable. Multiple studies back up the second hypothesis. First, embryonically generated one-eared animals compensate in a similar fashion in vestibular processing: At the cost of the normally preferred ipsiversive direction, the normally weak contraversive motion detection is increased to facilitate bidirectional motion sensing (Gordy and Straka, 2022). Second, multiple studies suggest that homeostasis is a common occurrence in tadpole plasticity, rather than response maximization. In both VOR and OKR, training stimuli induce motor output homeostasis rather than stronger compensation (Dietrich and Straka, 2016; Forsthofer and Straka, 2022). Theoretical studies corroborate this finding and suggest that consistency may be preferred, and full compensation may not be necessary to minimize retinal image slip under naturalistic conditions (Glasauer and Straka, 2022).

Role of ipsilateral RGC fibers

As only animals with no major ipsilateral projection show OKR compensation, we speculate that while these connections are likely functional on a physiological level, they impair appropriate sensorimotor function rather than contributing to compensation. They thus provide a possible alternative explanation for the impairment for the normally preferred motion direction in the vestibular system of embryonically one-eared tadpoles (Gordy and Straka, 2022). The detrimental effect of ipsilateral retinotectal connections may stem from functional miswiring, as RGCs selective for one motion direction wire to brain areas geared to process motion in the opposite direction. In Xenopus experiments placing a supernumerary eye or ear onto the head or even the tail, sensory afferents are either capable of functionally integrating or wire directly to their sensory nuclei, in large part probably due to conserved molecular guidance cues (Constantine-Paton and Law, 1978; Blackiston and Levin, 2013; Elliott and Fritzsch, 2018). Aside from erroneous crossing, the ipsi- or contralateral canonical pretectal and tectal targets of RGCs here are correctly innervated, and our study agrees with the notion that retinotectal and pretectal wiring is based majorly on genetic and molecular factors (Sperry, 1956; Fraser and Hunt, 1980; Mumm et al., 2006; Nevin et al., 2010). Within target nuclei, in addition to such factors, activity plays a major role to establish map formation and appropriate connectivity to postsynaptic neurons (Katz and Shatz, 1996). In experiments in zebrafish and Xenopus, ipsilateral connections from the eye to the tectum were induced through ablation of one tectal hemisphere. In both cases, these rerouted neurons targeted their appropriate topographical targets within the tectum and in case of zebrafish, even wired to the same directionally selective neurons as their ipsilateral counterparts along the rostrocaudal axis (Ruthazer et al., 2003; Ramdya and Engert, 2008). In the absence of an appropriately wired eye like in the present study, such direction appropriate wiring is likely to be detrimental: temporally selective RGCs of the right eye respond to rightward stimulus motion. Appropriately wired fibers to contralateral temporally selective pretectal neurons in the left pretectum will then initiate a rightward OKR (Kubo et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2020). However, if contacted, ipsilateral pretectal cells in the right pretectum initiate a leftward OKR in response to temporal stimulation, which would normally come from a leftward motion detected by the left eye. These competing signals may impair restoration of symmetric OKR. Multiple studies show similar behavioral effects of erroneous RGC projections in zebrafish mutants, where completely inversed retinotectal projections lead to an inversion of OKR (Karlstrom et al., 1996; Huang et al., 2006). Likewise, inversely connecting the left and the right eye with their ipsilateral tectum in adult frogs through surgery achieves inversion of the OKR (Sperry, 1945) in adult frogs. It is hypothesized that temporal motion is conveyed by pretectal commissures (Burgess et al., 2009; Portugues et al., 2014) while nasal motion stems from direct motor neuron activation (Cochran et al., 1984). Behavioral impairments following retinopretectal miswiring in our study therefore may go hand in hand with errors in commissural signaling or wrongly crossing fibers originating predominantly from temporal direction selective RGCs.

As changes in ipsi- or contralateral connectivity are associated with weaker compensation, adult plasticity mechanisms likely play a role in the observed compensation. In adult mice, functional plasticity in response to impairments in direction perception is following monocular deprivation (Prusky et al., 2006; Bauer et al., 2021), as it is in frog (Jardon and Bonaventure, 1992b). In the latter case, evidence suggests that the pretectum itself plays a role in maintaining directional symmetry (Jardon and Bonaventure, 1992b). Another likely candidate in Xenopus is the cerebellum, which is in general associated with OKR plasticity (Boyden et al., 2004). Specifically the gain upregulation required for the observed compensation in the temporal direction is linked to the cerebellum in Xenopus, which is close to functionally mature at our employed developmental stages (Forsthofer and Straka, 2022). Coupling of embryonic eye removals with developmentally early inactivation of the cerebellum could help identify contributions of the cerebellum either as a mediator or initiation site of such plasticity. Other brain areas are equally probable, particularly the hindbrain vestibular nuclei, which receive visual motion information and aid in consolidation of cerebellar-aided gaze stabilizing plasticity. Such contributions could be elucidated by means of visuovestibular mismatch studies. Finally, the pretectum itself has been shown to be involved in facilitation or impairment OKR symmetry following lesion-based impairments and warrants further functional investigations (Yücel et al., 1990; Jardon and Bonaventure, 1997; Van Alphen and De Zeeuw, 2002).

Overall, this study highlights two main aspects relating to plastic compensation of the OKR: In Xenopus, bilateral eyes are essential to reliably develop contralateral retinofugal fibers as previously shown in other species. Moreover, aberrant ipsilateral retinopretectal connections do not appropriately mediate visuomotor behaviors in the absence of another eye. Rather, they are correlated with impairments of specifically temporal eye motion, suggesting that embryonic pathfinding changes put a limit on optokinetic plasticity.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • The authors thank Prof. Dr. Ruben Portugues for support in finalizing and submitting the manuscript after the unexpected passing of Prof. Dr. Hans Straka on Dec. 9, 2022. The authors thank Dr. François Lambert, Prof. Boris Chagnaud, and Parthena Schneider-Soupiadis for critical proofreading and the Rupp lab and Hörmanseder lab for supplying of Xenopus embryos. We further thank PD Dr. Steffen Dietzel and the Center for Advanced Light Microscopy at the LMU, as well as the Center for Advanced Light Imaging (CALM) for support and access to microscopes. This research was funded by the German Science Foundation (RTG 2175, CRC 870, STR 478/3-1).

  • ↵†Deceased, Dec. 9, 2022.

  • ↵*M.F. and C.G. contributed equally to this work.

  • Michael Forsthofer's present address: School of Life Sciences, University of Sussex, Biology Road, Brighton BN1 9QG, UK.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bauer J,
    2. Weiler S,
    3. Fernholz MH,
    4. Laubender D,
    5. Scheuss V,
    6. Hübener M,
    7. Bonhoeffer T,
    8. Rose T
    (2021) Limited functional convergence of eye-specific inputs in the retinogeniculate pathway of the mouse. Neuron 109:2457–2468.e12. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2021.05.036
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Beazley LD
    (1975) Factors determining decussation at the optic chiasma by developing retinotectal fibres in Xenopus. Exp Brain Res 23:491–504. doi:10.1007/BF00234917
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Blackiston DJ,
    2. Levin M
    (2013) Ectopic eyes outside the head in Xenopus tadpoles provide sensory data for light-mediated learning. J Exp Biol 216:1031–1040. doi:10.1242/jeb.074963
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Boyden ES,
    2. Katoh A,
    3. Raymond JL
    (2004) Cerebellum-dependent learning: the role of multiple plasticity mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:581–609. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144238
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Branoner F,
    2. Straka H
    (2018) Semicircular canal influences on the developmental tuning of the translational vestibulo-ocular reflex. Front Neurol 9:404. doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.00404
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Burgess HA,
    2. Johnson SL,
    3. Granato M
    (2009) Unidirectional startle responses and disrupted left–right co-ordination of motor behaviors in robo3 mutant zebrafish. Genes Brain Behav 8:500–511. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00499.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Chan SO,
    2. Chung KY,
    3. Taylor JSH
    (1999) The effects of early prenatal monocular enucleation on the routing of uncrossed retinofugal axons and the cellular environment at the chiasm of mouse embryos. Eur J Neurosci 11:3225–3235. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00751.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Chan SO,
    2. Guillery RW
    (1993) Developmental changes produced in the retinofugal pathways of rats and ferrets by early monocular enucleations: the effects of age and the differences between normal and albino animals. J Neurosci 13:5277–5293. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-12-05277.1993
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Cima C,
    2. Grant P
    (1980) Ontogeny of the retina and optic nerve of Xenopus laevis: IV. Ultrastructural evidence of early ganglion cell differentiation. Dev Biol 76:229–237. doi:10.1016/0012-1606(80)90375-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Cline HT
    (1991) Activity-dependent plasticity in the visual systems of frogs and fish. Trends Neurosci 14:104–111. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(91)90071-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Cochran SL,
    2. Dieringer N,
    3. Precht W
    (1984) Basic optokinetic–ocular reflex pathways in the frog. J Neurosci 4:43–57. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-01-00043.1984
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Constantine-Paton M,
    2. Law MI
    (1978) Eye-specific termination bands in tecta of three-eyed frogs. Science 202:639–641. doi:10.1126/science.309179
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Dews PB,
    2. Wiesel TN
    (1970) Consequences of monocular deprivation on visual behaviour in kittens. J Physiol 206:437–455. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009023
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Dietrich H,
    2. Straka H
    (2016) Prolonged vestibular stimulation induces homeostatic plasticity of the vestibulo-ocular reflex in larval Xenopus laevis. Eur J Neurosci 44:1787–1796. doi:10.1111/ejn.13269
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Dingwell KS,
    2. Holt CE,
    3. Harris WA
    (2000) The multiple decisions made by growth cones of RGCs as they navigate from the retina to the tectum in Xenopus embryos. J Neurobiol 44:246–259. doi:10.1002/1097-4695(200008)44:2<246::AID-NEU13>3.0.CO;2-K
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Elliott KL,
    2. Fritzsch B
    (2018) Ear transplantations reveal conservation of inner ear afferent pathfinding cues. Sci Rep 8:13819. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-31952-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Forsthofer M,
    2. Straka H
    (2022) Homeostatic plasticity of eye movement performance in Xenopus tadpoles following prolonged visual image motion stimulation. J Neurol 270:57–70. doi:10.1007/s00415-022-11311-8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Fraser SE,
    2. Hunt RK
    (1980) Retinotectal specificity: models and experiments in search of a mapping function. Annu Rev Neurosci 3:319–352. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.03.030180.001535
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Gambrill AC,
    2. Faulkner RL,
    3. Cline HT
    (2018) Direct intertectal inputs are an integral component of the bilateral sensorimotor circuit for behavior in Xenopus tadpoles. J Neurophysiol 119:1947–1961. doi:10.1152/jn.00051.2018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Gaze RM,
    2. Straznicky C
    (1980) Regeneration of optic nerve fibres from a compound eye to both tecta in Xenopus: evidence relating to the state of specification of the eye and the tectum. Development 60:125–140. doi:10.1242/dev.60.1.125
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Gebhardt C,
    2. Auer TO,
    3. Henriques PM,
    4. Rajan G,
    5. Duroure K,
    6. Bianco IH,
    7. Del Bene F
    (2019) An interhemispheric neural circuit allowing binocular integration in the optic tectum. Nat Commun 10:1–12. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-13484-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Glasauer S,
    2. Straka H
    (2022) Low gain values of the vestibulo-ocular reflex can optimize retinal image slip. Front Neurol 13:897293. doi:10.3389/fneur.2022.897293
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Glastonbury J,
    2. Straznicky K
    (1978) Aberrant ipsilateral retinotectal projection following optic nerve section in Xenopus. Neurosci Lett 7:67–72. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(78)90114-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Gordy C,
    2. Straka H
    (2022) Developmental eye motion plasticity after unilateral embryonic ear removal in Xenopus laevis. Iscience 25:105165. doi:10.1016/j.isci.2022.105165
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Gordy C,
    2. Straka H,
    3. Houston DW,
    4. Fritzsch B,
    5. Elliott KL
    (2018) Transplantation of ears provides insights into inner ear afferent pathfinding properties. Dev Neurobiol 78:1064–1080. doi:10.1002/dneu.22629
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Grant S,
    2. Keating MJ
    (1989) Changing patterns of binocular visual connections in the intertectal system during development of the frog, Xenopus laevis. Exp Brain Res 75:99–116. doi:10.1007/BF00248534
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Holt CE
    (1989) A single-cell analysis of early retinal ganglion cell differentiation in Xenopus: from soma to axon tip. J Neurosci 9:3123–3145. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-09-03123.1989
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Hoskins SG,
    2. Grobstein P
    (1985) Development of the ipsilateral retinothalamic projection in the frog Xenopus laevis. II. Ingrowth of optic nerve fibers and production of ipsilaterally projecting retinal ganglion cells. J Neurosci 5:920–929. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-04-00920.1985
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Hua JY,
    2. Smear MC,
    3. Baier H,
    4. Smith SJ
    (2005) Regulation of axon growth in vivo by activity-based competition. Nature 434:1022–1026. doi:10.1038/nature03409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Huang Y-Y,
    2. Rinner O,
    3. Hedinger P,
    4. Liu S-C,
    5. Neuhauss SC
    (2006) Oculomotor instabilities in zebrafish mutant belladonna: a behavioral model for congenital nystagmus caused by axonal misrouting. J Neurosci 26:9873–9880. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2886-06.2006
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Jardon B,
    2. Bonaventure N
    (1992a) Plasticity of the frog monocular OKN: involvement of pretectal GABAergic and cholinergic systems. Neurosci Lett 137:189–192. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(92)90401-R
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Jardon B,
    2. Bonaventure N
    (1992b) The pretectal cholinergic system is involved through two opposite ways in frog monocular OKN asymmetry. Exp Brain Res 90:72–78. doi:10.1007/BF00229258
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Jardon BL,
    2. Bonaventure N
    (1997) Involvement of NMDA in a plasticity phenomenon observed in the adult frog monocular optokinetic nystagmus. Vis Res 37:1511–1524. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00291-X
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Jeffery G
    (2001) Architecture of the optic chiasm and the mechanisms that sculpt its development. Physiol Rev 81:1393–1414. doi:10.1152/physrev.2001.81.4.1393
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Karlstrom RO,
    2. Trowe T,
    3. Klostermann S,
    4. Baier H,
    5. Brand M,
    6. Crawford AD,
    7. Grunewald B,
    8. Haffter P,
    9. Hoffmann H,
    10. Meyer SU
    (1996) Zebrafish mutations affecting retinotectal axon pathfinding. Development 123:427–438. doi:10.1242/dev.123.1.427
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Katz LC,
    2. Shatz CJ
    (1996) Synaptic activity and the construction of cortical circuits. Science 274:1133–1138. doi:10.1126/science.274.5290.1133
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Keating MJ,
    2. Feldman JD
    (1975) Visual deprivation and intertectal neuronal connexions in Xenopus laevis. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 191:467–474.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  38. ↵
    1. Knorr A,
    2. Gravot C,
    3. Straka H,
    4. Glasauer S
    (2017) I spy with my little eye: a simple behavioral assay to test color perception in animal virtual reality setups. J Vis 17:661–661. doi:10.1167/17.10.661
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. Koser DE
    , et al. (2016) Mechanosensing is critical for axon growth in the developing brain. Nat Neurosci 19:1592–1598. doi:10.1038/nn.4394
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Kubo F,
    2. Hablitzel B,
    3. Dal Maschio M,
    4. Driever W,
    5. Baier H,
    6. Arrenberg AB
    (2014) Functional architecture of an optic flow-responsive area that drives horizontal eye movements in zebrafish. Neuron 81:1344–1359. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.043
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Masseck OA,
    2. Hoffmann KP
    (2009) Comparative neurobiology of the optokinetic reflex. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1164:430–439. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03854.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. McLaughlin T,
    2. O'Leary DD
    (2005) Molecular gradients and development of retinotopic maps. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:327–355. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135714
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Mumm JS,
    2. Williams PR,
    3. Godinho L,
    4. Koerber A,
    5. Pittman AJ,
    6. Roeser T,
    7. Chien CB,
    8. Baier H,
    9. Wong RO
    (2006) In vivo imaging reveals dendritic targeting of laminated afferents by zebrafish retinal ganglion cells. Neuron 52:609–621. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Murcia-Belmonte V,
    2. Erskine L
    (2019) Wiring the binocular visual pathways. Int J Mol Sci 20:3282. doi:10.3390/ijms20133282
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. ↵
    1. Nakagawa S,
    2. Brennan C,
    3. Johnson KG,
    4. Shewan D,
    5. Harris WA,
    6. Holt CE
    (2000) Ephrin-B regulates the ipsilateral routing of retinal axons at the optic chiasm. Neuron 25:599–610. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81063-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Naumann EA,
    2. Fitzgerald JE,
    3. Dunn TW,
    4. Rihel J,
    5. Sompolinsky H,
    6. Engert F
    (2016) From whole-brain data to functional circuit models: the zebrafish optomotor response. Cell 167:947–960.e20. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2016.10.019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Nevin LM,
    2. Robles E,
    3. Baier H,
    4. Scott EK
    (2010) Focusing on optic tectum circuitry through the lens of genetics. BMC Biol 8:126. doi:10.1186/1741-7007-8-126
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Niell CM,
    2. Smith SJ
    (2005) Functional imaging reveals rapid development of visual response properties in the zebrafish tectum. Neuron 45:941–951. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.047
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Nieuwkoop PD,
    2. Faber J
    (1994) Normal table of Xenopus laevis (Daudin): a systematical and chronological survey of the development from the fertilized egg till the end of metamorphosis. New York: Garland Publishing Inc. doi:10.1201/9781003064565
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  50. ↵
    1. Oster SF,
    2. Deiner M,
    3. Birgbauer E,
    4. Sretavan DW
    (2004) Ganglion cell axon pathfinding in the retina and optic nerve. Semin Cell Dev Biol 15:125–136. doi:10.1016/j.semcdb.2003.09.006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Pan C,
    2. Cai R,
    3. Quacquarelli FP,
    4. Ghasemigharagoz A,
    5. Lourbopoulos A,
    6. Matryba P,
    7. Plesnila N,
    8. Dichgans M,
    9. Hellal F,
    10. Ertürk A
    (2016) Shrinkage-mediated imaging of entire organs and organisms using uDISCO. Nat Methods 13:859–867. doi:10.1038/nmeth.3964
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Petros TJ,
    2. Rebsam A,
    3. Mason CA
    (2008) Retinal axon growth at the optic chiasm: to cross or not to cross. Annu Rev Neurosci 31:295–315. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125609
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Poggio GF,
    2. Poggio T
    (1984) The analysis of stereopsis. Annu Rev Neurosci 7:379–412. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.07.030184.002115
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Portugues R,
    2. Feierstein CE,
    3. Engert F,
    4. Orger MB
    (2014) Whole-brain activity maps reveal stereotyped, distributed networks for visuomotor behavior. Neuron 81:1328–1343. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Prusky GT,
    2. Alam NM,
    3. Douglas RM
    (2006) Enhancement of vision by monocular deprivation in adult mice. J Neurosci 26:11554–11561. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3396-06.2006
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Qian H,
    2. Zhu Y,
    3. Ramsey DJ,
    4. Chappell RL,
    5. Dowling JE,
    6. Ripps H
    (2005) Directional asymmetries in the optokinetic response of larval zebrafish (Danio rerio). Zebrafish 2:189–196. doi:10.1089/zeb.2005.2.189
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Ramdya P,
    2. Engert F
    (2008) Emergence of binocular functional properties in a monocular neural circuit. Nat Neurosci 11:1083–1090. doi:10.1038/nn.2166
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Rose T,
    2. Jaepel J,
    3. Hübener M,
    4. Bonhoeffer T
    (2016) Cell-specific restoration of stimulus preference after monocular deprivation in the visual cortex. Science 352:1319–1322. doi:10.1126/science.aad3358
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    1. Ruthazer ES,
    2. Akerman CJ,
    3. Cline HT
    (2003) Control of axon branch dynamics by correlated activity in vivo. Science 301:66–70. doi:10.1126/science.1082545
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Schindelin J
    , et al. (2012) Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods 9:676–682. doi:10.1038/nmeth.2019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Schönenberger N,
    2. Escher G,
    3. Van der Loos H
    (1983) Axon number in oculomotor nerves in Xenopus: removal of one eye primordium affects both sides. Neurosci Lett 41:239–245. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(83)90457-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Soupiadou P,
    2. Gordy C,
    3. Forsthofer M,
    4. Sanchez-Gonzalez R,
    5. Straka H
    (2020) Acute consequences of a unilateral VIIIth nerve transection on vestibulo-ocular and optokinetic reflexes in Xenopus laevis tadpoles. J Neurol 267:62–75. doi:10.1007/s00415-020-10205-x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  63. ↵
    1. Sperry RW
    (1945) Restoration of vision after crossing of optic nerves and after contralateral transplantation of eye. J Neurophysiol 8:15–28. doi:10.1152/jn.1945.8.1.15
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  64. ↵
    1. Sperry RW
    (1956) The eye and the brain. Sci Am 194:48–53. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0556-48
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  65. ↵
    1. Steeves JK,
    2. González EG,
    3. Steinbach MJ
    (2008) Vision with one eye: a review of visual function following unilateral enucleation. Spat Vis 21:509. doi:10.1163/156856808786451426
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Straznicky C,
    2. Tay D,
    3. Glastonbury J
    (1980) Regeneration of an abnormal ipsilateral visuotectal projection in Xenopus is delayed by the presence of optic fibres from the other eye. Development 57:129–141. doi:10.1242/dev.57.1.129
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  67. ↵
    1. Taylor JSH,
    2. Gaze RM
    (1990) The course of regenerating retinal axons in the frog chiasma: the influence of axons from the other eye. Anat Embryol 181:405–412. doi:10.1007/BF00186913
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Taylor JSH,
    2. Guillery RW
    (1995) Effect of a very early monocular enucleation upon the development of the uncrossed retinofugal pathway in ferrets. J Comp Neurol 357:331–340. doi:10.1002/cne.903570211
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Thanos S,
    2. Fujisawa H,
    3. Bonhoeffer F
    (1984) Elimination of ipsilateral retinotectal projections in mono-ophthalmic chick embryos. Neurosci Lett 44:143–148. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(84)90072-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. van Alphen AM,
    2. De Zeeuw CI
    (2002) Cerebellar LTD facilitates but is not essential for long-term adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Eur J Neurosci 16:486–490. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02094.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Van Hof-van Duin J,
    2. Mohn G
    (1986) Monocular and binocular optokinetic nystagmus in humans with defective stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 27:574–583.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Wu Y,
    2. Dal Maschio M,
    3. Kubo F,
    4. Baier H
    (2020) An optical illusion pinpoints an essential circuit node for global motion processing. Neuron 108:722–734.e5. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2020.08.027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Yücel YH,
    2. Jardon B,
    3. Kim M-S,
    4. Bonaventure N
    (1990) Directional asymmetry of the horizontal monocular head and eye optokinetic nystagmus: effects of picrotoxin. Vis Res 30:549–555. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(90)90067-U
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: David Schoppik, New York University

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Martha Bagnall. Note: If this manuscript was transferred from JNeurosci and a decision was made to accept the manuscript without peer review, a brief statement to this effect will instead be what is listed below.

The reviewer and I agree: This well-written and clearly explained manuscript does a very nice job of addressing, and explaining, biological variability in response to loss of a single eye. Unusually, we have no suggestions for improvement, as the manuscript is comprehensively referenced, the figures are clear, and the work is very nicely situated within the relevant literature.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 11 (1)
eNeuro
Vol. 11, Issue 1
January 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Bilateral Retinofugal Pathfinding Impairments Limit Behavioral Compensation in Near-Congenital One-Eyed Xenopus laevis
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Bilateral Retinofugal Pathfinding Impairments Limit Behavioral Compensation in Near-Congenital One-Eyed Xenopus laevis
Michael Forsthofer, Clayton Gordy, Meghna Kolluri, Hans Straka
eNeuro 20 December 2023, 11 (1) ENEURO.0371-23.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0371-23.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Bilateral Retinofugal Pathfinding Impairments Limit Behavioral Compensation in Near-Congenital One-Eyed Xenopus laevis
Michael Forsthofer, Clayton Gordy, Meghna Kolluri, Hans Straka
eNeuro 20 December 2023, 11 (1) ENEURO.0371-23.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0371-23.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Material and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • eye extirpation
  • frog
  • OKR
  • optokinetic reflex
  • plasticity
  • retinotectal

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
  • Interference underlies attenuation upon relearning in sensorimotor adaptation
Show more Research Article: New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
  • Interference underlies attenuation upon relearning in sensorimotor adaptation
  • Rod Inputs Arrive at Horizontal Cell Somas in Mouse Retina Solely via Rod–Cone Coupling
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.