Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

How Does Temporal Blurring Alter Movement Timing?

Dominika Drążyk and Marcus Missal
eNeuro 5 September 2023, 10 (9) ENEURO.0496-22.2023; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0496-22.2023
Dominika Drążyk
Institute of Neurosciences (IONS), Cognition and System (COSY), Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels 1200, Belgium
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Marcus Missal
Institute of Neurosciences (IONS), Cognition and System (COSY), Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels 1200, Belgium
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Marcus Missal
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Extended Data
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Description of the experimental paradigm. A, Timeline of visual events on the screen in front of the subject. First, two square boxes appeared with a fixation cross (+ sign) in the central one. The location of the eccentric box was randomly assigned to the left or to the right of the central one with the same probability. Afterwards, a noninformative visual stimulus appeared in the central box for 2000 ms (preforeperiod or “pre-FP” interval) followed by another 1100-ms fixation period. Thereafter, the warning stimulus (WS) was briefly presented in the central box (first red square). The appearance of the WS initiated the FP until the imperative stimulus (IS or “go” signal) appeared in the eccentric box (second red square). The arrow illustrates the direction of the correct saccade executed after the IS appearance at the end of the description of the Figure 1A. B, The high uncertainty U distribution was a composite obtained by convolving the anchor duration with a Gaussian kernel. C, Data collection started with either the short or the long anchor duration (counterbalanced between subjects). For a given anchor duration, two blocks of 100 trials each were collected: first, a baseline (B) condition consisting of only one duration; second, a block of trials drawn from the U distribution. Number of trials (‘N TRIALS’) per condition is included in the round brackets at the end of the description of the Figure 1C. D, Distributions of short (left panel) and long (right panel) anchors, grouped in 50-ms bins. Colored traces represent the B and U hazard functions.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Analysis of saccade latencies as a function of FP duration. A, Average saccade latency ± SE for different average FP durations. Each dot shows values for the corresponding bin of the distribution together with its SE (whiskers). Gray solid lines represent linear regression fits to the Ushort (left panel) and Ulong (right panel) distributions of the FP together with the 95% SE (gray ribbon). Yellow dotted lines indicate average latency for the B distributions. B, Average saccade latency as a function of reciprocal HR (in a.u. - arbitrary units) for the Ushort (left panel) and Ulong (right panel) distributions.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Analysis of the variability of saccadic latencies. A, SD of saccade latency in the different experimental conditions (dots) together with its SE (dark whiskers), and 95% confidence interval (CI, light whiskers). Horizontal gray bars indicate paired analysis between conditions (n.s., not significant). For the equivalence tests related to the magnitude of difference between the SD of the saccade latency, see Extended Data Figure 2-1A. B, Variance of saccadic latencies plotted against different FP lengths. Each dot shows the value for the corresponding bin of the U distributions. The solid line shows a linear regression line together with the standard error of the regression SE (gray ribbon). Yellow dot indicates average variance around anchor durations. C, SD of saccade latencies plotted against HRclassic for different FP lengths. Each dot shows the value for the corresponding bin of the U distributions. Solid lines show linear regression lines together with the standard error of the regression SE (gray ribbon). Yellow dot indicates average SDs around anchor durations.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Comparison of dominant hypotheses of temporal preparation and empirical data. Predictions of temporal preparation models (gray dotted curves) versus experimental data (blue lines for the uncertainty U condition and yellow dots for baseline B condition) for average latency (left panels) and SD (right panels). A, Left, Saccadic latency increased as a function of reciprocal of HR (HRrec). Right, The SD of saccadic latency decreased with increasing FP duration, in contradiction with the temporal blurring hypothesis based on HR. B, Left, Saccadic latency decreased with FP duration in contradiction with predictions of the PDF hypothesis. Indeed, where the PDF is maximum, movement latency should be minimum and “go” signal timings infrequently experienced on the sides of the PDF should evoke longer latencies (inverted U-shape). Right, Probabilistic “blurring” hypothesis. Movement timing variability should follow an inverted U-shape distribution. C, Left, As predicted by the fMTP model, a shorter FP duration during trial “n−1” than during the current trial “n” is associated with a relatively shorter movement latency. Right, The fMTP model assumes temporal blurring of the inner representation of FP duration and an increasing saccadic SD for longer FP durations. This is functionally equivalent to the one assumed by the HR hypothesis and was not observed here. D, Hypothetical build up of temporal expectancy of the imperative “go” signal for short and long anchor durations.

Tables

  • Figures
  • Extended Data
    • View popup
    Table 1

    Data preprocessing number and percentage of the trials that entered the final analysis

    Number of trialsN% of total
    Total16,800100.00
    After artifact rejection15,94894.93
    With visually guided saccades11,67669.50
    • View popup
    Table 2

    Linear mixed models analysis of FPn duration effects on saccade latency

    DatasetModelFixed termsβ ± SE95% CIp-valueRandom termsσ [ms]
    UshortFPn.rs1Intercept307.495 ± 14.293[279.498, 335.467]<2.22 × 10–16Residual51.905
    FPn−0.054 ± 0.008[−0.071, −0.038]1.00 × 10–10Subject33.571
    UlongFPn.rs1Intercept298.043 ± 20.465[257.938, 338.114]<2.22 × 10–16Residual52.322
    FPn−0.027 ± 0.008[−0.043, −0.011]9.49 × 10–4Subject34.545
    • Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method. Fixed and random effect structures were chosen in advance (see Extended Data Tables 1-1 and 1-2). Models were fitted using the Gaussian fit (see Extended Data Fig. 1-1 for the comparison between different distribution choices).

    • σ, SD of the random terms (subject and residual).

    • View popup
    Table 3

    Linear mixed models analysis of HRrec effects on saccade latency

    DatasetModelFixed termsβ ± SE95% CIp-valueRandom termsσ [ms]
    UshortHRrec.rs1Intercept217.924 ± 5.306[207.398, 228.425]<2.22 × 10–16Residual51.949
    HRrec0.371 ± 0.060[0.253, 0.489]8.86 × 10–10Subject33.400
    UlongHRrec.rs1Intercept231.100 ± 5.467[220.272, 241.932]<2.22 × 10–16Residual52.329
    HRrec0.190 ± 0.059[0.074, 0.306]0.001Subject34.503
    • Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method.

    • σ, SD of the random terms (subject and residual).

    • View popup
    Table 4

    Linear mixed models analysis of FPn–1 duration effects on saccade latency

    DatasetModelFixed termsβ ± SE95% CIp-valueRandom termsσ [ms]
    UshortFPn–1.rs1Intercept213.039 ± 14.550[184.560, 241.535]<2.00 × 10–16Residual52.311
    FPn–10.005 ± 0.008[−0.011, 0.022]0.541Subject33.102
    UlongFPn–1.rs1Intercept205.805 ± 20.649[165.360, 246.253]<2.00 × 10–16Residual52.410
    FPn–10.011 ± 0.008[−0.005, 0.027]0.176Subject34.389
    • Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method. For the equivalence tests related to the relationship between the saccade latency and FPn– 1 duration, see Extended Data Figure 2-1B.

    • σ, SD of the random terms (subject and residual).

    • View popup
    Table 5

    Linear mixed models analysis of sequence duration effects on saccade latency

    DatasetModelFixed termsβ ± SE95% CIp-valueRandom termsσ [ms]
    Ushortsequence.rs1Intercept220.861 ± 5.287[210.372, 231.328]<2.22 × 10–16Residual52.071
    Sequence−0.029 ± 0.006[−0.041, −0.018]7.01 × 10–7Subject33.453
    Ulongsequence.rs1Intercept232.557 ± 5.442[221.764, 243.332]<2.22 × 10–16Residual52.321
    Sequence−0.020 ± 0.006[−0.031, −0.008]8.41 × 10–4Subject34.498
    • Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method.

    • σ, SD of the random terms (subject and residual).

    • View popup
    Table 6

    Linear models and slope analysis (Ivry and Hazeltine, 1995) results for saccade latency

    DatasetFormulaβ ± SE95% CIp-valueAdj. R2
    UshortLatency ∼ FPn −0.051 ± 0.008[−0.068, −0.033]3.01 × 10–50.73
    Latency ∼ HRrec 0.319 ± 0.074[0.155, 0.484]0.0010.59
    σ2 (latency) ∼ k2 *FPn2 + c−18704.922 ± 7683.918[−35305.020, −2104.826]0.0300.26
    σ (latency) ∼ HRclassic −23.317 ± 3.831[−31.748, −14.885]7.88 × 10–50.75
    UlongLatency ∼ FPn −0.023 ± 0.005[−0.035, −0.011]0.0010.54
    Latency ∼ HRrec 0.158 ± 0.032[0.087, 0.228]5 × 10–40.66
    σ2 (latency) ∼ k2 * FPn2 + c−42253.758 ± 7100.721[−57593.93, −26913.580]4.82 × 10–50.71
    σ (latency) ∼ HRclassic −22.625 ± 5.603[−34.957, −10.293]0.0020.56
    • The dataset was averaged across subjects and distinct FPn lengths.

    • σ2, variance; k, Weber’s fraction; c, time-independent source of response variance; adj., adjusted.

Extended Data

  • Figures
  • Tables
  • Extended Data Figure 1-1

    Diagnostic plots for the FPn.rs1 model refitted using different distributions. A, Gaussian distribution, link: identity. Plots were generated using R package performance (Lüdecke et al, 2021). B, Gaussian distribution, link: log. C, Gaussian distribution, link: identity, fitting the log-transformed data. D, Gamma distribution, link: identity. Plots were generated using R package DHARMa (Hartig, 2020). E, Gamma distribution, link: log. F, Inverse Gaussian distribution, link: identity. G, Inverse Gaussian distribution, link: log. Download Figure 1-1, EPS file.

  • Table 1-1

    Linear mixed models random structure selection for the analysis of FPn, FPn-1 HRrec and sequence effects on saccade latency. Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method. Download Table 1-1, DOC file.

  • Table 1-2

    Linear mixed models random structure selection for the analysis of FPn, FPn-1 HRrec and sequence effects on saccade latency. Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method. Download Table 1-2, DOC file.

  • Extended Data Figure 2-1

    Plots of the equivalence test for the short (left panel) and the long anchor (right panel), separately. A, Equivalence test results for the latency SD between different anchors in the U and B distribution. A black square shows mean difference in raw scores between B and U groups with the 98% confidence intervals (black horizontal line). B, Equivalence test results for the FPn–1 effects on the saccade latency. Colored dot shows the mean effect size in standardized scores with the 98% confidence intervals (horizontal colored line). Download Figure 2-1, EPS file.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 10 (9)
eNeuro
Vol. 10, Issue 9
September 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
How Does Temporal Blurring Alter Movement Timing?
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
How Does Temporal Blurring Alter Movement Timing?
Dominika Drążyk, Marcus Missal
eNeuro 5 September 2023, 10 (9) ENEURO.0496-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0496-22.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
How Does Temporal Blurring Alter Movement Timing?
Dominika Drążyk, Marcus Missal
eNeuro 5 September 2023, 10 (9) ENEURO.0496-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0496-22.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • anticipation
  • eye movements
  • hazard rate
  • temporal cognition
  • temporal expectation

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • The use of internal states to guide behaviour is associated with functional engagement of the anterior insula in male rats.
  • Excitatory synaptic transmission is differentially modulated by opioid receptors along the claustro-cingulate pathway
  • Reinforced odor representations in the anterior olfactory nucleus can serve as memory traces for conspecifics
Show more Research Article: New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • A novel subpopulation of prepositus hypoglossi nucleus neurons projecting to the cerebellar anterior vermis and hemisphere in rats.
  • Reinforced odor representations in the anterior olfactory nucleus can serve as memory traces for conspecifics
  • Upright posture: a singular condition stabilizing sensorimotor coordination
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.