Abstract
Subjective uncertainty arises because the estimation of the timing of an event into the future is error prone. This impact of stimulus-bound uncertainty on movement preparation has often been investigated using reaction time tasks where a warning stimulus (WS) predicts the occurrence of a “go” signal. The timing of the “go” signal can be chosen from a particular probability distribution with a given variance or uncertainty. It has been repeatedly shown that reaction times covary with the shape of the used “go” signal distribution. This is interpreted as evidence for temporal preparation. Moreover, the variance of the response time should always increase with the duration of the delay between the WS and the “go” signal. This increasing variance has been interpreted as a consequence of the temporal “blurring” of future events (scalar expectancy). The present paper tested the validity of the temporal “blurring” hypothesis in humans with a simple oculomotor reaction time task where subjective and stimulus-bound uncertainties were increased. Subjective uncertainty about the timing of a “go” signal was increased by lengthening the delay between the WS and the “go” signal. Objective uncertainty was altered by increasing the variance of “go” signal timing. Contrary to temporal blurring hypotheses, the study has shown that increasing the delay between events did not significantly increase movement timing variability. These results suggest that temporal blurring could not be a property of movement timing in an implicit timing context.
Significance Statement
It is often assumed that the estimation of the timing of a distant future event is more error prone (or variable) than the estimation of a closer one. This phenomenon is referred to as temporal “blurring.” Metaphorically, this could be compared in the spatial domain with the blurring of a visual stimulus when perceived at an increasing distance. Surprisingly, in the current oculomotor implicit timing task, movement variability actually decreased with the increasing temporal delay between a warning and an imperative “go” signal. Therefore, temporal blurring is not a general property but depends on the precise context of timing behavior.
Introduction
Successfully processing the temporal uncertainty associated with a future event is essential to prepare an appropriate behavioral response. However, the timing of a movement is affected by at least two sources of uncertainty: the intrinsic temporal stochasticity of the stimulus (referred to as “stimulus-bound” or objective uncertainty) and the uncertainty about one’s estimation of its timing (subjective uncertainty). Movement timing is often studied with a paradigm where a warning stimulus (WS; e.g., a spot of light or a sound) precedes the appearance of an imperative “go” signal the subject has to quickly respond to; Woodrow, 1914; Niemi and Näätänen, 1981). The delay between the WS and the “go” signal is referred to as the foreperiod (FP). Before the “go” signal, temporal preparedness increases and reduces movement latency that is commonly used as a dependent variable (e.g., keypress reaction time or eye movement latency). It has been suggested that this process could rely on an estimation of the hazard rate (HR) of the “go” signal that is defined as the conditional probability that the stimulus will appear given that it has not appeared yet (Niemi and Näätänen, 1981; Luce, 1986; Trillenberg et al., 2000; Nobre et al., 2007; Vangkilde et al., 2012, 2013; Herbst et al., 2018; Tomassini et al., 2019; Salet et al., 2022). Mathematically, HR rapidly increases during the FP and reaches a maximum at the time of the longer “go” signal. Therefore, movement latency should be shorter when HR reaches that maximum. The subjective variant of the HR hypothesis further suggests that the inner representation of future events could be “blurred.” Indeed, the estimation of the timing of a distant future event is more error prone (or variable) than the estimation of a closer one. This could be modelled by convolving the objective probability distribution of the “go” signal with a variable Gaussian kernel whose variance linearly increases with elapsed time (Janssen and Shadlen, 2005; Pasquereau and Turner, 2015; Herbst et al., 2018) or equivalent procedure (de Hemptinne et al., 2007). Subjective uncertainty about the precise timing of a future event could explain why the variability of latency increases as the delay between the WS and the “go” signal increases. This phenomenon is probably a particular instantiation of the scalar variability property that describes how temporal perception becomes more variable as the duration to be estimated becomes longer (Gibbon, 1977; Gibbon et al., 1984; Gibbon and Church, 1990; Gallistel and Gibbon, 2000; Wearden and Lejeune, 2008).
An alternative hypothesis to HR suggests that in certain circumstances (e.g., absence of reward) the brain could not perform the delicate computation of HR but could instead use an internal representation of the probability density function of the “go” signal to plan a motor response (referred to as the PDF hypothesis). Where the PDF is maximum (peak value for a symmetrical function) movement latency should be minimum given the higher number of “go” signals experienced by the subject at that time. Stimulus timings infrequently experienced on the sides of the PDF should evoke longer latencies and an inverted U distribution of reaction times is predicted (Grabenhorst et al., 2019, 2021). The PDF hypothesis further suggests a form of probabilistic “blurring” where the size of the blurring kernel does not linearly increase with time but is a function of the probability distribution of the “go” signal. If a Gaussian-shaped distribution of “go” signal timing is used, then movement timing variability should be minimal around the mean value and increase on both sides of the distribution.
Lastly, in contrast with HR and PDF, the formalized multiple trace theory of temporal preparation (fMTP) suggests that a representation of “go” signal timing could emerge by classical associative learning without the need for sophisticated probability computations (Los et al., 2014; Salet et al., 2022). In fMTP, a memory trace of previously experienced timings is used to guide future behavior. For instance, a shorter FP duration during trial “n−1” than during the current trial “n” will be associated with a relatively shorter movement latency. Temporal blurring is explicitly represented in this model by a “smearing” factor k. This aspect of fMTP is functionally equivalent to a convolution with a Gaussian kernel.
The aim of the present study was to further test the hypothesis of temporal blurring in a simple oculomotor reaction time task where the timing of the “go” signal could be constant or vary according to a Gaussian-shaped distribution.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and ethics
The present study was conducted at the Université catholique de Louvain. Participants were volunteers (18–65 years old) who did not suffer from known neurologic or psychiatric diseases and did not take psychoactive substances at least a day before the experiment. Participants had corrected to normal vision if necessary. Three participants were excluded from the study because of obvious misunderstanding of the task or extensive noise in the oculomotor signal. This resulted in 42 participants included in the analysis (average ± SD, age 26.50 ± 4.30 years, 23 women). Participants provided their informed written consent to take part in this study. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki guidelines and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Université catholique de Louvain under number B403201733677. Data are available to participants on written request.
Experimental design
Each trial started with the appearance of two white boxes (3 × 3° visual angle each, see Fig. 1A). One box was located in the center of the screen (central box) and the second one was located either to the left or to the right (eccentric box, eccentricity 9.5° of visual angle from the central box) with the same probability (p = 0.5). These boxes were used to exclude potentially confounding effects of spatial uncertainty about the future position of the imperative “go” signal. Participants were initially required to maintain visual fixation of the central box for 1100 ms. Afterwards, an abstract noninformative shape was displayed in the central box for 2000 ms (referred to as the “pre-FP” interval). This pre-FP interval was introduced to potentially use the same paradigm with an explicit timing cue (not the aim of the present study). Next, the warning stimulus (WS; red square) was displayed in the central box for 50 ms. After a variable delay (foreperiod or FP, lasting between 1250 and 2750 ms) the imperative “go” stimulus (IS; eccentric red square) was displayed in the eccentric box for 50 ms. Participants were instructed to maintain gaze on the central box during the pre-FP and FP intervals and then initiate a visually-guided saccade toward the IS as quickly as possible. Trials ended with an intertrial interval (ITI) of randomized duration (1050 ± 400 ms).
Extended Data Figure 1-1
Diagnostic plots for the FPn.rs1 model refitted using different distributions. A, Gaussian distribution, link: identity. Plots were generated using R package performance (Lüdecke et al, 2021). B, Gaussian distribution, link: log. C, Gaussian distribution, link: identity, fitting the log-transformed data. D, Gamma distribution, link: identity. Plots were generated using R package DHARMa (Hartig, 2020). E, Gamma distribution, link: log. F, Inverse Gaussian distribution, link: identity. G, Inverse Gaussian distribution, link: log. Download Figure 1-1, EPS file.
In each block of trials, average FP duration was either 1600 ms (short) or 2400 ms (long), collectively referred to as “anchor.” For each anchor, two blocks of 100 trials each were displayed: first, a block of trials with a constant FP duration referred to as the baseline condition (abbreviated as B distribution, estimated technical jitter
In summary, there were four different blocks of trials (average ± SD): Bshort (anchor = 1600 ± 5 ms), Ushort (anchor = 1600 ± 120 ms), Blong (anchor = 2400 ± 5 ms), and Ulong (anchor = 2400 ± 120 ms; see Fig. 1C). A block of B trials was always firstly collected with anchors being either 1600 ms or 2400 ms. This block was followed by the corresponding U block of trials. The sequence was then repeated with the other anchor. As an example, if the anchor in the first B block was 2400 ms (Blong), it was followed by a U block of trials with the same anchor duration (2400 ms or Ulong). Next, a block of B trials with the other anchor duration (in this example, 1600 ms or Bshort) was presented, followed by the corresponding U block of trials (1600 ms or Ushort). This design was used to minimize carry-over effects. Indeed, experiencing U before B would change latencies in response to B that would not be a baseline any more (see Los et al., 2017, 2021; Crowe et al., 2021). This carry-over effect could persist even a week after changing the probability distribution of the FP (Mattiesing et al., 2017). Pairs of blocks of different anchors were presented in random order, counterbalanced between subjects.
Data collection and preprocessing
Eye movements were recorded binocularly at 500 Hz using an EyeLink1000 (SR Research), calibrated at the beginning of each data collection session and recalibrated every 30 trials. Stimuli display and eye movement recordings were created using Experimental Builder (SR Research). Stimuli were displayed on a high-resolution screen (size: 54 × 30 cm, 1920 × 1080 pixels, VPixx Technologies) at 60 Hz. Subjects were sitting at a distance of 80 cm from the display screen. Oculomotor recordings of poor quality were excluded from the analysis using an artifact rejection algorithm (DataViewer, SR Research). All trials including frequent blinking were removed from the analysis. Further, if the continuity of the oculomotor recording was not preserved throughout the whole trial, or if a drift of calibration occurred, it was also excluded. Eye blinks and lost eye signal amounted to 5.07% of all trials (see Table 1). Saccades were detected using an amplitude (>1°), velocity (>22°/s) and acceleration (>3800°/s2) criteria. Only visually-guided saccades initiated between 100 and 1000 ms after IS onset and landing within the IS box were further analyzed (69.5% of all trials).
Table 1-1
Linear mixed models random structure selection for the analysis of FPn, FPn-1 HRrec and sequence effects on saccade latency. Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method. Download Table 1-1, DOC file.
Table 1-2
Linear mixed models random structure selection for the analysis of FPn, FPn-1 HRrec and sequence effects on saccade latency. Models were fitted using the restricted maximum likelihood method. Download Table 1-2, DOC file.
Statistical analysis, linear mixed effects model
Trial-by-trial saccadic latencies were fitted using a linear mixed models approach (LMM; R package lme4 library version 1.1–30; Bates et al., 2015). Reaction time data are usually not normally distributed and fitted using non-Gaussian distributions (Baayen and Milin, 2010; Lo and Andrews, 2015). In the present study the latency variable was fitted using various distributions: γ, Gaussian, and inverse Gaussian. A log transformation of saccadic latencies was also calculated and fitted with the Gaussian distribution. All options were examined using the QQplot and residuals (R packages performance, Lüdecke et al., 2021 and DHARMa, Hartig, 2020). However, none of the above-mentioned distributions improve the fit compared with the Gaussian fit (for the comparison between these different choices, see Extended Data Fig. 1–1). As a result, Gaussian distribution was chosen to fit the latency models. Following the approach of Grabenhorst et al. (2019), we decided to fit three instances of the hazard rate function HR: (1) the classic HR function (HRclassic; see Fig. 1D) that presents a general growing trend with increasing FPn duration together with a local peak around the anchor duration; (2) the mirrored HR function (HRmirror) that is HRclassic mirrored around its mean. HRmirror shows a general decreasing trend with increasing FPn duration with a local minimum around the anchor duration; (3) the reciprocal HR (HRrec) that takes the reciprocal of HRclassic (1/HRclassic). HRrec presents an abruptly decreasing trend with increasing FPn duration and a negligible local minimum around the anchor duration.
Besides the hazard rate variants, main predictors of interest were: FPn, length of the foreperiod in the given trial n; FPn– 1, length of the foreperiod in the previous trial n−1; sequence, difference between the length of the current and previously displayed FP (FPn– 1 – FPn). Additionally, a trial variable (numeric index of the given trial in the block) was introduced in models, following the recommendation of Baayen and Milin (2010). Indeed, it was shown that including a variable representing trial number could help to track the influence of predictors on a trial-by-trial basis.
The labeling of models was as following: the full prefix indicated that the model includes the maximal fixed effect structure incorporating the main predictor and a trial variable. Models including only the main predictor were labeled using the name of the predictor of interest (e.g., FPn). Further, to describe the random structure of models, the suffix rs was used. Different random structures were denoted with consecutive numbers. For example, FPn.rs1 denotes the model with FPn as an only predictor and the first random slope structure.
The Bayes Information Criterion (BIC; Bolker et al., 2009) was estimated to choose an optimal random term structure for the full models fitted with restricted maximum likelihood method (REML; Zuur et al., 2010; Barr et al., 2013). For all those full models, random intercept structure performed better than the random slope (for details see the values of BIC for models full.rs1, full.rs2, full.rs3, and full.rs4 for all main predictors and both anchors in Extended Data Table 1-1). Therefore, rs1 random terms structure was chosen in the further analyses.
Next, to establish the best fixed term structure, models with different fixed terms were refitted using a maximum likelihood method (ML) and compared using BIC. For all main predictors and both anchors, models including only a main effect (FPn.rs1, HRrec.rs1, FPn– 1.rs1, and sequence.rs1) performed better than the corresponding full models (for details see the values of BIC in Extended Data Table 1–2). Results show that the incorporation of the trial variable in all models did not improve their fits. Therefore, only the main predictor models were refitted using the REML method and presented in Results. For each of the presented models, visual inspection of their diagnostics was conducted to monitor the convergence and accordance with the model assumptions.
Statistical analysis, simple linear model
Beside the LMM analyses conducted on the trial-by-trial basis, a simple linear model (LM) was used to test specific predictions of the HR, PDF and fMTP hypotheses. This time, the dataset was averaged across subjects and distinct FPn lengths. Three variants of the HR hypothesis were fitted to the data. Classical HR presented the best fit and therefore was included in Results. Furthermore, the relationship between latency variance and FPn duration was tested using the slope analysis proposed by Ivry and Hazeltine (Ivry and Hazeltine, 1995; see also Piras and Coull, 2011; Ameqrane et al., 2014). The slope analysis was used to estimate Weber’s fraction k, with the assumption that the variability of the subjective time estimation is a constant fraction of the estimated time. This model assumes also that the total response variance comes not only from the timing processes but also from a time-independent source, modelled by the variable c. Consequently, if saccadic latencies obey Weber’s law then the data could be modelled using the following equation:
Statistical analysis, equivalence tests
Because of the detection of nonsignificant results in the present study, an equivalence test was performed for the analysis of latency SD between different anchors in the U and B distribution (R package TOSTER; Caldwell, 2022) as well as for the influence of the FPn–1 on saccadic latency (R package parameters; Lüdecke et al., 2021). This equivalence test evaluates the hypothesis that the effect size obtained for the nonsignificant analyses is small enough to be called negligible (see Lakens et al., 2018 for details). Performing the test requires establishing the smallest effect size of interest (SESOI) that will create the equivalence bounds (eqb). Those should be chosen based on the effect sizes detected in similar experiments from the field. As it was difficult to find a proper sample of experiments mimicking the current design, the experiment of Tomassini et al. (2019) that is the closest to the current paradigm, was chosen as a reference. Reference data included only the control group of Tomassini et al., study. First, the equivalence bounds for the latency SD analysis were calculated based on the critical effect size of Tomassini et al., analyses (eqb = [−1.125, 1.125], Cohen’s dz type). Then, as the manuscript of Tomassini et al., does not include the analysis of previous FP length effect on response latency, the influence of FPn–1 on the latency was modelled on their data by the authors. Resulting equivalence bounds were based on Cohen’s f2 (Selya et al., 2012) of the calculated model (eqb = [−0.09, 0.09] for the short anchor and eqb = [−0.02, 0.02] for the long anchor).
Results
To test the influence of the duration of the current foreperiod (FPn) on saccadic latency in the high uncertainty U distributions, a linear mixed model (LMM) was applied to short (1600 ms) and long (2400 ms) anchors, separately. In short anchor model FPn.rs1 saccadic latency significantly decreased as a function of FP duration showing a potential effect of temporal preparation (β = −0.054 ± 0.008, p = 1 × 10–10). Similarly, for the long anchor model FPn.rs1 saccadic latency moderately decreased as a function of FP duration (β = −0.027 ± 0.008, p = 9 × 10–4; see Table 2 for details).
The potential of the HR hypothesis to explain observed saccadic latencies was also tested with LMM. For both anchor durations, saccadic latency significantly decreased as a function of HRrec showing the effect of temporal preparation (model HRrec.rs1, short anchor: β = 0.371 ± 0.060, p = 8.86 × 10–10; long anchor: β = 0.190 ± 0.059, p = 0.001; see Table 3 for details). However, the relationship between reciprocal HR and RT had a shallower slope for the Ulong distribution (Fig. 2B).
Extended Data Figure 2-1
Plots of the equivalence test for the short (left panel) and the long anchor (right panel), separately. A, Equivalence test results for the latency SD between different anchors in the U and B distribution. A black square shows mean difference in raw scores between B and U groups with the 98% confidence intervals (black horizontal line). B, Equivalence test results for the FPn–1 effects on the saccade latency. Colored dot shows the mean effect size in standardized scores with the 98% confidence intervals (horizontal colored line). Download Figure 2-1, EPS file.
The fMTP model suggests that the short-term memory of previously experienced FPs (FPn–1) could partly determine saccadic latency during the current FP (FPn). Therefore, two possible memory effects were tested. First, there was no effect of the previous FP duration on the latency of the saccade in the current trial (FPn–1.rs1 model: short anchor: p = 0.541; long anchor: p = 0.176; see Table 4 for details).
Second, the sequence variable (difference between FPn and FPn–1) was fitted to the latency data. Saccadic latency significantly decreased as a function of sequence for both anchor durations (model sequence.rs1, short anchor: β = −0.029 ± 0.006, p = 7.01 × 10–7, long anchor: β = −0.020 ± 0.006, p = 8.41 × 10–4; see Table 5 for details).
Beside the LMM analyses, a simple linear model (LM) was used to test specific predictions of the HR, PDF and fMTP models with latencies averaged across subjects and distinct FP lengths. Averaged saccadic latency decreased with increasing FP duration in Ushort (p = 3.01 × 10–5, adj. R2 = 0.73; Fig. 2A, left) and Ulong distribution (p = 0.001, adj. R2 = 0.54; Fig. 2A, right; see Table 6 for details). As expected, these results suggest that temporal preparation increased during the FP. Furthermore, a linear relationship between average latency and HRrec was found for the Ushort (p = 0.001, adj. R2 = 0.59) and Ulong distribution of FPs (p = 5 × 10–4, adj. R2 = 0.66; see Table 6 for details). However, no local minima were observed around anchor FP durations.
The HR, PDF and fMTP models all suggest that the SD of timing responses should increase with anchor because of temporal “blurring” or scalar expectancy. However, in the present study, the SD of saccadic latency for long and short anchors did not increase (B conditions, t(41) = 0.663, p = 0.511; U conditions, t(41) = 0.060, p = 0.952; see Fig. 3A for details). Furthermore, the SD of saccade latencies was also computed when movements were grouped according to the bins of the U distributions. To test the relationship between the SD of the latency and the length of the FP in the given trial, a slope analysis was conducted. The SD of saccadic latencies significantly decreased with elapsed time for the Ulong condition (p = 3.84 × 10–5, adj. R2 = 0.72; see Table 6). However, as the square of the Weber’s fraction was negative (k2 = −42 253.758), Weber’s law was not obeyed and the scalar property was not detected. Instead, the relationship between the SD of the saccadic latency distributions and variants of the hazard rate hypothesis was tested. Unexpectedly, the SD significantly decreased with HRclassic for both anchors (p = 7.88 × 10–5, adj. R2 = 0.75 for short and p = 0.002, adj. R2 = 0.56 for long anchor; see Table 6 and Fig. 3B for details) and therefore presented a better fit than FPn.
Equivalence tests were used to check whether the effect size obtained for the nonsignificant analyses was small enough to be called negligible (see Lakens et al., 2018 for details). Both equivalence tests related to the magnitude of difference between the SD of the latency in Bshort and Blong condition (t(41) = 5.330, p = 2.00 × 10–6) as well as Ushort and Ulong conditions were significant (t(41) = 3.254, p = 0.001; see Extended Data Fig. 2-1A). Therefore, in both those cases, the null equivalence hypothesis could be rejected and the size of the effect for both analyses was negligible. Finally, the equivalence test of the influence of the FP(n−1) length on the saccadic latency was shown significant for the short anchor (p < 2.22 × 10–16, 98% CI = [−0.01, 0.02]), but the test for the long anchor was not conclusive (p = 0.145, 98% CI = [−0.01, 0.03], see Extended Data Fig. 2-1B). Therefore, the null equivalence hypothesis was rejected and the negligible character of the effect size was accepted only for the short anchor.
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the temporal “blurring” hypothesis by altering stimulus-bound and subjective uncertainties in an oculomotor reaction time task. Subjects were firstly tested in a baseline condition B with a delay between the warning stimulus (WS) and an imperative “go” signal that was kept constant, either 1600 ms (short anchor) or 2400 ms (long anchor). One hundred B trials with one of these two anchor (Bshort or Blong) durations were firstly collected. Afterwards, stimulus-bound temporal uncertainty was increased and 100 trials from the U distribution were presented (same anchor duration as in B but σ = 120 ms; see Fig. 1 and Materials and Methods for more details). After data collection using both B and U distributions and the first anchor duration, the whole process was repeated with the other anchor duration. The dependent variable measured was saccadic latency.
First, a scalar increase of the variance of latencies between short (Bshort) and long (Blong) anchor durations (Fig. 3A) was not observed. This suggests that scalar expectancy was not observed in the present results. Second, comparison of U distributions revealed that the slope of the saccadic latency/FP relationship was shallower for long anchors (Ulong) than for short ones (Ushort; see Fig. 2A and Tables 2, 6 for details). These results suggest a reduced sensitivity to time around the long anchor duration. Third, no latency minimum around the anchor values was observed (Fig. 2A). This result suggests that latency was not directly dependent on FP probability. Fourthly, saccadic latencies did not co-vary with FP duration in the 1500- to 1750-ms range in the Ushort condition and in the 2100- to 2550-ms range in the Ulong condition (Fig. 2A). Subjectively, FP durations in these indifference ranges could have been interpreted as equivalent to the two anchor durations. The indifference range was larger for long durations, suggesting again an impact of subjective uncertainty but not as predicted by the temporal blurring hypothesis nor the PDF hypothesis. Therefore, scalar expectancy stricto sensu was not observed when comparing between distributions, neither in the B nor in the U conditions. Within U distributions, the variability of latencies unexpectedly decreased with increasing FP durations (see Table 6). The gradual decrease of the variability of movement latency could be modelled using the classic HR function (see Table 6). In summary, HR could have a double impact: reducing movement latency and increasing precision.
It has been suggested that scalar expectancy could not be observed in implicit oculomotor timing (Ameqrane et al., 2014). Scalar property would be present only in explicit timing tasks when there is an overt use of temporal information (Nobre and van Ede, 2018; Coull and Nobre, 2008). However, Piras and Coull (2011) suggested that scalar variability was indeed observed in a temporal expectancy task in humans. How could the volatility of scalar expectancy be explained? The oculomotor variant of scalar expectancy was initially proposed by Janssen and Shadlen (2005) in the Rhesus monkey where intensive animal training and reward expectation could have led to the emergence of an explicit representation of elapsed time during the FP, like in prospective timing. Moreover, it could be suggested that the motor system used to provide a response could also play a major role. Indeed, to our knowledge, most human timing studies required a manual response (typically a button press) after a visual or auditory “go” signal was presented (Piras and Coull, 2011; Grabenhorst et al., 2019, 2021; Tomassini et al., 2019; Visalli et al., 2021; Tal-Perry and Yuval-Greenberg, 2022; but for critical review, see Grondin, 2012). This could have induced another sensorimotor mapping with explicit attention devoted to the production of an accurate manual response, therefore scalar expectancy. A systematic comparison between manual and oculomotor responses over the same range of durations and for the same probability distributions remains to be done.
It could also be suggested that scalar expectancy was not observed in the present study because temporal preparation itself was reduced. However, a robust reduction of movement latency with increasing FP duration in the U conditions was shown in the current research. Figure 4 shows a schematic comparison of predictions issued from current hypotheses about temporal preparation (dashed lines) and experimental results presented here (blue continuous lines and yellow dots). If the PDF hypothesis were true then the latency/FP relationship should be U-shaped with a minimum around the mean where “go” signal timing was the most frequent (Fig. 4A; see Grabenhorst et al., 2019, 2021). This was not observed. If temporal preparation was based on an estimation of the objective (HRclassic) or subjective (HRsubj) hazard rate (Fig. 4B) then shorter latencies should be observed for long FP durations where these functions reached a maximum. This prediction was confirmed within U distributions. However, the HRsubj hypothesis additionally implies that the variance of latencies should increase with elapsed time. This was not observed here. Altogether, the best model fit to the average latency data were the reciprocal of HR (HRrec).
Two memory-related effects were tested in the current study. A memory effect of the previously experienced FP duration (FPn–1) on saccade latency was not observed here. This is in contradiction with previously published studies from authors group that demonstrated a strong influence of trial history on the latency of saccades during trial “n” even if the total number of trials was rather small (∼100–200 trials; see Ameqrane et al., 2014; Degos et al., 2018; Hsu et al., 2019). Authors suggest that the memory of past FPs could play a role only if the total number of possible “go” signal timings is small. On the other hand, a sequence effect was found here (difference between FPn and FPn–1) as suggested by the fMTP model (Visalli et al., 2021; Salet et al., 2022). Nevertheless, temporal blurring was also hypothesized in the fMTP model, in contradiction with the observation in the current study. In conclusion, the hazard rate hypothesis provides a better explanation of the empirical data than other approaches. The HRrec function could be used to model the influence of temporal preparation on average latency whereas the HRclassic function could model the reduction of movement variability with elapsed time. These results suggest that temporal blurring does not necessarily occur and that the particular timing context, implicit or explicit, plays a major role.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique (FNRS) Grant CDR/OL J0132.21.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.