Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: Confirmation, Cognition and Behavior

Exploring the Neural Processes behind Narrative Engagement: An EEG Study

Hossein Dini, Aline Simonetti and Luis Emilio Bruni
eNeuro 17 July 2023, 10 (7) ENEURO.0484-22.2023; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0484-22.2023
Hossein Dini
1The Augmented Cognition Lab, Aalborg University, Copenhagen 2450, Denmark
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Aline Simonetti
2Department of Marketing and Market Research, University of Valencia, Valencia 46022, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Luis Emilio Bruni
1The Augmented Cognition Lab, Aalborg University, Copenhagen 2450, Denmark
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Analysis of event segmentation, self-report, and recall. A, Upper panel, The starting point of each phase as identified by the independent participants after participating in a workshop, including the span of each phase and the phase’s names. The y-axis of this panel indicates the expected engagement evoked by the narrative plot. A, Middle panel, The raters’ z-normalized engagement ratings. The hot colors denote higher scores, and the cold colors indicate lower scores. A, Bottom panel, The z-transformed group-averaged engagement ratings. The dashed vertical lines represent the start of the new phase and the end of the previous phase. B, The participants’ recall similarity for each separate phase. In this case, the falling action and denouement phases were combined. The matrices show the similarities between the subjects, with the hot colors indicating higher similarity scores. The horizontal bars show the SDs of the subjects’ recall similarities. The stars indicate that there was a significant difference (p < 0.001) between the SDs of the last phase as compared with other phases.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    The procedure for calculating the dynamic intersubject correlation (dISC) and the results thereof. A, The steps in preprocessing the EEG data. Each participant’s data went through a preprocessing stage in which the raw data were de-noised. After this, the volume conduction effect was corrected by implementing the current source density approach and then downsampling and z-transforming. B, The steps for calculating the dISC for three participants (from a total of 32) and one channel (out of all the channels). The defined window (indicated by the gray) was applied to every channel for all participants. After this, we calculated the correlation between the participants’ corresponding channels for all participant pairs. Thus, this figure demonstrates the correlations for all three participant pairs as an example. The correlation between each window pair is one sample point of pairwise correlations (shown by the purple, blue, and brown arrows at the top of panel). Sliding the window throughout the entire signal, we obtained a correlation signal for the three participant pairs. The average for these correlation signals was then used in the next step. C, The procedure for the statistical analysis that was conducted using a permutation test. First, we calculated the actual correlation between the averaged dISC and group-averaged engagement rates. Then, we phase-randomized the group-averaged correlations 10,000 times and calculated the correlation between the averaged dISC and each of these phase randomized engagement ratings. Thus, a null distribution and an actual correlation was obtained. D, The results from the comparison of the actual correlation to the null distribution. The orange, cyan, and, purple violin plots correspond to the FC5, FC1, and CP2 channels, respectively. The location and the obtained p-values are reported in this panel. The black horizontal lines represent the observed correlations. E, The results from the comparison of the actual correlation to the null distribution, separately for each phase of narrative. The first row shows the actual correlation against the null distribution and the second row indicate the channel locations which their correlation is reported. The phases in which dISC was significantly correlated with engagement ratings (rising, crisis, and climax) are shown with colorful violin plots and channel locations. The phases in which dISC failed to significantly predict the engagement ratings (exposition, falling, and denouement) are plotted in gray. Total of two channels (CP5 and FP2) in rising, three channels (Pz, Cz, and FP2) in crisis, and two channels (FT8 and FC6) in climax were the predictor channels. For the nonsignificant phases (exposition, falling and denouement), we just plotted three examples of the channels (C4, FC6, and CP5) to show where the actual correlation is located against the null distribution.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    The schematic representation of the dynamic predictive model. A, The sample of preprocessed signals and sliding window (gray squares). B, The functional connectivity among all channels was obtained from the signals occurred within each window. C, Out of each functional connectivity matrix, three graph features, node degree, clustering coefficient, and betweenness centrality, were extracted. D, The procedure of the predictive model. In “Step 1,” we concatenated all the functional connectivity matrices (or graph features) of all subjects and divided it to a train and test sets (the leave-one-subject-out procedure). In “Step 2,” we trained a support vector regression model using the functional connectivities of all subjects but one to predict the averaged engagements and tested the model on the leaved-out subject. The output of Step 2 is the predicted engagement rating. By calculating the correlation between actual engagement and predicted engagement, we obtained the actual correlation. In “Step 3,” we shuffled the engagement ratings as presented in Figure 2 and repeated Step 2 to predict these shuffled ratings. By calculating the correlation between the predicted engagement and each shuffled engagement scores and repeating it for all the 1000 shuffled engagement scores, we obtained a null distribution of correlations. By testing the actual correlation on this null distribution, we obtained the significancy level of the prediction. Note that we implemented this procedure for functional connectivity features and all three graph features separately.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    The engagement level predictions for the whole narrative. A, The testing of the actual correlation against the null distribution. Each violin plot refers to the prediction results for each EEG feature. The colored violin plots refer to statistically significant prediction features, and the gray plots refer to the features that were not statistically significant. The dark red violin plot refers to the dFC feature, and dark green plot refers to the BC feature. The light to dark gray spectrum refers to the CC, ND, and EEG features, respectively. The gray circles in the violin plots show the actual correlation obtained in each cross-validation fold with reference to each participant. The red horizontal lines show the observed correlation. The stars indicate that the corresponding feature had been a predictor that was chosen because of it being significantly higher than a chance occurrence. B, The predictor FCs that are either positively (red) or negatively (blue) correlated with engagement ratings across brain regions (F = frontal, C = central, P = parietal, T = temporal, O = occipital). Each cell represents the average number of times that each region played a predictor role. The light to dark colors (red and blue) refer to the strength of their score, which is the number of times they were considered a predictor divided by the length of all possible connections with other channels. To show the positive and negative features simultaneously, we used an upper triangle for positive and a lower triangle for negative features. The original features are symmetrical. C, the predictor FC features and channels on the scalp from three perspectives. The predictor FCs that were averaged in panel B are shown individually here in that the positively correlated items are indicated by red and the negatively correlated items are indicated by blue. The predictor channels obtained from the BC features are represented as colored dots, with bigger dots denoting higher proportions.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    The results for predicting group averaged engagement ratings using dISC-bands. The violin plots refer to the null distribution of the correlation value (r) between surrogated engagement scores and the calculated dISC-bands, only for the significant channels. The observed r, which is the correlation between actual engagement scores and dISC-bands, is also indicated black lines over violin plots. The topo-plots indicate the observed r of the channels that could significantly predict the engagement scores, on the scalp. The p-values are reported above each violin plot, and all of them are corrected. In dISC-δ and dISC-θ, five channels could significantly predict the group-averaged engagement ratings. The spatial activity of the predictor channels can be seen on the topo-plot beside the violin plot. In dISC-β, two channels could significantly predict the engagement scores and the spatial distribution of the predictor channels can be seen in the topo-plot.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    See Figure 4. The predictions of the engagement ratings of each phase using the corresponding features. The black horizontal lines show the observed correlation, and the violins show the null distribution. A, The phase prediction results for all phases using dFC features. It shows that the prediction in the last two phases (falling action and denouement) was significantly higher than could be explained by chance. B, The prediction results using BC features and the significant prediction that occurred in the last two phases. C, The predictor FCs across regions and their proportion scores for each phase. D, The predictive FCs and the channels that the BCs played as predictor roles for each phase.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1

    The starting time points indicated by the participants

    PhaseStarting point (s)Duration (s)Number of participants who agreed (%)SD of all responses (s)
    Exposition015119 (100%)0
    Rising action15114913 (68.42%)95.83
    Crisis3004510 (52.63%)65.92
    Climax34511512 (63.16%)32.37
    Falling action4602217 (89.47%)23.74
    Denouement4822418 (94.74%)4.05
    • This table shows the time stamps for starting and duration made by independent raters. The choice of the starting time point of each phase of the dramatic arc was obtained from the data of 19 participants that indicated these moments. From the starting time points, the duration of each phase was calculated. The number of participants that indicated the same time points statistically and the standard deviation (SD) of all responses are indicated in the last two columns, respectively.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 10 (7)
eNeuro
Vol. 10, Issue 7
July 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Exploring the Neural Processes behind Narrative Engagement: An EEG Study
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Exploring the Neural Processes behind Narrative Engagement: An EEG Study
Hossein Dini, Aline Simonetti, Luis Emilio Bruni
eNeuro 17 July 2023, 10 (7) ENEURO.0484-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0484-22.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Exploring the Neural Processes behind Narrative Engagement: An EEG Study
Hossein Dini, Aline Simonetti, Luis Emilio Bruni
eNeuro 17 July 2023, 10 (7) ENEURO.0484-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0484-22.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • dramatic arc
  • dynamic functional connectivity
  • EEG
  • engagement
  • intersubject correlation
  • narrative cognition

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: Confirmation

  • Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Encodes the Trace Period during Appetitive Pavlovian Conditioning
  • Dissociating Frontal Lobe Lesion Induced Deficits in Rule Value Learning Using Reinforcement Learning Models and a WCST Analog
  • Aniracetam Ameliorates Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Behavior in Adolescent Mice
Show more Research Article: Confirmation

Cognition and Behavior

  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
  • Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Encodes the Trace Period during Appetitive Pavlovian Conditioning
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.