Abstract
The imagination of tactile stimulation has been shown to activate primary somatosensory cortex (S1) with a somatotopic specificity akin to that seen during the perception of tactile stimuli. Using fMRI and multivariate pattern analysis, we investigate whether this recruitment of sensory regions also reflects content-specific activation (i.e., whether the activation in S1 is specific to the mental content participants imagined). To this end, healthy volunteers (n = 21) either perceived or imagined three types of vibrotactile stimuli (mental content) while fMRI data were acquired. Independent of the content, during tactile mental imagery we found activation of frontoparietal regions, supplemented with activation in the contralateral BA2 subregion of S1, replicating previous reports. While the imagery of the three different stimuli did not reveal univariate activation differences, using multivariate pattern classification, we were able to decode the imagined stimulus type from BA2. Moreover, cross-classification revealed that tactile imagery elicits activation patterns similar to those evoked by the perception of the respective stimuli. These findings promote the idea that mental tactile imagery involves the recruitment of content-specific activation patterns in sensory cortices, namely in S1.
Significance Statement
It has been shown previously that mental imagery of sensations in different modalities can activate respective primary sensory cortices (visual/auditory/tactile). However, a relation of such activation to the imagined mental content was mainly shown for the visual system. Here, we generalize this concept to the somatosensory domain by showing that content-specific activation during tactile mental imagery can be found in primary somatosensory cortex subarea BA2. Most importantly, we show that tactile imagery elicits activation patterns similar to those evoked by sensory stimulation. Our results provide further evidence that sensory recruitment is among the brain processes that allow conscious information representation.
Introduction
It is a fundamental cognitive ability of humans to mentally imagine diverse content in varying degrees of vividness. The underlying neural mechanisms that generate and process mental content in the absence of sensory stimulation remain subject to ongoing investigations, despite a long research history (Kosslyn et al., 2001; Tong, 2013; Nanay, 2018). Generally, frontal areas, such as the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), medial frontal gyrus, and supplementary motor area (SMA), as well as multimodal parietal regions including the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) are most commonly reported to be activated during imagery across modalities (Hassabis and Maguire, 2009; Zvyagintsev et al., 2013; Schmidt and Blankenburg, 2019). These higher-order frontoparietal areas are thought to relate to the general mental construction process (e.g., by contributing attentional resources; Lückmann et al., 2014). However, these brain regions do not seem to code information about the mental content per se (Riggall and Postle, 2012). What types of neural activation reflect specific mental content is an open question in current research (Christophel et al., 2017).
With regard to this question, it has previously been shown that the imagery of a specific sensation, such as the imagery of being touched or seeing an object, can activate corresponding sensory regions (Kosslyn et al., 1995), a process referred to as sensory recruitment (Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005). Especially for the visual domain this has been frequently shown (Kosslyn et al., 1993; Klein et al., 2000; Kosslyn and Thompson, 2003; Ganis et al., 2004), while the activation of corresponding sensory cortices induced by auditory (Zatorre et al., 1996) or tactile mental imagery (Yoo et al., 2003; Schmidt and Blankenburg, 2019) have been examined less often. Sensory recruitment of the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and the secondary somatosensory cortex (S2) was initially indicated by means of signal increases related to the imagery of brushing stimuli (Yoo et al., 2003). Furthermore, a somatotopic activation of the hierarchically highest subarea BA2 of S1 has been demonstrated during imagery of vibrotactile stimuli at different body parts (Schmidt and Blankenburg, 2019), and even down to BA1 during more detailed vibrotactile mental representations (Schmidt et al., 2014). The extent to which these activations in sensory regions directly relate to the content of mental imagery, however, remains an open question.
Multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) has proven to be a useful approach for determining the content-specific activation of perception and mental imagery. Several studies have revealed shared neural codes for visual stimulation and corresponding imagery (Stokes et al., 2009, 2011; Reddy et al., 2010; Cichy et al., 2012). While Reddy et al. (2010) found the visual presentation of simple objects and their imagery to activate shared codes in the ventral temporal cortex, other studies revealed that imagery of simple shapes (distinct letter stimuli) activates the same neural representations within high-level visual cortex that are activated by corresponding visual stimulation (Stokes et al., 2009, 2011). But also early visual areas were shown to contain the content of mental imagery. Cichy et al. (2012) revealed that, in addition to category-selective regions and higher-order visual areas, also hierarchically lower subregions of the visual cortex (V1, V2, V3) contain shared neural codes of visual stimulation and imagery. And by applying MVPA to fMRI data, Albers et al. (2013) again showed that early visual cortices exhibit content-specific activation patterns during mental imagery. These studies in the visual system demonstrate that MVPA allows identifying content-specific activation not detectable by univariate analyses. With regard to the somatosensory system, it still needs to be tested whether content representation of tactile mental imagery can be found in S1.
In this fMRI study, we used three different types of vibrotactile stimulation (Press, Flutter, and Vibration) in a 2 × 3 factorial design [Stimulation (Stim) vs Imagery (Imag)] to test whether content-specific activity of mental imagery can be found in S1 by applying univariate and multivariate analyses. The stimuli were chosen so as to activate different mechanoreceptors in the skin, which are known to preferably respond to different features of tactile stimulation, such as pressure or vibration (Delhaye et al., 2018), and therefore should induce different patterns of somatosensory cortical activation. As the higher-order subregion BA2 of S1 was recently shown to be involved in mental tactile imagery (Schmidt and Blankenburg, 2019), we hypothesized that it is more likely to detect content-specific codes in BA2 than in the other subregions BA3b and BA1.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The final sample of participants included in the analysis comprised N = 21 participants (16 female, 4 male, 1 diverse; mean age ± SD, 24.90 ± 5.24 years). Twenty-seven participants completed the fMRI measurement. Of these, six participants were excluded because of low ratings on successful mental imagery (see below). All volunteers gave written informed consent to participate in the experiment and received remuneration for their time. Handedness of all participants was assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Veale, 2014). Within a range of −100 to +100 for fully left and right handedness, 20 of the included subjects were classified as right handed (mean ± SD laterality score, 77.8 ± 30.6), and one classified as left handed (laterality score, −23.1). All procedures were in line with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the study was approved by the local ethics committee.
Procedure
After informing the participants about the experiment and safety requirements as well as checking their suitability for MRI, participants were instructed about the mental imagery task outside of the scanner and then familiarized with the task during the anatomic scan. Participants performed the task during six functional MRI runs interspersed with breaks on demand overall for ∼1 h. After leaving the scanner, they filled out the Vividness of Tactile Imagery Questionnaire (VTIQ; see below), concerning their overall ability to imagine tactile stimuli.
Stimuli
As applied in previous studies on mental imagery and perception (Schmidt et al., 2014, 2017; Nierhaus et al., 2015; Schröder et al., 2019, 2021; Forschack et al., 2020), the vibrotactile stimuli were presented to the left index finger via a piezoelectric Braille-like display (Piezostimulator, QuaeroSys, St. Johann, Germany) with 16 pins (4 × 4 matrix with 2.5 mm spacing; Fig. 1A). To ensure that the module was held in the same position throughout the experiment, it was taped to the participants’ index fingers. The three different stimulus types (Press, Flutter, and Vibration) were presented or had to be imagined for 3 s duration to allow clear perception and imagination. To optimally apply a slowly changing stimulus in the low-frequency range, the Press stimulus comprised 2 Hz half-sine wave pulses. To further minimize the physical and perceptual differences among the three stimulus types, for the other two stimulus types the 2 Hz half-sine wave pulses were modulated by 30 Hz (Flutt) or 150 Hz (Vibro), corresponding to the flutter and vibration frequency ranges, respectively (Fig. 1B).
Together with the vibrotactile stimuli, visual cues were presented, projected on a screen visible from within the scanner. Visual cues comprised three centrally presented shapes (star, circle, and pentagon) depicted in either blue or yellow. The color corresponded to the Stimulation or Imagery condition, and shapes indicated the Vibration type. Color and shape associations with stimulus type were balanced across participants.
Experimental paradigm
The experimental paradigm comprised a 2 × 3 design, with the factors Stimulation/Imagery and stimulus types Press/Flutt/Vibro. Figure 1C illustrates the time course of one trial. Each trial started with a 1 s visual precue to prepare for the task. Then the cue increased in size to be shown for 3 s while the participants either perceived a stimulation or had to imagine it. In a quarter of the Stimulation trials, no stimulation was presented despite the presentation of a stimulation cue, and the participants had to attend to correctly respond whether or not a stimulus was presented. This trial design was chosen to assure that participants directed their spatial attention during all precues to the stimulation site. Finally, participants reported by a right-hand index/middle finger button response whether they felt a stimulation during the Stimulation trials or whether or not their imagination was successful. Left/right–yes/no assignment was balanced across trials. During the 2-, 3-, 4-, 5-, or 6-s-long intertrial intervals (ITIs), a fixation cross was presented. Each experimental run comprised 48 trials presented in a random order: six trials per experimental condition (StimPress, StimFlutt, StimVibro, ImagPress, ImagFlutt, ImagVibro), complemented with six No-Stimulation trials (two per stimulus cue) and six unannounced Null events occurring as a longer ITI in which the fixation cross was presented 6 s longer. Six runs of 8 min were conducted.
Questionnaire assessment
Inspired by the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ) and its revised version (VVIQ-2; Marks, 1973, 1995), we developed the VTIQ to assess the participant’s ability for tactile imagery. As with the VVIQ, the VTIQ contains 16 items to determine the lifelikeness of an imagined sensation (see extended data for full questionnaire). The participants were asked to rate how precise, detailed, and similar to an actual sensation the imagery of different daily situations was. Responses were given on a 5-point scale. As in the VVIQ-2, high scores indicated a high lifelikeness of the mental imagery (1 = I don’t have any imagery, I only know that I’m thinking about the sensation described; 2 = Vague and blurry; 3 = Somewhat precise and vivid; 4 = Precise and vivid; 5 = Very precise and vivid, It feels like I’m actually perceiving the sensation described).
FMRI data acquisition and preprocessing
The data were acquired on a 3 T MRI scanner (TIM Trio, Siemens) equipped with a 32-channel head coil. The whole brain was scanned in 37 slices of ascending order. A run comprised 242 functional T2*-weighted images (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; matrix size = 64 × 64; flip angle = 70°; 3 × 3 × 3 mm3). Additionally, a T1-weighted image was acquired, comprising 176 sagittal slices (TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.52 ms; 1 × 1 × 1 mm3).
Data preprocessing was mainly done using SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute for Neurology, University College London, London, UK) running on MATLAB, version 2021b (MathWorks). Additionally, the toolboxes Rest (version 1.8; Song et al., 2011) and dPABI (Yan et al., 2016) were used. To quality check the data and correct for potential compromised slices, the ArtRepair-Toolbox for SPM (Mazaika et al., 2005) was utilized and run with a default threshold of 18, which refers to the degree of accepted noise per scan. Only for six participants an interpolation of slices was suggested and realized as implemented in the ArtRepair-toolbox. For each of them, ≤5% of slices were identified for interpolation, together speaking for an overall low amount of motion in the data. The further preprocessing comprised slice time correction; realignment; coregistration; normalization to MNI space using unified segmentation, including reslicing of the functional data to 2 × 2 × 2 mm3; smoothing with an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel; and detrending according to Macey et al. (2004).
FMRI data analysis and statistical assessment
Univariate analysis
Using SPM12, a first-level model was constructed, incorporating HRF-convolved regressors for each of the following six conditions: StimPress, StimFlutt, StimVibro, ImagPress, ImagFlutt, and ImagVibro. For the Imagery conditions, the regressors were modeled using only trials rated as successful. Therefore, six participants had to be excluded because of empty regressors in one or more runs. Please note that all participants had almost 100% performance in the stimulation condition, therefore no additional modeling of unsuccessful stimulation was necessary. To allow for a later conjunction analysis, Null events were randomly assigned to and modeled in two separate regressors. Three additional regressors of no interest were included, as follows: (1) the precue, (2) the remaining (unsuccessful) imagery trials, and (3) the button presses. Modeling the precue for every trial as a regressor of no interest was done to account for the influences of spatial attention, which could potentially drive activation during mental imagery. On the subject level (first-level analysis), contrasts against implicit baseline were calculated for the six experimental conditions and the two groups of Null events, which were then forwarded to group-level (second-level) analyses, modeled with flexible factorial design specification of SPM.
To test for activations associated with perceptual or mental imagery processing, we first contrasted the mean of the three Stimulation and the three Imagery conditions, respectively, against Null events. Furthermore, we computed a conjunction against the conjunction null hypothesis (Friston et al., 2005), conjoining the contrasts Stimulation > Null and Imagery > Null using the estimates of the two independent Null event regressors. To establish cytoarchitectonic references, we used the Anatomy Toolbox version 2.2 (Eickhoff et al., 2005).
Next, to test for differences in perceptual processing of the three stimulus types, we computed second-level contrasts for the six possible pairings of the three Stimulation conditions (StimPress, StimFlutt, StimVibro). The same was done for the three Imagery conditions (ImagPress, ImagFlutt, ImagVibro).
ROI definition
To examine content-related activity within the somatosensory processing stream, we defined regions of interest (ROIs) based on the probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps of the Anatomy Toolbox version 2.2 for contralateral S1 (BA3b, BA1, BA2) and bilateral S2 (OP1, OP2, OP3, OP4), with a 50% probability cutoff. To increase the specificity of our analysis to those aspects of S1 and S2, which are activated by our task, we intersected the anatomic masks with activation clusters resulting from the contrast Stimulation > Null. As univariate effects within the somatosensory processing stream only occurred in the three subregions of contralateral S1 as well as bilateral S2, we focused on these five resulting ROIs. For the visualization of ROIs, we used MRIcron version 2.1 (Rorden and Brett, 2000). Within these masks, we performed a decoding analysis to test for content-specific (Press, Flutt, Vibro) multivariate activation patterns.
Multivariate pattern analysis
To identify content-specific differences between the experimental conditions and to thus go beyond mere univariate BOLD activation differences, we used MVPA. All decoding analyses were performed using The Decoding Toolbox (Hebart et al., 2015), which allows the application of support vector machine (SVM) classification to neuroimaging data.
Except for smoothing and detrending, the same preprocessing steps as for the univariate analysis were applied to the data. For the decoding analysis, we used condition-specific β estimates from a first-level model with the same design as used for the univariate analyses. We performed two decoding analyses, where β estimates of the six conditions (StimPress, StimFlutt, StimVibro, ImagPress, ImagFlutt, and ImagVibro) were extracted (1) from the cortical clusters resulting from the (univariate) conjunction analysis, and (2) from the predefined ROIs. The β estimates were z-scaled (normalized) across the samples for each voxel to control for univariate effects and entered into the cross-validated classification schema. We used linear multiclass SVMs (libsvm) in a leave-one-run-out sixfold cross-validation scheme over the six runs, with the SVM trained on each possible selection of data from five runs and then tested on the data from the remaining sixth run. First, SVMs were trained and tested within the Stimulation and Imagination conditions, respectively (i.e., one decoding schema for StimPress, StimFlutt and StimVibro, and one decoding schema for ImagPress, ImagFlutt and ImagVibro). For a comprehensive display of the findings, we report results as “above chance decoding accuracy.” For all analyses, the chance level of the three-class SVM classification was 33.33%. Options for hyperparameter tuning as well as for feature and parameter selection were kept at default values, provided by the toolbox, to allow for comparisons with previous work and simplicity. The resulting confusion matrices of the different decoding analyses are provided in Extended Data Figure 4-1.
Next, we used the same leave-one-run-out sixfold cross-validation scheme to test for cross-classification (i.e., the classifier was trained on data from the Stimulation conditions and tested on the Imagery conditions, and vice versa). Results are presented analogously to the main decoding analysis.
As an additional control analysis, multiclass SVMs were trained and tested on all six conditions at once. For this decoding analysis, the chance level was 16.67%. As expected, the confusion matrices of the six-class decoding analyses show that Stimulation and Imagery conditions are clearly distinguishable (Extended Data Fig. 4-2).
To validate our results from the decoding analyses and account for potential non-normally distributed decoding accuracy values (Stelzer et al., 2013), we performed nonparametric permutation tests, as implemented in The Decoding Toolbox (Hebart et al., 2015), with 1000 iterations per ROI. For this purpose, a random permutation of the six labels (StimPress, StimFlutt, StimVibro, ImagPress, ImagFlutt, ImagVibro) was drawn for each fold of the leave-one-out cross-validation, keeping the classification parameters as default. After averaging over subjects, this resulted in five distributions (one for each ROI) of decoding-accuracy values with 1000 data points per ROI. These distributions were then used to calculate the probability of the occurrence of values of interest resulting from our decoding analysis, described above.
This was done by dividing the number of results for the ROI-matching reference set, which were of greater or equal value to the averaged results in question by the number of reference results of smaller value. The minimal possible p value was therefore approximately p = 0.001. This resulted in a p-value per ROI for Stimulation, Imagery, and cross-classification.
Data availability
The code to run the experiment and analyze the data can be found on GitHub (https://github.com/Neurocomputation-and-Neuroimaging-Unit/Tactile_Imagery_Content_in_SI). The fMRI and behavioral data can be shared by the corresponding author on request.
Results
Behavioral data
The response data of the N = 21 participants that were included for data analysis revealed a homogeneous performance regarding the Stimulation trials: 94.1% of the No-Stimulation trials were identified correctly (SEM = 0.02); and 97.1% of the Stimulation trials were reported as perceived (SEM = 0.01). The remaining 2.9% of responses to Stimulation trials were missed (SEM = 0.01). Figure 2 shows the performance (i.e., the percentage of trials rated as successful) in Imagery trials. No significant differences in Imagery success among the three types of to-be-imagined stimulations was found, when tested with a repeated-measures 1 × 3 ANOVA (F(2,377) = 0.59, p = 0.557).
To check for increasing or declining performance throughout the experiment (e.g., because of learning or tiredness), we computed a repeated-measures ANOVA over the three stimulus types with the six runs as a measure of time (3 × 6 factors). No significant main effect was found for the runs (F(5,300) = 0.66, p = 0.650) or interaction with stimulus type (F(10,300) = 0.51, p = 0.879).
The VTIQ was descriptively evaluated: 3.50 ± 0.59 (mean ± SD). For comparison, Campos and Pérez-Fabello (2009) report mean VVIQ-2 scores of 3.8 ± 0.6. The results of the VTIQ are therefore within an expected range compared with other tools assessing mental imagery.
To test whether VTIQ scores relate to the Imagery success in our paradigm, we correlated the VTIQ scores with participants mean success ratings. We found a significant correlation (r = 0.74, p < 0.001), indicating that the subjects’ self-assessment of their imaginative ability (VTIQ ratings) directly relates to the imagery performance report in our experimental paradigm.
fMRI data
Univariate analysis of stimulation and imagery
To locate sensory areas activated by the applied vibrotactile stimulation, we first averaged the three stimulation conditions (StimPress, StimFlutt, StimVibro) and computed the contrast Stimulation > Null at p < 0.05 FWE corrected (Fig. 3A). We found strong activation in the right (contralateral to stimulation) S1 and bilateral S2. Additional clusters had their peak voxel in the parietal and frontal cortices, as well as left temporal areas (Table 1, complete report).
To identify brain areas activated by tactile mental imagery, we averaged the three imagery conditions (ImagPress, ImagFlutt, ImagVibro) and computed the contrast Imagery > Null at p < 0.05 FWE corrected (Fig. 3B). This showed activation of the posterior medial frontal cortex including the SMA and various parietal areas (Table 1). Notably, this contrast revealed an activation cluster with maximum in right BA2, according to the Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005).
To test for activation shared by Stimulation and Imagery, we computed the conjunction of the contrasts Stimulation > Null and Imagery > Null, presented FWE cluster corrected at p < 0.05 in Figure 3C. Shared activations can be found in SMA, right insula, left IPL, as well as bilateral temporal areas (Table 1). In line with the previous contrasts, this analysis confirms that the same subregion in right BA2 is activated during Stimulation and Imagery. When intersecting the conjunction with the somatosensory probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps, 114 voxels are found in right BA2, but no activation in other ipsilateral or contralateral somatosensory cortices.
To test whether a univariate difference between the three stimulus types (Press, Flutter, Vibro) can be found in the somatosensory cortex, we investigated the BOLD activation within the somatosensory probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps for the contrasts of the three Stimulation conditions (StimPress, StimFlutt, StimVibro) against each other. None of the six contrasts of pairs of conditions revealed any significant differences at p < 0.05 (FWE corrected). The only contrast showing any activation differences within the somatosensory processing stream is the contrast StimVibro > StimPress, where two clusters of six voxels each can be found in contralateral S1 and S2, respectively [S1 peak: x = 44, y = −20, z = 54, t score = 3.71, z score = 3.63, p = 0.998 (FWE cluster corrected); S2 peak: x = 38, y = −30, z = 20, t-score = 3.73, z score = 3.65, p = 0.998 (FWE cluster corrected)]. Computing the same contrasts between stimulus types within the Imagery conditions (ImagPress, ImagFlutt, ImagVibro) revealed no significant activation differences.
Multivariate analysis of stimulation and imagery
Using MVPA, we tested whether activation patterns allow distinguishing between stimulus types (i.e., the vibration frequency of the stimulation as well as its respective mental imagery content). When performing multiclass decoding analyses using the five cortical clusters resulting from the conjunction analysis as regions of interest, we find that the Stimulation conditions can be distinguished in all five areas, demonstrated by significant decoding accuracy minus chance values (mean ± SEM; right BA2: 10.8 ± 2.4%, p = 0.001; left IPL: 11.4 ± 2.7%, p = 0.001; SMA: 7.7 ± 3.4%, p = 0.001; right insula: 4.5 ± 2.9%, p = 0.002; left temporal pole: 5.8 ± 2.3%, p = 0.001). In contrast, the decoding accuracy for the Imagery conditions was, as expected, considerably lower and only significantly above chance in the following three clusters: right BA2 (2.7 ± 3.0%, p = 0.031), left IPL (2.4 ± 2.7%, p = 0.047), and SMA (4.2 ± 2.3%, p = 0.003). Finally, for the cross-classification trained on the Stimulation conditions and tested on the Imagery conditions, only the following two clusters showed significant results: the right BA2 (4.2 ± 1.8%, p = 0.001) and left IPL (5.0 ± 2.2%, p = 0.001). Cross-classification trained on Imagery and tested on Stimulation conditions showed no significant results.
To further examine content-related activity patterns within the somatosensory processing stream, we used ROIs in contralateral S1 (BA3b, 60 voxels; BA1, 105 voxels; BA2, 157 voxels) and bilateral S2 (left S2, 181 voxels; right S2, 197 voxels), as derived from the intersection of the stimulation versus baseline contrast and anatomic masks from the Anatomy Toolbox (see Materials and Methods). For each ROI, the mean decoding accuracy minus chance values for Stimulation and Imagery are shown in Figure 4A. The multiclass decoding analyses within the Stimulation conditions revealed significant decoding results in all ROIs. When testing whether different imagery content leads to distinguishable activation patterns, we found significant decoding only in S1 subarea BA2 and contralateral S2. The cross-classification decoding analysis trained on the Stimulation conditions and tested on the Imagery conditions revealed accuracies significantly greater than chance in the same two ROIs. Corresponding confusion matrices can be found in Extended Data Figure 4-1.
Extended Data Figure 4-1
Confusion Matrices for multi-class decoding with three classes for each ROI, separate for the conditions Stimulation and Imagination, as well as for the cross-classification (xClass) trained on Stimulation and tested on Imagination. The percentage of assignment from each class to all classes is colour coded. Download Figure 4-1, DOCX file.
Extended Data Figure 4-2
Confusion Matrices for multi-class decoding with six classes for each ROI. The percentage of assignment from each class to all classes is colour coded. In general, fewer data is confused between conditions (Imagination vs. Stimulation), i.e. most classification errors are made between content of one condition. Download Figure 4-2, DOCX file.
Discussion
This study addressed whether primary somatosensory cortices contribute to the representation of mental content during tactile imagery. In our fMRI paradigm participants either perceived or imagined three different vibrotactile stimulus types (Press, Flutter, Vibro). With conventional univariate analyses, we observe that tactile perception and imagery share activation in contralateral BA2, the hierarchically highest subarea of S1, thus confirming previous findings (Schmidt and Blankenburg, 2019), but it was not possible to distinguish the different imagery conditions. Using MVPA, however, we were able to differentiate the resulting patterns of somatosensory cortical activation: for the Stimulation conditions within all subregions of the somatosensory processing stream, whereas for the Imagery conditions most consistently within contralateral BA2. Furthermore, a cross-classification with training on Stimulation and testing on Imagery conditions was possible. Thus, we were able to show that the BA2 activation induced by Imagery reflects tactile mental content, where a similar neuronal activation pattern as that during the perception of corresponding stimuli was found.
Perception and imagery of somatosensory stimuli
Both perception and mental imagery of vibrotactile stimuli showed expected fMRI activation in the univariate BOLD analysis. The processing of vibrotactile stimulation induces activation in contralateral S1 and bilateral S2, the typical evoked response for somatosensory stimulation (Francis et al., 2000; Nierhaus et al., 2009; Grund et al., 2021; Schröder et al., 2021). Tactile mental imagery activates a broad network of frontal and parietal regions, including the SMA, IFG, IPL, and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). This activation pattern, similar to the task-positive network, seems to be related to the general mental construction process, as it is activated irrespective of the mental content of imagery (Hassabis and Maguire, 2009; Zvyagintsev et al., 2013; Schmidt and Blankenburg, 2019). We additionally found an activation of the contralateral S1 during tactile mental imagery. The overlap of S1 activation revealed by the conjunction of Stimulation > Null and Imagery > Null emphasizes that perceptually activated areas are also recruited during mental imagery. According to the “sensory recruitment theory,” early sensory areas contribute to the mental representation of a sensory stimulation during mental imagery (Kosslyn et al., 2001; Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005; Serences, 2016). Our finding of activation in S1 during imagery replicates the results of Schmidt and Blankenburg (2019), where, similar to the study at hand, mental imagery of simple vibrotactile stimuli only activated the hierarchically highest S1 subarea BA2. This finding can be interpreted as further evidence for sensory recruitment where only very fine-grained, detailed mental imagery of high vividness activates lower-order sensory areas (Kosslyn and Thompson, 2003; Schmidt et al., 2014). In the somatosensory system, this was shown with mental imagery of more complex spatial information with cortical activation down to BA1 (Schmidt et al., 2014). In contrast, the mental imagery of abstract concepts or symbolic, language-based content does not require the recruitment of regions that are specialized in the processing of perceptual stimulus features (Lee et al., 2013; Lee and Baker, 2016; Christophel et al., 2017). In brief, our results so far show that tactile mental imagery (1) activates frontoparietal areas, whose general contribution to mental processes is well described; and (2) goes along with sensory recruitment of BA2 within S1.
Content-specific activation in primary somatosensory cortex
But does the observed activation of BA2 also relate to the representation of a specific mental content? While the presentation of different stimulus types led only to subtle activation differences for the StimVibro > StimPress contrast, univariate analyses did not reveal any activation differences during respective mental imagery. The applied types of vibrotactile stimulation (different content) were designed to all comprise 2 Hz half-sine wave pulses. For one stimulation type, the 2 Hz pulses were unmodulated (Press), the other two stimulation types were modulated by 30 or 150 Hz, corresponding to the Flutter and Vibration frequency range, respectively. Thus, we tried to minimize the physical and perceptual difference between the three different stimulus types and focus the participant’s perception on the applied frequency (Press, Flutter, Vibro). The perception of these different vibrotactile frequencies have been linked to different mechanoreceptors: slowly adapting Merkel cells sensitive to frequencies <10 Hz, rapidly adapting Meissner’s corpuscles responding most strongly to frequencies in the flutter range, and Pacinian corpuscles most sensitive to high-frequency vibrations >100 Hz (Harrington and Hunter Downs, 2001; Friedman et al., 2004). Vibrotactile stimulation in these different frequency ranges have previously been shown to elicit neural responses differentiable with noninvasive neuroimaging. Using EEG, evoked responses in S1 were found for 24 Hz stimulation, while 240 Hz stimulation evoked responses in both S1 and S2 (Hämäläinen et al., 1990). In an fMRI experiment, 150 Hz compared with 35 Hz stimulation showed stronger activation in posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and S2 (Harrington and Hunter Downs, 2001). Furthermore, using fMRI, the contralateral S1 was also shown to carry frequency-dependent information (Kim et al., 2016). Therefore, our finding, pointing toward a slightly stronger activation in contralateral S1 and S2 for the StimVibro condition, is well in line with the literature.
During mental tactile imagery, we observe activation in contralateral S1 (BA2), which is suggestive of content-specific activation, in the sense of sensory recruitment theory. However, we did not find univariate activation differences among the three Imagery conditions, namely the three types of mental contents. We used multivariate decoding analysis, as a more sensitive test for differences in the local voxel activation pattern. First, we tested for the possibility to classify the different stimulus types within the regions revealed by the conjunction analysis between Stimulation and Imagery condition. It was possible to classify the three different types of vibrotactile stimulation in all five regions, whereas the corresponding three Imagery types were decodable only from the SMA, left IPL, as well as right BA2 clusters. Strikingly, cross-classification, where the classifier was trained on the Stimulation and tested on the Imagery conditions, was possible only in left IPL and right BA2. These findings show content-specific activation during imagery within S1, in line with sensory recruitment theory. The fact that our analysis also showed sensitivity to the mental content in IPL and SMA might be related to content-specific contributions of the mental construction process, or alternatively, differences in the cognitive strategy to imagine the different stimulus types.
Building on this finding, we next wanted to test whether content-specific codes are specific to BA2 and whether other regions within the somatosensory processing stream also exhibit content-specific codes during mental imagery. To this end, we used ROIs derived from the Anatomy Toolbox, but limited to areas that were generally activated for Stimulation. Thus, we increased the specificity of our decoding results to those areas and subareas within S1/S2, which were activated by the vibrotactile stimulation. We found that for Stimulation, the three stimulation types can be classified throughout the somatosensory processing stream. Most interestingly, the three Imagery conditions can only be classified in contralateral BA2, the hierarchically highest subarea in S1, as well as in contralateral S2. Furthermore, above chance cross-classification was also possible in contralateral BA2 and S2. Therefore, our MVPA revealed content-specific activation during mental tactile imagery within the contralateral S1, which relies on similar patterns as activated by stimulation.
The neuronal foundations that underlie the differences in local activation patterns, however, remain speculative. Previous studies that decoded from S1 used stimuli with differentiable spatial layout (Schmidt et al., 2014); therefore, decodability was most likely based on fine-grained somatotopic organization. In the study at hand, spatial features were kept constant, but the stimuli were designed so as to activate different mechanoreceptors in the skin. Although it seems unlikely that either the different stimuli activate one receptor exclusively or that signals from different receptors activate strictly distinct neuronal populations in S1 (Saal et al., 2015), an activation of overall different patterns of neuronal populations can still be expected. However, since the spatial distribution of such populations is below the resolution of MRI, decoding is potentially based on hyperacuity (Boynton, 2005; Kriegeskorte et al., 2010), which again needs to be investigated in future studies with higher resolution.
Together, our results for the first time provide direct evidence that S1 activation during mental imagery is specific to a mental content. Moreover, the fact that mental content is represented in BA2 but not in hierarchically lower subregions of S1 supports the hypothesis of top-down sensory recruitment.
Mental imagery and working memory
As both mental imagery and working memory (WM) rely on internal mental representations, sensory recruitment is also discussed in the context of sensory working memory studies (Tong, 2013; Christophel et al., 2017). The main difference between the two paradigms lies in the proposed way of how a mental image comes into being: while the internal representations created in working memory tasks result from an externally initiated process, mental imagery is assumed to internally activate mental representations through a top-down driven processing (Mechelli et al., 2004; Stokes et al., 2009; Dijkstra et al., 2017). The evidence for sensory recruitment is mixed across WM studies (Serences, 2016; Christophel et al., 2017; Uluç et al., 2018; Wu et al., 2018), similar to what has been reported for mental imagery studies (Kosslyn and Thompson, 2003). For the visual modality, working memory and mental imagery have previously been found to rely on similar primary visual areas (Albers et al., 2013; Tong, 2013). Also, a series of tactile working memory decoding studies tested the cortices that retain information during delay phases. Schmidt et al. (2017) have recently shown that the frequency of a vibrotactile stimulus can be decoded from activation in right IFG, while information on the spatial layout of tactile stimuli can be decoded from the PPC (Schmidt and Blankenburg, 2018). Interestingly, Schmidt and Blankenburg (2018) found content-specific activity in S1 only in an early phase of a 12 s WM retention period. This implies that in WM the tactospatial information is initially represented in S1, but with a longer retention—and potentially a less vivid mental representation—it is rather hierarchically higher-order areas that exhibit content-specific activation. One interpretation of these findings further supports the idea, discussed above, that the extent of activation in S1 corresponds to the vividness of tactile mental imagery (Kosslyn and Thompson, 2003), while the mental representation loses this phenomenal character with longer retention and the mental image becomes rather abstract in its nature.
Mental imagery and attention
Another challenge for both working memory and mental imagery research, is to distinguish between attentional mechanisms and the representation of mental content. Conceptually, it is difficult to draw a clear-cut distinction between attentional mechanisms that support the maintenance of a specific mental content and the mental content representation as such (Schmidt et al., 2021). In our paradigm, we included No-Stimulation trials, where the participants were only directing their spatial attention to the stimulation site while they did not have to imagine a stimulus, nor was a stimulus applied. By modeling the precue for every trial as a regressor of no interest, we therefore tried to account for the influences of spatial attention concerning the time, place, and type of a stimulus, which might potentially drive activation during mental imagery. This makes it highly unlikely that our findings of activation during tactile mental imagery were driven by spatial attention. The conjoined activations we found for Stimulation and Imagery depict major components of the commonly reported task-positive network elicited by tactile imagery. This is in line with previous studies on auditory and visual mental imagery, which found activation in multimodal and various frontal areas, which are also associated with attentional processes (Zvyagintsev et al., 2013; Lückmann et al., 2014). Furthermore, a model containing neural correlates of selective internal attention during the prioritization of information in working memory tasks attributes activity in the pre-SMA, frontal-eye field, IPS and IPL to attentional processes (Myers et al., 2017), which is in line with our findings and with the suggestion that working memory and mental imagery share core processes (Tong, 2013). Finally, our finding that tactile imagery content can also be classified in IPL fits with the reported role of the parietal cortex in tactile attention (Goltz et al., 2015), suggesting that attentional mechanisms appear to be interwoven with the activation of a specific mental content.
Conclusion
We show that mental content representations of vibrotactile stimuli can be found in a specific activation pattern within the hierarchically highest subregion of S1, namely BA2. We further found that the activation in sensory cortices induced by tactile imagery relies on patterns similar to those activated by tactile stimulation. Our results are in line with the view of mental imagery as an internally driven process involving frontoparietal areas as well as sensory recruitment of primary somatosensory cortex.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This investigator-initiated study was partly supported by donations from private citizens. We acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Fund of the Freie Universität Berlin.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.