Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro
eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Cognition and Behavior

Brain Activation and Functional Connectivity of Reappraisal and Acceptance for Anxious Events

Masayuki Tsujimoto, Yutaka Matsuzaki, Noriki Yamaya and Ryuta Kawashima
eNeuro 23 May 2023, 10 (6) ENEURO.0033-23.2023; https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0033-23.2023
Masayuki Tsujimoto
1Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging, and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan 980-8575
2Graduate School of Medicine, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan 980-8575
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Masayuki Tsujimoto
Yutaka Matsuzaki
1Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging, and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan 980-8575
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Noriki Yamaya
1Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging, and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan 980-8575
2Graduate School of Medicine, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan 980-8575
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Noriki Yamaya
Ryuta Kawashima
1Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute of Development, Aging, and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan 980-8575
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Despite the significant health consequences of anxiety, the neural basis of regulation for personal anxious events is not well understood. We examined brain activity and functional connectivity during cognitive emotion regulation strategies (reappraisal and acceptance) for personal anxious events. Functional MRI (fMRI) data were obtained while 35 college students were thinking about (the control condition), reappraising, or accepting their own anxiety-provoking situations. Although reappraisal and acceptance decreased anxiety, no statistically significant differences were observed in the brain activation levels between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and the control condition. However, acceptance decreased activation in the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus more than reappraisal. Moreover, functional connectivity with the amygdala and ventral anterior insula distinguished the emotion regulation strategies for anxiety. Reappraisal showed stronger negative functional connectivity with the amygdala and cognitive control regions than other strategies. In addition, reappraisal had negative functional coupling between the ventral anterior insula and temporal pole compared with acceptance. In contrast, acceptance showed stronger positive functional coupling between the ventral anterior insula and precentral and postcentral gyrus compared with the control condition. Our findings contribute to the understanding of emotion regulation processes by revealing the brain activity and functional connectivity patterns in reappraisal and acceptance for personal anxious events.

  • acceptance
  • anxiety
  • emotion regulation
  • functional connectivity
  • neural basis
  • reappraisal

Significance Statement

This study is the first to reveal the differences in brain activity and functional connectivity between reappraisal and acceptance for individual anxious events. We found that reappraisal strengthened the negative functional coupling between the cognitive control areas and emotion-processing regions. In contrast, acceptance was characterized by a reduction in the self-reflection process and strengthened the functional coupling between emotion-processing regions and self-reflective and emotion recognition areas. These results contribute to a better understanding of emotion regulation processes by providing the differences in relationships of activation in self-reflection, cognitive control, and emotion-processing regions between reappraisal and acceptance for personal anxious events. These findings may help discover interventions for mitigating the negative effects of anxiety.

Introduction

Regulating anxiety is crucial because anxiety decreases psychological, physical, and work functioning (Kubzansky and Kawachi, 2000; Haller et al., 2014; Moran, 2016; Vancampfort et al., 2017). Understanding the processes of effective strategies for one’s anxiety helps to develop interventions aimed at reducing anxiety and understand the dysfunction of anxiety regulation. Therefore, this functional MRI (fMRI) study examined the neural basis of emotion regulation strategies for anxiety induced by the participants’ personal events.

Garnefski et al. (2001) have identified five adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, and especially reappraisal and acceptance are negatively related to psychopathology (Aldao et al., 2010). Reappraisal consists of reframing the self-relevant meaning of the emotion-provoking situation (Gross and John, 2003; Etkin et al., 2015), while acceptance involves experiencing emotions fully and being open to internal experiences without trying to change or avoid them (Hayes et al., 1999; Wolgast et al., 2011). In laboratory studies, both strategies have been shown to alleviate subjective negative emotions and adverse physiological effects (Hofmann et al., 2009; Wolgast et al., 2011; Keng et al., 2013; Troy et al., 2018).

Previous studies have demonstrated that reappraisal recruits cognitive control regions, such as the prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal lobule (Ochsner et al., 2012; Buhle et al., 2014; Frank et al., 2014; Morawetz et al., 2017) and involves the negative functional coupling between the prefrontal cortex and amygdala (Sarkheil et al., 2019; Berboth and Morawetz, 2021). Contrastingly, brain activity during acceptance has begun to be examined, but its functional connectivity has been less explored. Previous studies have found that acceptance decreases activation in the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex, suggesting that acceptance reduces self-reflection (Dixon et al., 2020; Messina et al., 2021), which is a process of reflecting on one’s own characteristics, abilities, and attitudes (Johnson et al., 2002). Additionally, Goldin et al. (2019) found that acceptance recruited less activation of cognitive control regions compared with that of reappraisal. Regarding functional connectivity, Kober et al. (2019) investigated the functional coupling of acceptance of pain and unpleasant images but did not find statistically significant functional coupling with the amygdala or insula compared with the natural reaction. To date, no study has compared the functional connectivity of acceptance with that of other adaptive strategies, such as reappraisal, which may help clarify the unique neural mechanisms of acceptance compared with others by focusing on differences between emotion regulation strategies.

Brain activity has been suggested to vary with the complexity of stimulus materials and the type of emotions (Kim and Hamann, 2007; Vytal and Hamann, 2010; Dörfel et al., 2014; Saarimäki et al., 2016; Tsujimoto et al., 2022). Several studies have investigated the neural bases of regulating emotions induced by personal events and discrete emotions (Fabiansson et al., 2012; Zeidan et al., 2014; Goldin et al., 2019). Increased activity of fear circuits, such as the amygdala and insula, has been found in anticipatory anxiety, and reappraisal in anticipatory anxiety was associated with increased activity in prefrontal regions and decreased amygdala activation (Yoshimura et al., 2014). Additionally, reappraisal in social anxiety autobiographical scripts was related to the negative functional coupling between the left amygdala and bilateral prefrontal cortex (Goldin et al., 2009). Furthermore, acceptance was associated with greater activation in the anterior cingulate cortex than thinking about worries in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (Ellard et al., 2017). However, the neural basis of acceptance of anxiety in healthy adults remains unclear, and no study has compared the neural bases of reappraisal and acceptance for individuals’ anxious events. Identifying neural mechanisms of emotion regulation that focus on discrete emotions and materials can contribute to a further understanding of emotion regulation processes.

The present study investigated the brain activation and functional connectivity of reappraisal and acceptance for anxious events. We hypothesized that reappraisal would be associated with higher activation of the cognitive control regions than other strategies, whereas acceptance would deactivate the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus. Regarding functional connectivity, we used the region of interest (ROI) of the amygdala and ventral anterior insula. The anterior insula is related to anxiety and interoception (Paulus and Stein, 2006; Terasawa et al., 2013), and particularly, the ventral anterior insula is strongly involved in emotional awareness and the experience of anxiety (Carlson et al., 2011; Denny et al., 2014). We expected that reappraisal would show stronger negative connectivity of cognitive control regions and anxiety-processing regions than thinking about anxious events. Finally, we hypothesized that acceptance would have stronger functional connectivity between the regions related to self-reflection and anxiety-processing than other strategies.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Human participants were recruited at Tohoku University, Japan. Forty-one healthy college students participated in an fMRI study of emotion regulation and executive control (this paper reports only the data on emotion regulation). According to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory – Short Form (Veale, 2014), all participants were right-handed (mean: 92.44 ± 11.59). Participants who presently had or reported pregnancy, claustrophobia, history of mental disorders, metallic implants in the body, or medication use that affects cognitive function were excluded. In addition, we excluded the data from three participants who fell asleep during the task, two participants whose images were not transferred successfully from the MRI console to the server, and one participant with an average framewise displacement (Power et al., 2012) >0.3 mm. Therefore, the data from 35 participants (13 females, mean age: 20.6 ± 1.8, range: 18–25 years) were analyzed. The experiment was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Smart-Ageing Research Center of Tohoku University.

Procedure

The individuals participated in this experiment for three consecutive days. They reported keywords of three unresolved anxiety-inducing situations (Keng et al., 2013; Shields et al., 2016), which were used as stimuli for emotion regulation tasks involving reappraisal, acceptance, and the control conditions ∼3 d before participating in the experiment. On the first day, the participants were briefed about the experiment, provided informed consent, received instructions and examples of an emotion regulation strategy, and practiced the tasks under one of the three conditions. The instructions were developed based on previous studies (Keng et al., 2013; Noguchi et al., 2017). Reappraisal involved changing one’s thoughts and interpretations of anxiety-inducing situations. Acceptance meant accepting one’s emotions, thoughts, and body sensations, even if they were uncomfortable, without trying to change, control, or avoid them. In the control condition, participants thought about their unresolved anxiety-inducing situations. The emotion regulation task was practiced during two trials. After practicing, they worked on the first cognitive task, the emotion regulation task with the instructed strategy, and the second cognitive task in an MRI scanner. After the scan, they were asked to rate how well they could implement the strategy during an emotion regulation task (1, not at all; 7, very successfully) outside the scanner. This value was used to indicate whether the emotion regulation strategies could be applied rather than whether anxiety was reduced. On the second and third days, the participants practiced and performed the tasks in the same manner as that on the first day. For the emotion regulation task, the participants used a different emotion regulation strategy than on the previous day. On the second day, they answered several questionnaires after completing the tasks (not reported in this article). The participants received JPY 3000 for their participation in the study.

Emotion regulation task

We used reappraisal, acceptance, and control as the three conditions. Each day’s emotion regulation task consisted of 12 trials in one of the three conditions. During each trial, a fixation cross was presented for 2 s, followed by 30 s of an instructed strategy and an emotion-inducing keyword, during which participants engaged in emotion regulation. The keywords were those reported by the participants in advance (described above, Procedure), and each keyword was displayed four times on each day in a random order. The participants then rated the degree of anxiety they felt at that moment on a seven-point Likert scale, with 1 representing “not at all” and 7 representing “very much” by pressing one of seven response keys. At the end of the trial, a “rest” message was displayed, and the participants were instructed to rest for 12 s (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic of one trial. Each trial included a fixation cross for 2 s, an emotion regulation phase for 30 s, a rating phase for 6 s, and a rest phase for 12 s.

MRI data acquisition and imaging parameters

Image acquisition was performed with a 3 T MRI scanner (Philips Achieva dStream 3.0T). fMRI data were acquired with T2-weighted gradient echo-planar imaging. Specifically, 32 slices of gradient-echo images (echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 80°, thickness = 3 mm, field of view = 192 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, slice gap = 0.5 mm, and voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm) were acquired with a repetition time of 2000 ms. The T1-weighted anatomic images (thickness = 1 mm, field of view = 240 mm, matrix = 240 × 240) were acquired using magnetization-prepared, rapid-acquisition gradient-echo at the end of the third experimental day.

Analysis

Behavioral data analysis

To evaluate whether the participants were able to use the strategy, the successful implementation of reappraisal and acceptance was compared using a t test. One-way repeated measures analysis of variance was used in anovakun version 4.8.5 to analyze differences in anxiety reduction of the cognitive emotion regulation conditions (reappraisal and acceptance) and the control condition. Independent variables were the conditions, and dependent variables were the mean of anxiety ratings for each condition per participant. Multiple comparisons were performed by Shaffer’s modified sequential rejection Bonferroni procedure. All behavioral data analyses were performed using R version 4.0.0 (R Core Team; https://www.R-project.org/).

fMRI data preprocessing

Image preprocessing was performed using SPM 12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) and MATLAB R2020b (MathWorks). Preprocessing included correction for realignment, slice timing correction, co-registration, segmentation of structural images, spatial normalization using the anatomic image and the Montreal Neurologic Institute template, and smoothing using a Gaussian kernel with full-width at a half-maximum of 8 mm.

Whole-brain analysis

Statistical fMRI analyses were performed using SPM12. A general linear model was specified for each participant to model the BOLD response using a canonical hemodynamic response function at the individual level. The duration of emotion regulation (30 s) and six movement parameters were entered as regression variables. At the group-level, paired t tests were conducted with contrasts: reappraisal > control, acceptance > control, and reappraisal > acceptance, as well as the inverse contrasts. The results of the group-level analysis were assessed at a threshold of p < 0.001 at the voxel level and p < 0.05 with false discovery rate (FDR) correction at the cluster level. Regions were labeled using the SPM Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005).

Functional connectivity analysis

Functional connectivity analysis was performed using the CONN toolbox, version 20.b (www.nitrc.org/projects/conn, RRID: SCR_009550; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012) to investigate how the time course of the anxiety processing regions, such as the amygdala (Ellard et al., 2017; Babaev et al., 2018) and ventral anterior insula (Paulus and Stein, 2006; Carlson et al., 2011; Denny et al., 2014) related to other brain regions. The ROI of the left and right amygdala was defined using the Wake Forest University PickAtlas (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/wfu_pickatlas/; Maldjian et al., 2003) with the automatic anatomic labeling atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Additionally, we obtained the predefined ROI of the left and right ventral anterior insula directly from a previous study (Deen et al., 2011), in which the insular lobe was divided into three subregions by clustering of functional connectivity patterns. The functional data preprocessing consisted of realignment and unwrapping, slice timing correction, outlier detection with ART, segmentation and normalization, and smoothing. Segmentation and normalization were used as preprocessing for the structural data. Parameters for realignment and scrubbing were entered as first-level covariates. MRI data for 30 s × 12 trials of emotion regulation were used for analysis. ROI-to-voxel analyses for each participant were conducted with the ROI of the left and right amygdala and ventral anterior insula. At the group level, paired t tests were conducted to compare the functional connectivity of the anxiety processing regions with other regions among the conditions (reappraisal > control, acceptance > control, reappraisal > acceptance, and the inverse relationships). The results were evaluated at a threshold of p < 0.001 at the voxel level and p < 0.05 at the cluster level with FDR correction.

Data and code accessibility

The data and code described in the paper are freely available online at https://osf.io/nyrws/. The code is available as Extended Data 1.

Results

Behavioral results

To confirm whether the participants were able to use the strategy, the degree of perceived success of emotion regulation for each participant was collected outside the MRI scanner immediately after the task was completed. No statistically significant difference was observed between the successful implementation of reappraisal and that of acceptance [reappraisal: 5.26 ± 1.31, acceptance: 5.31 ± 1.23, t(34) = −0.18, p = 0.86, 95% confidence interval (CI) [−0.691–0.577], Cohen’s d = −0.04].

We compared the effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and the control condition on reducing anxiety. There was a statistically significant main effect of the conditions (F(2,68) = 31.18, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.28). The post hoc t tests indicated a greater reduction in anxiety for reappraisal (mean = 3.15 ± 0.84, t(34) = 7.86, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−1.622–−1.121], d = −1.56) and acceptance (mean = 3.59 ± 1.01, t(34) = 4.92, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−1.205–−0.661], d = −0.97) than for the control condition (mean = 4.53 ± 0.91; Fig. 2). Compared with acceptance, reappraisal was more effective in reducing anxiety (t(34) = 2.61, p < 0.05, 95% CI [−0.680–−0.198], d = −0.47).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Subjective rating of anxiety. ***p < 0.001, *p < 0.05. The p-values were corrected by Shaffer’s modified sequential rejection Bonferroni procedure. Error bars = SEM.

Brain activation

Table 1 shows the results of the comparison of brain activation under various conditions. No voxels with significantly increased or decreased activity survived in reappraisal or acceptance compared with the control condition. Reappraisal increased activation in the bilateral posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus significantly more than acceptance (Fig. 3; Table 1). No significantly increased activation was observed during acceptance compared with reappraisal.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Results of whole-brain analysis

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Whole-brain analysis for reappraisal > acceptance. Threshold: p < 0.001 uncorrected at the voxel level and p < 0.05 with FDR correction at the cluster level. FDR = false discovery rate.

Functional connectivity

Figures 4 and 5 show the results of the functional connectivity analysis.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Functional connectivity between the left amygdala and other brain regions for (a) reappraisal > control, (b) control > reappraisal, (c) acceptance > control, (d) reappraisal > acceptance, (e) acceptance > reappraisal, and between the right amygdala and other brain regions for (f) control > reappraisal, (g) reappraisal > acceptance, (h) acceptance > reappraisal. Threshold: p < 0.001 uncorrected at the voxel level and p < 0.05 with FDR correction at the cluster level. FDR = false discovery rate.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Functional connectivity between the left ventral anterior insula and other brain regions for (a) acceptance > reappraisal and between the right ventral anterior insula and other brain regions for (b) acceptance > control. Threshold: p < 0.001 uncorrected at the voxel level and p < 0.05 with FDR correction at the cluster level. FDR = false discovery rate.

Functional connectivity with the amygdala

Reappraisal showed significantly positive correlations with the left amygdala in the bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, left thalamus proper, hippocampus, and middle cingulate cortex compared with the control condition (Table 2). Significantly more negative correlations were found between the left amygdala and the left inferior frontal gyrus during reappraisal compared with the control condition. During acceptance, significantly more positive correlations were observed between the left amygdala and left caudate compared with the control. However, no significant negative correlations were observed in acceptance compared with the control condition.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

Results of functional connectivity analysis of the left amygdala

Compared with acceptance, reappraisal exhibited significantly positive functional connectivity in the right fusiform gyrus and negative functional connectivity in the bilateral thalamus proper, left anterior and middle cingulate cortex with the left amygdala.

Additionally, significant negative correlations were observed between the right amygdala and bilateral supramarginal gyrus, left precentral gyrus, and middle and inferior frontal gyrus during reappraisal compared with the control condition (Table 3). We did not find significant functional coupling with the inverse contrasts. Moreover, acceptance did not show significant functional connectivity in any regions with the right amygdala compared with the control condition.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Results of functional connectivity analysis of the right amygdala

During reappraisal compared with acceptance, significant positive correlations were observed between the right amygdala and right frontal pole and medial orbital gyrus. Additionally, reappraisal showed negative functional connectivity between the right amygdala and right precuneus, supramarginal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, and bilateral anterior and middle cingulate cortex.

Functional connectivity with the ventral anterior insula

Negative functional coupling with the left ventral anterior insula emerged in the bilateral temporal pole in reappraisal compared with acceptance (Table 4). However, no significant positive correlations were observed in the inverse contrasts. Additionally, compared with the control condition, reappraisal and acceptance did not show significant functional coupling with the left ventral anterior insula.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Results of functional connectivity analysis of the left ventral anterior insula

Compared with the control condition, acceptance showed significant positive correlations between the right ventral anterior insula and bilateral precentral gyrus and right postcentral gyrus (Table 5). No significant negative functional coupling with the right ventral anterior insula was observed in acceptance compared with the control. Finally, no significant functional connectivity with the right ventral anterior insula was observed in reappraisal compared with the other conditions.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 5.

Results of functional connectivity analysis of the right ventral anterior insula

Discussion

The present study is the first to directly compare the neural bases of reappraisal and acceptance for anxious events. Reappraisal was the most effective way to reduce anxiety, which is consistent with the findings of a previous study (Hofmann et al., 2009). Contrary to expectations, no statistically significant differences were observed in the brain activation levels between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and the control condition. However, acceptance decreased activation in the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus more than reappraisal. Moreover, functional coupling with the amygdala and ventral anterior insula distinguished the emotion regulation strategies for anxiety. Reappraisal showed strong negative functional connectivity between the amygdala and cognitive control regions, such as the middle and inferior frontal gyrus and supramarginal gyrus.

With respect to behavioral data, reappraisal and acceptance significantly decreased anxiety compared with the control condition. This result is consistent with previous research showing the effectiveness of these emotion regulation strategies in decreasing negative emotions (Hofmann et al., 2009; Wolgast et al., 2011; Troy et al., 2018; Goldin et al., 2019) and indicates that reappraisal and acceptance are also effective for an individual’s anxious events.

Contrary to the hypothesis that levels of activation in the regions would differ depending on the emotion regulation strategies, no significant difference was observed in brain activity levels of reappraisal and acceptance compared with those of the control condition. Our results reflect the possibility that brain activity does not differ between continuing to think about emotion-inducing personal events and cognitive emotion regulation strategies. This possibility was partially supported by a study comparing brain activity during reappraisal and rumination in personal anger, which found no difference in brain activity levels (Fabiansson et al., 2012).

Reappraisal showed greater activation of the bilateral posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus, which are related to self-reflective processes (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Cavanna, 2007; Brewer et al., 2013) than acceptance. Therefore, reappraisal may strengthen self-relevant information, whereas acceptance may weaken self-reflective information. Additionally, a previous study suggested that increased posterior cingulate cortex activity is involved in effortful cognitive control (Garrison et al., 2013), and our results may reflect the differences in effortful cognitive load manifested in the differences in brain activity during reappraisal and acceptance.

Consistent with our hypothesis, differences in functional connectivity with the amygdala were observed between reappraisal and the control condition. Reappraisal exhibited stronger negative correlations between the right amygdala and the middle and inferior frontal gyrus and supramarginal gyrus compared with the control. The amygdala is related to the evaluation of anxiety (Ellard et al., 2017; Babaev et al., 2018), and the right amygdala, in particular, is associated with fast and automatic responses to stimuli (Costafreda et al., 2008). The middle and inferior frontal gyrus and supramarginal gyrus play important roles in cognitive control, such as selective attention, working memory, and inhibition (Ochsner et al., 2012; Barbey et al., 2013). Moreover, our results are consistent with the findings of a previous meta-analysis (Berboth and Morawetz, 2021), and suggest that on reappraisal, the cognitive control areas regulate amygdala activity to achieve reduction of personal anxiety, especially modulation of the initial emotional changes caused by personal anxious events.

Furthermore, reappraisal showed positive functional connectivity between the left amygdala and the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, in addition to negative functional coupling between the left amygdala and the inferior frontal gyrus compared with the control condition. The left amygdala is engaged in language or delayed, evaluative responses (Costafreda et al., 2008). Prior studies have shown reduced activity in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex because of emotion regulation (Kanske et al., 2011), and these regions were co-activated during the perception of negative stimuli (Pezawas et al., 2005). Thus, reappraisal may alter the broader emotion-processing system.

In contrast, acceptance showed positive functional connectivity between the left amygdala and the caudate compared with the control condition. The caudate is involved in the perception of emotional information (Delgado et al., 2004; Kemp et al., 2013), suggesting that acceptance may alter the perception of anxiety. Furthermore, reappraisal showed negative functional coupling between the amygdala and middle frontal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, precuneus, and anterior and middle cingulate cortex compared with acceptance. Thus, the involvement of self-reflective processes may differ between reappraisal and acceptance, and reappraisal controls amygdala activity more through cognitive control compared with acceptance.

Acceptance showed stronger positive functional coupling between the ventral anterior insula and precentral gyrus and postcentral gyrus compared with the control condition. The ventral anterior insula is involved in anxiety, interoceptive sense, and emotional anticipation (Paulus and Stein, 2006; Berntson et al., 2011; Carlson et al., 2011; Alvarez et al., 2015). The postcentral gyrus, together with the anterior insula, is related to interoceptive sense (Craig, 2002; Min et al., 2022); and the precentral gyrus is known to be involved in self-image (Théoret et al., 2004) and voluntary motor control (Banker and Tadi, 2019). Therefore, in acceptance, the emotional anticipation and self-reflective processes are likely synchronized. In contrast, reappraisal had negative functional coupling between the ventral anterior insula and the temporal pole compared with acceptance. The temporal pole is implicated in a variety of functions, including emotional and social behavior and semantic processing. In particular, this region has been suggested to play a role in linking complex processed perceptual inputs to visceral emotional responses (Olson et al., 2007; Córcoles-Parada et al., 2019). Thus, our results may reflect differences in the complexity of emotional processes between reappraisal and acceptance.

This study had several limitations. First, because this study limited the target population to college students, further investigation in different age groups is needed to generalize the results (Allard and Kensinger, 2014; Mather, 2016). Second, the proportion of female participants in this study was low. Third, we did not use physiological measures for anxious reactions. The use of physiological measures would allow us to objectively assess the effectiveness of anxiety regulation. Finally, in this study, the cognitive task was performed before the emotion regulation task, which may have affected brain activity during emotion regulation despite the control between conditions.

Nevertheless, this study contributes to a better understanding of emotion regulation processes by providing information on brain activity and functional connectivity in reappraisal and acceptance for personal anxious events. Reappraisal strengthened the negative functional coupling between the cognitive control areas and emotion-processing regions, while acceptance was characterized by a reduction in the self-reflection process. Our findings will help determine interventions aimed at mitigating the negative effects of anxiety.

Extended Data 1

Scripts for the analyses used in the paper. Download Extended Data 1, ZIP file.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: We thank Editage for English language editing.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This work was supported by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology/Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, World-leading Innovative & Smart Education Program: Advanced Graduate Program for Future Medicine and Health Care, Tohoku University and the Japan Science and Technology Agency, Support for Pioneering Research Initiated by the Next Generation, Grant Number JPMJSP2114.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S (2010) Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 30:217–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 pmid:20015584
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Allard ES, Kensinger EA (2014) Age-related differences in neural recruitment during the use of cognitive reappraisal and selective attention as emotion regulation strategies. Front Psychol 5:296. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00296 pmid:24782800
    OpenUrlPubMed
  3. ↵
    Alvarez RP, Kirlic N, Misaki M, Bodurka J, Rhudy JL, Paulus MP, Drevets WC (2015) Increased anterior insula activity in anxious individuals is linked to diminished perceived control. Transl Psychiatry 5:e591. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.84 pmid:26125154
    OpenUrlPubMed
  4. ↵
    Babaev O, Piletti Chatain C, Krueger-Burg D (2018) Inhibition in the amygdala anxiety circuitry. Exp Mol Med 50:1–16. https://doi.org/10.1038/s12276-018-0063-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Banker L, Tadi P (2019) Neuroanatomy, precentral gyrus. StatPearls Publishing LLC. Available at https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK544218/.
  6. ↵
    Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J (2013) Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex 49:1195–1205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022 pmid:22789779
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Berboth S, Morawetz C (2021) Amygdala-prefrontal connectivity during emotion regulation: a meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Neuropsychologia 153:107767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107767 pmid:33516732
    OpenUrlPubMed
  8. ↵
    Berntson GG, Norman GJ, Bechara A, Bruss J, Tranel D, Cacioppo JT (2011) The insula and evaluative processes. Psychol Sci 22:80–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610391097 pmid:21148459
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Brewer JA, Garrison KA, Whitfield-Gabrieli S (2013) What about the “self” is processed in the posterior cingulate cortex? Front Hum Neurosci 7:647. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00647 pmid:24106472
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. ↵
    Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wage TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, Webe J, Ochsner KN (2014) Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 24:2981–2990. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht154 pmid:23765157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Carlson JM, Greenberg T, Rubin D, Mujica-Parodi LR (2011) Feeling anxious: anticipatory amygdalo-insular response predicts the feeling of anxious anticipation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 6:74–81. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq017 pmid:20207692
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Cavanna AE (2007) The precuneus and consciousness. CNS Spectr 12:545–552. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1092852900021295 pmid:17603406
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129:564–583. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl004 pmid:16399806
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Córcoles-Parada M, Ubero-Martínez M, Morris RGM, Insausti R, Mishkin M, Muñoz-López M (2019) Frontal and insular input to the dorsolateral temporal pole in primates: implications for auditory memory. Front Neurosci 13:1099. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.01099 pmid:31780878
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. ↵
    Costafreda SG, Brammer MJ, David AS, Fu CHY (2008) Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies. Brain Res Rev 58:57–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.012 pmid:18076995
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Craig AD (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:655–666. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn894 pmid:12154366
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Deen B, Pitskel NB, Pelphrey KA (2011) Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster analysis. Cereb Cortex 21:1498–1506. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq186 pmid:21097516
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Delgado MR, Stenger VA, Fiez JA (2004) Motivation-dependent responses in the human caudate nucleus. Cereb Cortex 14:1022–1030. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh062 pmid:15115748
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Denny BT, Ochsner KN, Weber J, Wager TD (2014) Anticipatory brain activity predicts the success or failure of subsequent emotion regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:403–411. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss148 pmid:23202664
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Dixon ML, Moodie CA, Goldin PR, Farb N, Heimberg RG, Gross JJ (2020) Emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder: reappraisal and acceptance of negative self-beliefs. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 5:119–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.07.009 pmid:31547972
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. ↵
    Dörfel D, Lamke JP, Hummel F, Wagner U, Erk S, Walter H (2014) Common and differential neural networks of emotion regulation by detachment, reinterpretation, distraction, and expressive suppression: a comparative fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 101:298–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.051 pmid:24993897
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K, Zilles K (2005) A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25:1325–1335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034 pmid:15850749
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Ellard KK, Barlow DH, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Gabrieli JDE, Deckersbach T (2017) Neural correlates of emotion acceptance vs worry or suppression in generalized anxiety disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 12:1009–1021. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx025 pmid:28402571
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    Etkin A, Büchel C, Gross JJ (2015) The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat Rev Neurosci 16:693–700. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4044 pmid:26481098
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Fabiansson EC, Denson TF, Moulds ML, Grisham JR, Schira MM (2012) Don’t look back in anger: neural correlates of reappraisal, analytical rumination, and angry rumination during recall of an anger-inducing autobiographical memory. Neuroimage 59:2974–2981. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.078 pmid:22015853
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Frank DW, Dewitt M, Hudgens-Haney M, Schaeffer DJ, Ball BH, Schwarz NF, Hussein AA, Smart LM, Sabatinelli D (2014) Emotion regulation: quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 45:202–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.010 pmid:24984244
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P (2001) Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Pers Individ Dif 30:1311–1327. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    Garrison KA, Santoyo JF, Davis JH, Thornhill IT, Kerr CE, Brewer JA (2013) Effortless awareness: using real time neurofeedback to investigate correlates of posterior cingulate cortex activity in meditators’ self-report. Front Hum Neurosci 7:440. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00440 pmid:23964222
    OpenUrlPubMed
  29. ↵
    Goldin PR, Manber-Ball T, Werner K, Heimberg R, Gross JJ (2009) Neural mechanisms of cognitive reappraisal of negative self-beliefs in social anxiety disorder. Biol Psychiatry 66:1091–1099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.014 pmid:19717138
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Goldin PR, Moodie CA, Gross JJ (2019) Acceptance versus reappraisal: behavioral, autonomic, and neural effects. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 19:927–944. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00690-7 pmid:30656602
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    Gross JJ, John OP (2003) Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:348–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 pmid:12916575
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Haller H, Cramer H, Lauche R, Gass F, Dobos GJ (2014) The prevalence and burden of subthreshold generalized anxiety disorder: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 14:128. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-14-128
    OpenUrlPubMed
  33. ↵
    Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG (1999) Acceptance and commitment therapy. New York: Guilford Press.
  34. ↵
    Hofmann SG, Heering S, Sawyer AT, Asnaani A (2009) How to handle anxiety: the effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal. Behav Res Ther 47:389–394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010 pmid:19281966
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Johnson SC, Baxter LC, Wilder LS, Pipe JG, Heiserman JE, Prigatano GP (2002) Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain 125:1808–1814. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf181 pmid:12135971
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Kanske P, Heissler J, Schönfelder S, Bongers A, Wessa M (2011) How to regulate emotion? Neural networks for reappraisal and distraction. Cereb Cortex 21:1379–1388. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq216 pmid:21041200
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Kemp J, Berthel MC, Dufour A, Després O, Henry A, Namer IJ, Musacchio M, Sellal F (2013) Caudate nucleus and social cognition: neuropsychological and SPECT evidence from a patient with focal caudate lesion. Cortex 49:559–571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.01.004 pmid:22325164
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Keng SL, Robins CJ, Smoski MJ, Dagenbach J, Leary MR (2013) Reappraisal and mindfulness: s comparison of subjective effects and cognitive costs. Behav Res Ther 51:899–904. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.10.006 pmid:24225174
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Kim SH, Hamann S (2007) Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. J Cogn Neurosci 19:776–798. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.776 pmid:17488204
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Kober H, Buhle J, Weber J, Ochsner KN, Wager TD (2019) Let it be: mindful acceptance down-regulates pain and negative emotion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 14:1147–1158. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsz104 pmid:31989171
    OpenUrlPubMed
  41. ↵
    Kubzansky LD, Kawachi I (2000) Going to the heart of the matter: do negative emotions cause coronary heart disease? J Psychosom Res 48:323–337. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00091-4 pmid:10880655
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH (2003) An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19:1233–1239. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1 pmid:12880848
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Mather M (2016) The affective neuroscience of aging. Annu Rev Psychol 67:213–238. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033540 pmid:26436717
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Messina I, Grecucci A, Viviani R (2021) Neurobiological models of emotion regulation: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of acceptance as an emotion regulation strategy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 16:257–267. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab007 pmid:33475715
    OpenUrlPubMed
  45. ↵
    Min J, Nashiro K, Yoo HJ, Cho C, Nasseri P, Bachman SL, Porat S, Thayer JF, Chang C, Lee TH, Mather M (2022) Emotion downregulation targets interoceptive brain regions while emotion upregulation targets other affective brain regions. J Neurosci 42:2973–2985. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1865-21.2022 pmid:35193926
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    Moran TP (2016) Anxiety and working memory capacity: a meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychol Bull 142:831–864. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000051 pmid:26963369
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Morawetz C, Bode S, Derntl B, Heekeren HR (2017) The effect of strategies, goals and stimulus material on the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 72:111–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.014 pmid:27894828
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Noguchi R, Sekizawa Y, So M, Yamaguchi S, Shimizu E (2017) Effects of five-minute internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy and simplified emotion-focused mindfulness on depressive symptoms: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry 17:85. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1248-8
    OpenUrl
  49. ↵
    Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT (2012) Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1251:E1–E24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x pmid:23025352
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y (2007) The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 130:1718–1731. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm052 pmid:17392317
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Paulus MP, Stein MB (2006) An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 60:383–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042 pmid:16780813
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR (2005) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat Neurosci 8:828–834. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1463 pmid:15880108
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59:2142–2154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018 pmid:22019881
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Saarimäki H, Gotsopoulos A, Jääskeläinen IP, Lampinen J, Vuilleumier P, Hari R, Sams M, Nummenmaa L (2016) Discrete neural signatures of basic emotions. Cereb Cortex 26:2563–2573. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv086 pmid:25924952
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Sarkheil P, Klasen M, Schneider F, Goebel R, Mathiak K (2019) Amygdala response and functional connectivity during cognitive emotion regulation of aversive image sequences. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 269:803–811. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-018-0920-4 pmid:30008118
    OpenUrlPubMed
  56. ↵
    Shields GS, Moons WG, Tewell CA, Yonelinas AP (2016) The effect of negative affect on cognition: anxiety, not anger, impairs executive function. Emotion 16:792–797. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000151 pmid:27100367
    OpenUrlPubMed
  57. ↵
    Terasawa Y, Shibata M, Moriguchi Y, Umeda S (2013) Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:259–266. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss108 pmid:22977199
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Théoret H, Kobayashi M, Merabet L, Wagner T, Tormos JM, Pascual-Leone A (2004) Modulation of right motor cortex excitability without awareness following presentation of masked self-images. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 20:54–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.01.003 pmid:15130589
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Troy AS, Shallcross AJ, Brunner A, Friedman R, Jones MC (2018) Cognitive reappraisal and acceptance: affects on emotion, physiology, and perceived cognitive costs. Emotion 18:58–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000371 pmid:29154585
    OpenUrlPubMed
  60. ↵
    Tsujimoto M, Saito T, Matsuzaki Y, Kojima R, Kawashima R (2022) Common and distinct neural bases of multiple positive emotion regulation strategies: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage 257:119334.
    OpenUrl
  61. ↵
    Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, Joliot M (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15:273–289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 pmid:11771995
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Vancampfort D, Koyanagi A, Hallgren M, Probst M, Stubbs B (2017) The relationship between chronic physical conditions, multimorbidity and anxiety in the general population: a global perspective across 42 countries. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 45:1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2016.11.002 pmid:28274332
    OpenUrlPubMed
  63. ↵
    Veale JF (2014) Edinburgh Handedness Inventory – Short Form: a revised version based on confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality 19:164–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045 pmid:23659650
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Vytal K, Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: a voxel-based meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 22:2864–2885. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21366 pmid:19929758
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A (2012) Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect 2:125–141. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2012.0073 pmid:22642651
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    Wolgast M, Lundh LG, Viborg G (2011) Cognitive reappraisal and acceptance: an experimental comparison of two emotion regulation strategies. Behav Res Ther 49:858–866. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.011 pmid:21999941
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Yoshimura S, Okamoto Y, Yoshino A, Kobayakawa M, Machino A, Yamawaki S (2014) Neural basis of anticipatory anxiety reappraisals. PLoS One 9:e102836. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102836 pmid:25048028
    OpenUrlPubMed
  68. ↵
    Zeidan F, Martucci KT, Kraft RA, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC (2014) Neural correlates of mindfulness meditation-related anxiety relief. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:751–759. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst041 pmid:23615765
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Alexander Soutschek, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat Munchen

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: NONE.

1) The relationship between the aim of the study (“In light of the negative effects described above, this functional MRI (fMRI) study examined the neural basis of emotion regulation strategies for anxiety induced by the participants’ personal events”) and the negative effects of anxiety described in the first lines of the introduction is not clear. If the aim of the study is solely descriptive (neural basis of emotion regulation strategies), this has to be clarified.

In other words, the question is whether the present study helps reducing/managing anxiety negative effects in some ways? If yes, please specify how.

2) It remains quite obscure what is actually missing in the literature regarding acceptance (lines 66-69). Please specify the gap in the literature, referring to the meta-analysis previously cited (line 62, Messina et al.).

View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 10 (6)
eNeuro
Vol. 10, Issue 6
June 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Brain Activation and Functional Connectivity of Reappraisal and Acceptance for Anxious Events
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Brain Activation and Functional Connectivity of Reappraisal and Acceptance for Anxious Events
Masayuki Tsujimoto, Yutaka Matsuzaki, Noriki Yamaya, Ryuta Kawashima
eNeuro 23 May 2023, 10 (6) ENEURO.0033-23.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0033-23.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Brain Activation and Functional Connectivity of Reappraisal and Acceptance for Anxious Events
Masayuki Tsujimoto, Yutaka Matsuzaki, Noriki Yamaya, Ryuta Kawashima
eNeuro 23 May 2023, 10 (6) ENEURO.0033-23.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0033-23.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • acceptance
  • anxiety
  • emotion regulation
  • functional connectivity
  • neural basis
  • reappraisal

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Release of extracellular matrix components after human traumatic brain injury
  • Action intentions reactivate representations of task-relevant cognitive cues
  • Interference underlies attenuation upon relearning in sensorimotor adaptation
Show more Research Article: New Research

Cognition and Behavior

  • Transformed visual working memory representations in human occipitotemporal and posterior parietal cortices
  • Neural Speech-Tracking During Selective Attention: A Spatially Realistic Audiovisual Study
  • Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Encodes the Trace Period during Appetitive Pavlovian Conditioning
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.