Abstract
The highly-conserved stomatin domain has been identified in genes throughout all classes of life. In animals, different stomatin domain-encoding genes have been implicated in the function of the kidney, red blood cells, and specific neuron types, although the underlying mechanisms remain unresolved. In one well-studied example of stomatin domain gene function, the Caenorhabditis elegans gene mec-2 and its mouse homolog Stoml3 are required for the function of mechanosensory neurons, where they modulate the activity of mechanosensory ion channels on the plasma membrane. Here, we identify an additional shared function for mec-2 and Stoml3 in a very different sensory context, that of olfaction. In worms, we find that a subset of stomatin domain genes are expressed in olfactory neurons, but only mec-2 is strongly required for olfactory behavior. mec-2 acts cell-autonomously and multiple alternatively-spliced isoforms of mec-2 can be substituted for each other. We generate a Stoml3 knock-out (KO) mouse and demonstrate that, like its worm homolog mec-2, it is required for olfactory behavior. In mice, Stoml3 is not required for odor detection, but is required for odor discrimination. Therefore, in addition to their shared roles in mechanosensory behavior, mec-2 and Stoml3 also have a shared role in olfactory behavior.
Significance Statement
In this manuscript we reveal a surprising moonlighting function for the conserved stomatin domain genes mec-2 (worm) and Stoml3 (mouse). Both factors have previously been studied extensively for their roles in mediating touch sensation in mechanosensory neurons. Here, we demonstrate that, in both worms and mice, they play an additional previously-unappreciated role in a different sensory modality, that of olfaction.
Introduction
Olfaction is a remarkable sensory system, enabling animals to detect and distinguish among a very large range of odors, and at very low concentrations. A number of cellular and molecular aspects of the olfactory system of various animals have been well characterized, including odorant-receptor interactions, signal transduction cascades within olfactory neurons, and downstream neuronal circuits (Buck and Axel, 1991; Katada et al., 2005; Hart and Chao, 2010; Del Mármol et al., 2021). However, many factors that are highly expressed in olfactory neurons remain completely uncharacterized or with poorly defined function (Kobayakawa et al., 2002; Kanageswaran et al., 2015; Dibattista et al., 2021).
We recently found that the stomatin domain gene mec-2 is highly expressed in Caenorhabditis elegans olfactory neurons, and is required for olfaction (Liang et al., 2022). The canonical role for mec-2 and its mouse homolog Stoml3 is as a component of the mechanotransduction machinery expressed in mechanosensory neurons (Huang et al., 1995; Wetzel et al., 2007). Our recent results therefore suggest that mec-2, and perhaps Stoml3 or other stomatin domain genes, might have previously unappreciated roles in olfactory neurons.
mec-2 and Stoml3 belong to a family of proteins defined by the presence of a highly-conserved stomatin domain, which can be found across all domains of life (Green and Young, 2008). The typical architecture of stomatin domain genes entails a central conserved stomatin domain and a hydrophobic region that mediates membrane association (Fig. 1A). These are flanked by divergent N and C termini, which can confer specific functional and regulatory features (Rungaldier et al., 2017; Cullinan et al., 2021; Liang et al., 2022). Stomatin domain proteins localize to the plasma membrane via their hydrophobic region, where some family members have been shown to modulate the activity of membrane proteins, although the precise mechanisms by which they do so remain elusive (Goodman et al., 2002; Genetet et al., 2017; Cullinan et al., 2021).
Extended Data Figure 1-1
Expression and location of Stomatin domain gene homologues. A, FPKMs for osm-6 as a positive control for a gene highly expressed in olfactory neurons (it serves as a sensory neuron marker gene) and lowly expressed in other neurons, including motor neurons. B, Additional cluster of Drosophila Podocin homologues on chromosome 3. Download Figure 1-1, TIF file.
Extended Data Figure 2-1
Stomatin domain genes required for behavior in C. elegans. A, Fraction of worms that fail to leave the 1-cm diameter origin circle after 1 h. Note that the majority of sto-4 and unc-1 mutants fail to locomote away from origin, whereas both wild-type and chemotaxis defective mutants do not remain in origin. B, Maximal ΔF/F GCaMP signals during odor presentation of isoamyl alcohol (1:1000) as displayed in Figure 2G. Unpaired two-tail t test, p < 0.05. C, D, AWB and AWC neuronal cell bodies visualized by transgene oyIs44 (ord-1p::RFP), revealing no obvious morphological differences in cell body position, axons, or dendrites between wild-type (C) and mec-2 mutant worms (D). Download Figure 2-1, TIF file.
Here, we show that both worms and mice express a small subset of stomatin domain genes in olfactory neurons, and that a single stomatin domain gene, mec-2, is strongly required for olfactory behavior in worms. We demonstrate that mec-2 acts in a cell-autonomous manner and that multiple alternatively spliced mec-2 isoforms can substitute for each other. We also generate a Stoml3 knock-out (KO) mouse and demonstrate that, like its homolog mec-2 in worms, Stoml3 is required for proper olfactory behavior in mice. Specifically, Stoml3 KO mice are able to detect odors, but are unable to efficiently distinguish between odors. Therefore, in addition to their conserved roles in mechanosensory behavior, mec-2 and Stoml3 have an additional conserved role in olfactory behavior in both worms and mice.
Materials and Methods
RNA Seq data
Raw fastq files were downloaded from the NCBI SRA for C. elegans (Taylor et al., 2021) and mouse (Bandyopadhyay et al., 2013; Kanageswaran et al., 2015) neuron-specific sequencing. Reads were mapped using STAR and alternative splicing mapped using JUM as previously reported (Dobin et al., 2013; Wang and Rio, 2018; Choudhary et al., 2021).
Chemotaxis assays
Unseeded plates were divided into four quadrants, two with buffer and two with odorant, chemotaxis index = (# animals in two odorant quadrants)/(# animals in any of the four quadrants). Only worms which moved beyond the central circle in which they were deposited were counted. Animals were staged at larval L4 stage, M9 buffer was used for washes, animals were pelleted via. Assays were extended to 2 h to allow for a greater number of the uncoordinated worms sto-4 and unc-1 worms to move out of the origin. mec-2(e75) is used as reference allele throughout.
Smell on a stick
Worms were staged to larval L4s. A total of 30+ worms of each genotype were transferred to unseeded plates. Each genotype was tested for lack of reaction to toothpick alone as well as toothpick and 70:30 95% EtOH: H2O (diluent). A 1:100,000 dilution of octanol in 70:30 95% EtOH: H2O was made (testing solution). A sterile toothpick was dipped into the testing solution and placed just in front of a forward moving worm. The worm was scored for positive response (moving away from the test solution) or negative response (no response). Once a worm was tested it was removed to ensure it would not be re-assayed; n = 30 worms per genotype per assay, assays conducted in triplicate.
Calcium imaging
Calcium imaging was performed on young adult hermaphrodites on a microfluidic “worm chip” as previously described (Chalasani et al., 2007), using the integrated transgene kyIs722 str-2p::GCaMP5(D380Y) to image GCaMP in the AWC(on) neuron. Isoamyl alcohol at 1:1000 was used as olfactant.
Mice
Stoml3 −/− mice were generated by deleting exon 2–5 of Stoml3 using CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing. gRNAs used to generate deletion. Upstream: CACATATGCGGGATGGTTTG and TAAACACCACATATGCGGGA. Downstream: GAGCCAAGACTCCCCAGCCC and AGTACAGCTATCCCTGGGCT. Mice were housed in a temperature and light controlled room (12/12 h dark/light cycle) and all animal experiments were conducted in accordance with policies of NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). Adult mice (more than six months old) were used in this study, and both sexes of animals were used. A total of 16 wild types and 12 Stoml3 mutants were used for each of the three olfactory tests unless otherwise noted, and the same mice were used for all behavioral tests. Behavioral videos were analyzed by an observer blind to mouse genotypes. In the block test and the habituation/dishabituation test, direct nasal contact with the odor stimulus was counted as investigation time.
Genotyping
DNA was extracted from tails of 19- to 21-d-old mice. Wild-type and mutant mice were determined by PCR using primer set (Stoml3 common forward primer 5′–TGTTCTCCCACATGCACACC–3′, Stoml3 wild-type reverse primer 5′–GGACCCTCATTAGATGCCCC–3′, Stoml3 mutant reverse primer 5′–GGCATCAGGTCCTCTGGAAC–3′).
Innate olfactory attraction test
Olfactory assays were conducted as previously described, with some modifications (Kobayakawa et al., 2007). Mice were isolated and habituated with a block 1 d before the test day. On the test day, mice were transferred to a clean cage with a thin layer of bedding for at least 10 min for habituation. Bedding is essential in this step to help mice reduce the fear of open space. After habituation, a scented wood block with different test odor was introduced. Animals were recorded from the front side of the cage for 3 min. Sniffing time was defined as nasal contact with the block and was measured afterward by analyzing the video. Wild-type mice (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 11), analyzed via ordinary one-way ANOVA (*p < 0.05). Odorants used were water (control, 80 μl), peanut butter (10% w/v, 80 μl) and mouse urine (80 μl). Mouse urines were collected freshly from different litters and mixed well before use.
Buried cereal test
Food restricted mice (90% of body weight) were used in buried cereal test to ensure mice were motivated to seek food. Mice were individually separated, and body weight was monitored every day before testing. A sweetened cereal was given to the tested mice before testing to overcome food neophobia. On all test days, mice were habituated for 1 h in their cages without water or food. Clean cages were prepared with ∼3 cm evenly distributed bedding, and one piece of sweetened cereal was buried 0.5 cm below the bedding. Mice were transferred from the habituation cage to the prepared new cage. A 5-min timer was started when the mouse was introduced in the testing cage, and time was recorded when the mouse found the cereal and began eating it. If the mouse did not uncover the pellet within 5 min, 300 s was recorded for that mouse. The buried cereal test was performed 5 d in a row, and the surface test was performed on the 6th test (cereal was put on the surface of bedding). Wild-type (n = 14) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 10). ns represents no significant difference between wild types and Stoml3 knock-outs using Mann–Whitney U test. The time to uncover the buried pellet from day 3 to 5 was averaged and then compared between wild-type and mutant mice using a Mann–Whitney U test, similarly, time to uncover the surface pellet from 6th day was compared between groups.
Habituation/dishabituation test
Mice were isolated in a clean cage overnight before testing. A tissue cartridge holding a nonscented cotton ball was placed in the cage to let mice get used to the novel item. On the testing day, mice were moved to the testing area without water and food for 1-h habituation. After habituation, a scented tissue cartridge (Fig. 4D, noted as odor 1) was placed into cage. Animals were recorded from the front side of the cage for 30 s. This was repeated for six consecutive trials with odor 1, with intertrial intervals of 5 min. On the 7th trial, a novel odor (Fig. 4D, odor 2) was introduced. Time sniffing was measured on each trial during the 30-s test period. Almond extract (5 μl) was used as odor 1, banana extract (5 μl) was used as odor 2. Almond and banana scent were selected because they were considered as neutral odors for mice (Yang and Crawley, 2009). Wild-type mice (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 12). Mean time investigating a scented cartridge during 30-s test period across seven trials. Almond extract was used on trials 1–6, and banana extract was introduced as a novel scent on trial 7. Significant difference of sniffing time in wild types is observed when novel scent is introduced on trial 7 (**p < 0.01), Mann–Whitney U test.
Block test
To measure ability to discriminate social odors, we performed the block test as previously described (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). Each mouse was individually housed in a clean cage with bedding and five wood blocks labeled A–E at least 24 h before testing. On the test day, all five blocks were removed from cage and placed into a sealed bag. Mice were transferred to testing area without food and water for 1-h habituation. After habituation, blocks A–D from the same mouse were placed back into the cage. Mice were video recorded from front side of the cage for 30 s. This procedure was repeated six times with at least 5-min interval. On the seventh trial, the same procedure was performed, but block D was replaced with block E from another mouse’s cage so that A, B, C were home-cage blocks and block E was from other mouse’s cage. Sniffing time of each block during the 30-s test period from seventh trial was measured. Wild-type (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 12). **p < 0.01, Mann–Whitney U test, error bars represent SEM.
Results
Conserved and species-specific stomatin domain genes
We recently showed that the C. elegans stomatin domain protein MEC-2 is expressed in olfactory neurons in addition to its previously-described expression in mechanosensory neurons (Liang et al., 2022). We also showed that mec-2 mutants are deficient in both mechanosensory and olfactory behaviors (Liang et al., 2022). The worm genome contains nine additional stomatin domain genes (Fig. 1B), and while mec-2 has been extensively characterized in C. elegans, many of the other stomatin domain genes remain uncharacterized. To test whether additional stomatin domain genes are involved in olfaction, we first determined phylogenetic relationships among stomatin domain proteins in C. elegans and other well-studied metazoan genomes (Fig. 1B), with the aim of determining whether closely related genes are co-expressed in specific neuron types (Fig. 1C,D).
We noticed a striking expansion of specific stomatin domain gene members in both Drosophila and C. elegans. For example, a single Stoml2 orthologue exists in humans, mice, flies, and worms. On the other hand, the Stomatin/Stoml3 subfamily contains two genes in humans and mice, three in flies, and eight in worms (Fig. 1B). This observation led us to examine whether there is evidence for recent expansion of stomatin domain genes in Drosophila or C. elegans. Indeed, three of the eight C. elegans Stomatin/Stoml3 homologues reside in a cluster on chromosome X (Fig. 1E). Such clustering is a hallmark of recently-duplicated genes (Katju and Lynch, 2003; Thomas, 2006). The remaining C. elegans Stomatin/Stoml3 homologues do not reside in genomic clusters, but are all located on the X chromosome, while the more distantly-related Stomatin domain genes are distributed across autosomes.
Drosophila stomatin domain genes also exhibit genomic clustering: two of the three Stomatin/Stoml3 homologues are located immediately next to each other on chromosome X (Fig. 1F), as are two of the three closely-related Drosophila homologues of Podocin on chromosome 3 (Extended Data Fig. 1-1B). Together, these results indicate that C. elegans and Drosophila genomes contain expanded repertoires of stomatin domain genes, most notably in genes related to Stomatin/Stoml3.
Subset of stomatin domain genes expressed in C. elegans olfactory neurons
We analyzed RNA Seq data generated from specific FACS-sorted neuron types (Taylor et al., 2021) to determine which of the C. elegans stomatin domain genes are expressed in olfactory neurons. We focused on the AWA and AWB neurons, which are involved in detecting attractive and repulsive odors, respectively (Ferkey et al., 2021). AWA and AWB cell bodies are located near the terminal pharyngeal bulb. Their dendrites extend anteriorly to the nose tip, ending in sensory cilia, and their axons synapse onto downstream interneurons. It was previously shown that mec-2 is required for detection of both AWA-specific and AWB-specific odorants (Liang et al., 2022), and cell-specific RNA Seq confirms that mec-2 is highly expressed in both AWA and AWB neurons (Fig. 1C; Extended Data Fig. 1-1A). As a comparison, we also assayed expression of stomatin domain genes in two classes of neurons that are not sensory neurons, the VD and DD motor neurons. In contrast to olfactory neurons, mec-2 expression is nearly undetectable in motor neurons (Fig. 1D). This demonstrates that mec-2 is expressed in specific subsets of neurons, including the AWA and AWB olfactory neurons.
We also detected substantial levels of expression for both sto-4 and unc-1 in AWA and AWB olfactory neurons (Fig. 1C). In contrast to mec-2, however, sto-4 and unc-1 are also highly expressed in motor neurons, suggestive of a broader function in various neuron types (Fig. 1D). Indeed, using both transcriptional and translational reporters, unc-1 was previously shown to be expressed and active in a variety of muscles and neurons, including motor neurons (Chen et al., 2007; Rajaram et al., 1999). Finally, we note that within the functional classes of neurons we tested, stomatin domain genes tend to exhibit similar expression levels between individual cell sub-types (e.g., mec-2 is highly expressed in both AWA and AWB). The one exception is sto-2, which is highly expressed in motor neurons, but with consistent >3-fold higher expression in DD than in VD motor neurons (Fig. 1D). Together, these data reveal a diversity of stomatin domain genes in the C. elegans genome, which exhibit cell-type specific gene expression within the nervous system.
Specific stomatin domain genes required for C. elegans olfactory behavior
We next tested whether the Stomatin domain genes expressed in olfactory neurons are required for olfactory behavior. Using standard chemotaxis assays (Fig. 2A) we previously showed that mec-2 is required for chemotaxis responses to a variety of volatile odorants, both attractive and repulsive (Liang et al., 2022; Fig. 2B,C). We obtained multiple loss-of-function alleles for both of the olfactory-neuron-expressed stomatin domain genes, unc-1 and sto-4. We found that mutations in both genes cause reduced locomotion, complicating the interpretation of standard chemotaxis assays, which require coordinated locomotion over time toward or away from the olfactants. Only worms that move from the origin in which they are deposited (Fig. 2A) are assayed, to control for animals that are unable to locomote. The majority of unc-1 and sto-4 animals remain within the origin, in contrast to wild types or mec-2, indicating an inability to locomote effectively (Extended Data Fig. 2-1A). Even so, among the minority of animals locomoting beyond the origin, both unc-1 and sto-4 mutants exhibit chemotaxis defects (Fig. 2B,C), although we reasoned that this could still be attributable to locomotory deficits.
To further disentangle the effects of unc-1 and sto-4 on olfaction versus locomotion, we turned to a “smell on a stick” assay, in which individual worms are directly presented with an aversive odor, causing wild-type worms to quickly perform a locomotion reversal to avoid the odor (Fig. 2D). This assay requires much less coordination of locomotion, allowing us to focus on the olfactory abilities of the mutants. mec-2 mutants exhibit strong defects in this assay when presented with the repellant odor octanol, while sto-4 and unc-1 mutations cause only minor olfactory defects (Fig. 2E). From these results, we conclude that mec-2 is the primary stomatin domain gene required for olfaction in C. elegans. While sto-4 and unc-1 are expressed in olfactory neurons, they seem to play a minor role in olfaction itself, and their requirement for chemotaxis stems largely from locomotory defects. This is consistent with their high levels of expression in motor neurons as well (Fig. 1C).
In mechanosensation, mec-2 has been shown to affect activity of mechanosensory channels, but not the development or structure of the mechanosensory neurons themselves (Zhang et al., 2004). We likewise wanted to test whether mec-2 plays a role in the development or activity of olfactory neurons. We examined the structure of the AWA neuron and found no differences between wild-type and mec-2 mutants: their cell bodies, dendrites, and cilia are all positioned appropriately, as is the olfactory receptor ODR-10 (Fig. 2F). We likewise found no morphologic differences in AWB or AWC neurons (Extended Data Fig. 2-1C,D) We therefore conclude that mec-2 is not required for the development or morphology of the AWA olfactory neuron.
We also tested the activity of olfactory neurons in wild-type and mec-2 animals, using the well-characterized str-2::GCaMP5 reporter of calcium activity in AWC(on) olfactory neurons (Chalasani et al., 2007; Akerboom et al., 2012). As previously described, in wild-type neurons odor presentation results in a robust decrease in calcium, and odor removal results in a robust increase in calcium (Chalasani et al., 2007; Akerboom et al., 2012; Fig. 2G). In mec-2 mutants, the calcium response is broadly similar, but the magnitude of the calcium response is decreased (Fig. 2G). The magnitude of the decrease is modest, but statistically significant (Extended Data Fig. 2-1B). From these experiments we conclude that mec-2 has a modest effect on olfactory neuron activity in response to olfactory stimuli.
Finally, to test whether mec-2 acts cell autonomously in olfactory neurons, we performed the smell on a stick assay in mec-2 mutants with cell-specific mec-2 re-expression. We used the str-1 promoter to re-introduce mec-2 to the AWB neuron, which is involved in sensing the aversive odor octanol (Chao et al., 2004). We previously demonstrated that mec-2 encodes three different isoforms with distinct functional properties in different cell types (Fig. 2H,I). In agreement with previous work using reporters (Liang et al., 2022), cell-specific RNA Seq indicates that the mec-2B isoform predominates in AWA/AWB olfactory neurons (Fig. 2J). We generated cDNAs for each of the three isoforms and tested whether each could rescue the olfactory response to octanol when artificially expressed in AWB. Indeed, transgenic re-introduction of any of the three mec-2 isoforms in the AWB neuron partially rescues octanol avoidance responses in mec-2 mutants (Fig. 2K). This is in contrast with previous work in mechanosensory neurons, which showed that mec-2B is not functional, while mec-2A and mec-2E must be co-expressed for proper mechanosensory behavior (Liang et al., 2022). Together, these experiments indicate that all three mec-2 isoforms are functional when ectopically expressed in AWB, and that mec-2 acts cell-autonomously in the AWB olfactory neuron.
Subset of stomatin domain genes expressed in mouse olfactory neurons
Previous work in C. elegans demonstrated a critical role for mec-2 in mechanosensory behavior, and inspired follow-up work in mice demonstrating a role for Stoml3 in mammalian mechanosensory behavior (Huang et al., 1995; Wetzel et al., 2007). We similarly wished to determine whether the role for mec-2 in olfaction represents a novel evolutionarily-conserved function for stomatin domain genes.
We first tested whether specific stomatin domain genes are selectively expressed in mouse olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs). To do so we analyzed RNA Seq data in which mouse olfactory receptor neurons were GFP labeled and FACS sorted, and compared this with similar data obtained from mouse motor neurons (Bandyopadhyay et al., 2013; Kanageswaran et al., 2015; Fig. 3A–D). We confirmed that marker genes for the respective neuron classes are expressed in the expected cell types (Fig. 3A,C), and found that stomatin domain genes exhibit distinct expression patterns (Fig. 3B,D). We observed high expression of both Stoml3 and Stomatin in ORNs, but not in motor neurons, while Stoml1 and Stoml2 are expressed in motor neurons but not olfactory neurons (Fig. 3A,B). Because Stoml3 displays the highest level of expression in ORNs, and because of previous work demonstrating striking localization of Stoml3 to ORN sensory cilia (Kobayakawa et al., 2002; Goldstein et al., 2003), we prioritized Stoml3 for further genetic and behavioral analysis.
Stoml3 is required for a subset of olfactory behaviors in mice
Stoml3 KO mice have previously been generated (Wetzel et al., 2007), but we were unable to obtain these mice, so we used CRISPR/Cas9 to generate a similar Stoml3 KO mouse (Fig. 3E–G). We removed the same two exons as in the published Stoml3 KO, as well as two additional downstream exons. The deletion results in the loss of the conserved stomatin domain of Stoml3 and introduction of downstream premature stop codons. We found that Stoml3−/− mice are born at Mendelian ratios and do not display overt defects in development, behavior, grooming, or body weight. This is in agreement with the previously-generated Stoml3−/− mice, which were further shown to possess normal motor performance (Wetzel et al., 2007).
To test whether Stoml3 mutant mice are defective in olfactory behavior, we performed a number of standard olfactory behavioral assays on Stoml3−/− mice and their wild-type littermates. We first tested the ability to detect and respond to olfactory stimuli that elicit innate behavioral responses in wild-type mice (Kobayakawa et al., 2007; Qiu et al., 2021). We measured time spent sniffing attractive odors compared with controls and found that both wild-type and Stoml3 mutant mice tend to spend more time sniffing attractive odors, although the trend only reaches statistical significance for urine scent in wild type (Fig. 4A,B). Time spent investigating each scent does not significantly differ between wild-type and Stoml3 mutants (Extended Data Fig. 4-1A), suggesting that Stoml3 mutants are not grossly different from wild type in their ability detect odors. We next measured latency to find and eat a buried cereal pellet, because mice with impaired olfaction have been demonstrated to take longer to uncover buried food (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014; Nathan et al., 2004). As with the innate olfactory assays, we found no differences between Stoml3 and wild-type mice in latency to uncover the buried pellet (Fig. 4C, S3B, p > 0.01). Together, these results suggest that loss of Stoml3 does not strongly affect innate olfactory response to attractive stimuli such as food odors.
Extended Data Figure 4-1
Stoml3 behavior and mec-2 expression. A, Latency to uncover pellet in cereal test measured on a daily basis, showing no significant differences between dynamics of wild-type and Stoml3 KO mice. B, In trial 6 of the block test, where no novel odor is present, time spent exploring the blocks is minimal. C, Single-cell sequencing data from CenGEN consortium data on mec-2 expression in representative neuron populations. Size of dot represents proportion of single cells in which mec-2 was detected, and heatmap represents scaled TPM of the mec-2 gene. A few sensory cell types are highlighted. Download Figure 4-1, TIF file.
To test whether Stoml3 knock-out mice are able to distinguish between different odors, we performed a habituation/dishabituation test in which mice are habituated with one scent for six consecutive trials, then switched to a novel scent on the seventh trial (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). We measured the time spent sniffing the odor cartridge during each trial. Both wild-type and Stoml3 mutant mice display an initial interest in the odor followed by a period of habituation and decreased interest in the odor, as previously described (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). On the seventh trial, the odor was changed to a novel scent. Wild-type mice spend more time sniffing the novel scent compared with a familiar scent (Fig. 4D, p < 0.01), as previously described (Wesson et al., 2008). In contrast, Stoml3 mutant mice do not exhibit an increased interest in the novel scent in the seventh trial (p > 0.01) and spend the same amount of time investigating the novel scent in trial 7 as with the habituated scent in trial 6. This suggests that Stoml3 mice, unlike wild types, are unable to (or unmotivated to) discriminate between familiar and novel scents (Fig. 4D).
Further evidence for this hypothesis comes from experiments on social odors in the “block test.” In this test, we housed mice individually with several wooden blocks for 24 h, allowing the blocks to acquire the odors of the home cage (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). The next day, we removed and reintroduced these home cage blocks for six consecutive trials to measure investigation time of the familiar blocks. Mice became habituated to the presence of the blocks and spent minimal time investigating them by the sixth trial (Extended Data Fig. 4-1C). On the seventh trial, one of the blocks was swapped with a block from the cage of a different mouse, thus introducing a block with novel social odors. Wild-type mice spend a substantial amount of time sniffing of the novel-odor block, as previously described (Wesson et al., 2008; Fig. 4E, p < 0.01). Stoml3 mutants also spend more time sniffing the novel-odor block, but spend significantly less time sniffing the novel-odor block when compared with wild-type mice (Fig. 4E, p < 0.01). This result indicates a deficit in responding to self versus nonself social odors, which could arise from reduced interest/motivation, discriminatory ability, or a combination of factors.
Together, these experiments demonstrate that Stoml3 knock-out mice do not completely lose the ability to detect odors (Fig. 4A–C). Rather, loss of Stoml3 impairs the behavioral response to odor discrimination, including familiar versus novel odors (Fig. 4D), and self versus nonself odors (Fig. 4E).
Discussion
Here, we demonstrate that worms and mice express a small subset of stomatin domain genes in olfactory neurons. Of these, mec-2 and Stoml3 are required for proper olfactory behavior in both worms and mice. In worms, mec-2 is required for chemotaxis, acts in a cell-autonomous manner, and multiple alternatively spliced mec-2 isoforms can substitute for one another. In mice, we find Stoml3 to be highly expressed in olfactory neurons. Our trancscriptomic analysis agrees with previous work showing that Stoml3 expression is highly restricted (Kobayakawa et al., 2002; Goldstein et al., 2003). It is undetectable in most tissues, but strongly expressed in olfactory neurons and in mechanosensory neurons of the dorsal root ganglia (Goldstein et al., 2003; Wetzel et al., 2007). Given this highly restricted expression, we speculate that in mice, as in worms, the function of Stoml3 in olfactory behavior is cell-autonomous. We show that Stoml3 KO mice are able to detect odors, but unable to efficiently distinguish between odors. Together, these experiments suggest that in addition to their well-established roles in mechanosensory behavior, mec-2 and Stoml3 have an additional shared role in olfactory behavior.
The precise biochemical role of stomatin domain proteins remains somewhat mysterious. A common theme is the modulation of ion channel function (Price et al., 2004; Huber et al., 2006), but the molecular mechanisms have yet to be fully elucidated. A model consistent with our data are that mec-2 and Stoml3 modulate ion channel activity in olfactory neurons in a manner required for their normal levels of activity. It will be interesting to test whether mec-2/Stoml3 exhibit similar or distinct interactions with ion channels or other membrane proteins in mechanosensory versus olfactory neurons. In a recent study, electrophysiological recordings from olfactory epithelium slices revealed that Stoml3 KO ORNs exhibit modest reductions in spontaneous firing frequency (Agostinelli et al., 2021, p. 3). Odor-evoked firing frequency is not affected, but spike duration and number are modestly reduced (Agostinelli et al., 2021, p. 3). These electrophysiological properties of Stoml3 KO OSNs could underly the deficits in olfactory behavior we describe here.
We now know that mec-2 and Stoml3 are required for at least two different sensory behaviors mediated by two different sensory neuron types (mechanosensory and olfactory). Are there additional neuronal roles yet to be uncovered? We note that in C. elegans, mec-2 transcripts are expressed not only in the AWA and AWB olfactory neurons described here, but at comparably high levels in gustatory, thermosensory, and other specific neuronal types as well (Extended Data Fig. 4-1D). It will be interesting to determine whether mec-2 and/or Stoml3 also play a role in these sensory activities as they do in mechanosensation and olfaction.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments: We thank the entire Norris Lab, and to Megan Norris for helpful discussions, feedback, and critical reading/review of this manuscript.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
A.N. was supported by grants from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke of the National Institutes of Health (NIH; R01NS111055) and the Welch Foundation (N-2042-20200401). Some strains were provided by the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center, which is funded by the NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs Grant P40 OD010440.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.