Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Sensory and Motor Systems

A Conserved Role for Stomatin Domain Genes in Olfactory Behavior

Xiaoyu Liang, Morgan Taylor, Rebekah Napier-Jameson, Canyon Calovich-Benne and Adam Norris
eNeuro 1 March 2023, 10 (3) ENEURO.0457-22.2023; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0457-22.2023
Xiaoyu Liang
Department of Biological Sciences, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas 75275
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Morgan Taylor
Department of Biological Sciences, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas 75275
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Morgan Taylor
Rebekah Napier-Jameson
Department of Biological Sciences, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas 75275
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Canyon Calovich-Benne
Department of Biological Sciences, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas 75275
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Adam Norris
Department of Biological Sciences, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas 75275
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The highly-conserved stomatin domain has been identified in genes throughout all classes of life. In animals, different stomatin domain-encoding genes have been implicated in the function of the kidney, red blood cells, and specific neuron types, although the underlying mechanisms remain unresolved. In one well-studied example of stomatin domain gene function, the Caenorhabditis elegans gene mec-2 and its mouse homolog Stoml3 are required for the function of mechanosensory neurons, where they modulate the activity of mechanosensory ion channels on the plasma membrane. Here, we identify an additional shared function for mec-2 and Stoml3 in a very different sensory context, that of olfaction. In worms, we find that a subset of stomatin domain genes are expressed in olfactory neurons, but only mec-2 is strongly required for olfactory behavior. mec-2 acts cell-autonomously and multiple alternatively-spliced isoforms of mec-2 can be substituted for each other. We generate a Stoml3 knock-out (KO) mouse and demonstrate that, like its worm homolog mec-2, it is required for olfactory behavior. In mice, Stoml3 is not required for odor detection, but is required for odor discrimination. Therefore, in addition to their shared roles in mechanosensory behavior, mec-2 and Stoml3 also have a shared role in olfactory behavior.

  • behavior
  • bhemotaxis
  • msec-2
  • stomatin
  • stoml3
  • olfaction

Significance Statement

In this manuscript we reveal a surprising moonlighting function for the conserved stomatin domain genes mec-2 (worm) and Stoml3 (mouse). Both factors have previously been studied extensively for their roles in mediating touch sensation in mechanosensory neurons. Here, we demonstrate that, in both worms and mice, they play an additional previously-unappreciated role in a different sensory modality, that of olfaction.

Introduction

Olfaction is a remarkable sensory system, enabling animals to detect and distinguish among a very large range of odors, and at very low concentrations. A number of cellular and molecular aspects of the olfactory system of various animals have been well characterized, including odorant-receptor interactions, signal transduction cascades within olfactory neurons, and downstream neuronal circuits (Buck and Axel, 1991; Katada et al., 2005; Hart and Chao, 2010; Del Mármol et al., 2021). However, many factors that are highly expressed in olfactory neurons remain completely uncharacterized or with poorly defined function (Kobayakawa et al., 2002; Kanageswaran et al., 2015; Dibattista et al., 2021).

We recently found that the stomatin domain gene mec-2 is highly expressed in Caenorhabditis elegans olfactory neurons, and is required for olfaction (Liang et al., 2022). The canonical role for mec-2 and its mouse homolog Stoml3 is as a component of the mechanotransduction machinery expressed in mechanosensory neurons (Huang et al., 1995; Wetzel et al., 2007). Our recent results therefore suggest that mec-2, and perhaps Stoml3 or other stomatin domain genes, might have previously unappreciated roles in olfactory neurons.

mec-2 and Stoml3 belong to a family of proteins defined by the presence of a highly-conserved stomatin domain, which can be found across all domains of life (Green and Young, 2008). The typical architecture of stomatin domain genes entails a central conserved stomatin domain and a hydrophobic region that mediates membrane association (Fig. 1A). These are flanked by divergent N and C termini, which can confer specific functional and regulatory features (Rungaldier et al., 2017; Cullinan et al., 2021; Liang et al., 2022). Stomatin domain proteins localize to the plasma membrane via their hydrophobic region, where some family members have been shown to modulate the activity of membrane proteins, although the precise mechanisms by which they do so remain elusive (Goodman et al., 2002; Genetet et al., 2017; Cullinan et al., 2021).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A subset of stomatin domain genes are expressed in C. elegans olfactory neurons. A, Domain organization of stomatin domain genes. Colored in shades of gray are the central conserved stomatin domain and the hydrophobic domain that mediates association with the membrane. Flanking these are the divergent N and C termini. B, Gene tree for family of stomatin domain gene homologues, TF105750 (stomatin-like 2) from TreeFam (Ruan et al., 2008). Some organism silhouettes created with BioRender. C, D, RNA Seq data from CenGEN consortium sorted neuron populations, mapped to the worm genome with STAR and gene reads counted by HTSeq. C, Olfactory neurons AWA and AWB. D, Motor neurons DD and VD. See also Extended Data Figure 1-1A for comparison with olfactory neuron marker gene FPKM. E, Cluster of three Stoml3/Stomatin-like genes in the C. elegans genome on chromosome X. F, Two immediately adjacent Stoml3/Stomatin-like genes in the Drosophila genome on chromosome X. See also Extended Data Figure 1-1.

Extended Data Figure 1-1

Expression and location of Stomatin domain gene homologues. A, FPKMs for osm-6 as a positive control for a gene highly expressed in olfactory neurons (it serves as a sensory neuron marker gene) and lowly expressed in other neurons, including motor neurons. B, Additional cluster of Drosophila Podocin homologues on chromosome 3. Download Figure 1-1, TIF file.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Specific stomatin domain genes required for olfaction in C. elegans. A, Schematic of the chemotaxis assay. Worms are placed in the center of an unseeded worm plate, given a choice between point sources of odor (represented by +) and unscented diluent (represented by 0), and given 1 h to chemotax. B, C, Chemotaxis assays for the attractive odor pyrazine (1:1000; B) or the repulsive odor octanol (1:1000; C). The chemotaxis index = ((worms in + quadrant)(worms in 0 quadrant))/(worms in all quadrants), with a maximum value of 1 (attractive) and minimum value of −1 (repulsive). D, Schematic of the smell on a stick assay for repulsive odors. E, Smell on a stick assay for octanol. F, AWA neurons labeled by translational ODR-10::GFP fusion. White arrowheads denote sensory cilia (where ODR-10 localizes). Gray arrowheads denote cell bodies. The cell body in the mec-2 image is slightly out of the plane of focus. Scale bar represents 50 μm. See also Extended Data Figure 2-1C,D. G, GCaMP5 imaging of AWC(on) neurons using a microfluidic “olfactory chip.” Isoamyl alcohol (1:1000) administration represented by the gray box. Error bars represent SEM N = 12 young adult worms (5 wild-type, 7 mec-2 mutants). F, Schematic of mec-2 alternative splicing and (G) subsequent protein isoforms. H, I, mec-2 alternative isoforms at the level of pre-mRNA splicing (H) and protein isoforms (I). The isoforms affect the divergent C terminus of mec-2. J, CenGEN RNA Seq data reveals mec-2 is found primarily as the mec-2B isoform in AWA and AWB olfactory neurons. K, Smell on a stick assay, 1:100,000 octanol, statistical test (ANOVA) performed against mec-2() mutants. Re-expression of any mec-2 isoform in AWB neuron partially rescues response. Asterisks indicate p < 0.05, one-way ANOVA. See also Extended Data Figure 2-1.

Extended Data Figure 2-1

Stomatin domain genes required for behavior in C. elegans. A, Fraction of worms that fail to leave the 1-cm diameter origin circle after 1 h. Note that the majority of sto-4 and unc-1 mutants fail to locomote away from origin, whereas both wild-type and chemotaxis defective mutants do not remain in origin. B, Maximal ΔF/F GCaMP signals during odor presentation of isoamyl alcohol (1:1000) as displayed in Figure 2G. Unpaired two-tail t test, p < 0.05. C, D, AWB and AWC neuronal cell bodies visualized by transgene oyIs44 (ord-1p::RFP), revealing no obvious morphological differences in cell body position, axons, or dendrites between wild-type (C) and mec-2 mutant worms (D). Download Figure 2-1, TIF file.

Here, we show that both worms and mice express a small subset of stomatin domain genes in olfactory neurons, and that a single stomatin domain gene, mec-2, is strongly required for olfactory behavior in worms. We demonstrate that mec-2 acts in a cell-autonomous manner and that multiple alternatively spliced mec-2 isoforms can substitute for each other. We also generate a Stoml3 knock-out (KO) mouse and demonstrate that, like its homolog mec-2 in worms, Stoml3 is required for proper olfactory behavior in mice. Specifically, Stoml3 KO mice are able to detect odors, but are unable to efficiently distinguish between odors. Therefore, in addition to their conserved roles in mechanosensory behavior, mec-2 and Stoml3 have an additional conserved role in olfactory behavior in both worms and mice.

Materials and Methods

RNA Seq data

Raw fastq files were downloaded from the NCBI SRA for C. elegans (Taylor et al., 2021) and mouse (Bandyopadhyay et al., 2013; Kanageswaran et al., 2015) neuron-specific sequencing. Reads were mapped using STAR and alternative splicing mapped using JUM as previously reported (Dobin et al., 2013; Wang and Rio, 2018; Choudhary et al., 2021).

Chemotaxis assays

Unseeded plates were divided into four quadrants, two with buffer and two with odorant, chemotaxis index = (# animals in two odorant quadrants)/(# animals in any of the four quadrants). Only worms which moved beyond the central circle in which they were deposited were counted. Animals were staged at larval L4 stage, M9 buffer was used for washes, animals were pelleted via. Assays were extended to 2 h to allow for a greater number of the uncoordinated worms sto-4 and unc-1 worms to move out of the origin. mec-2(e75) is used as reference allele throughout.

Smell on a stick

Worms were staged to larval L4s. A total of 30+ worms of each genotype were transferred to unseeded plates. Each genotype was tested for lack of reaction to toothpick alone as well as toothpick and 70:30 95% EtOH: H2O (diluent). A 1:100,000 dilution of octanol in 70:30 95% EtOH: H2O was made (testing solution). A sterile toothpick was dipped into the testing solution and placed just in front of a forward moving worm. The worm was scored for positive response (moving away from the test solution) or negative response (no response). Once a worm was tested it was removed to ensure it would not be re-assayed; n = 30 worms per genotype per assay, assays conducted in triplicate.

Calcium imaging

Calcium imaging was performed on young adult hermaphrodites on a microfluidic “worm chip” as previously described (Chalasani et al., 2007), using the integrated transgene kyIs722 str-2p::GCaMP5(D380Y) to image GCaMP in the AWC(on) neuron. Isoamyl alcohol at 1:1000 was used as olfactant.

Mice

Stoml3 −/− mice were generated by deleting exon 2–5 of Stoml3 using CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing. gRNAs used to generate deletion. Upstream: CACATATGCGGGATGGTTTG and TAAACACCACATATGCGGGA. Downstream: GAGCCAAGACTCCCCAGCCC and AGTACAGCTATCCCTGGGCT. Mice were housed in a temperature and light controlled room (12/12 h dark/light cycle) and all animal experiments were conducted in accordance with policies of NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). Adult mice (more than six months old) were used in this study, and both sexes of animals were used. A total of 16 wild types and 12 Stoml3 mutants were used for each of the three olfactory tests unless otherwise noted, and the same mice were used for all behavioral tests. Behavioral videos were analyzed by an observer blind to mouse genotypes. In the block test and the habituation/dishabituation test, direct nasal contact with the odor stimulus was counted as investigation time.

Genotyping

DNA was extracted from tails of 19- to 21-d-old mice. Wild-type and mutant mice were determined by PCR using primer set (Stoml3 common forward primer 5′–TGTTCTCCCACATGCACACC–3′, Stoml3 wild-type reverse primer 5′–GGACCCTCATTAGATGCCCC–3′, Stoml3 mutant reverse primer 5′–GGCATCAGGTCCTCTGGAAC–3′).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Stomatin domain genes expressed in mouse neurons, and Stoml3 knock-out mice. A–D, Cell-specific sequencing of mouse olfactory neurons (A, B) and motor neurons (C, D). Omp is used as a positive marker for olfactory neurons, and Chat as a positive marker for cholinergic motor neurons. E, Stoml3 gene in mouse, indicating the region we deleted in orange, and the primer sets used to detect both the wild-type and mutant genomic DNA (black arrows). F, Sanger sequencing confirming the ∼5.4-kb deletion. G, Genotyping gels for identifying homozygous Stoml3 mutants and their wild-type littermates.

Innate olfactory attraction test

Olfactory assays were conducted as previously described, with some modifications (Kobayakawa et al., 2007). Mice were isolated and habituated with a block 1 d before the test day. On the test day, mice were transferred to a clean cage with a thin layer of bedding for at least 10 min for habituation. Bedding is essential in this step to help mice reduce the fear of open space. After habituation, a scented wood block with different test odor was introduced. Animals were recorded from the front side of the cage for 3 min. Sniffing time was defined as nasal contact with the block and was measured afterward by analyzing the video. Wild-type mice (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 11), analyzed via ordinary one-way ANOVA (*p < 0.05). Odorants used were water (control, 80 μl), peanut butter (10% w/v, 80 μl) and mouse urine (80 μl). Mouse urines were collected freshly from different litters and mixed well before use.

Buried cereal test

Food restricted mice (90% of body weight) were used in buried cereal test to ensure mice were motivated to seek food. Mice were individually separated, and body weight was monitored every day before testing. A sweetened cereal was given to the tested mice before testing to overcome food neophobia. On all test days, mice were habituated for 1 h in their cages without water or food. Clean cages were prepared with ∼3 cm evenly distributed bedding, and one piece of sweetened cereal was buried 0.5 cm below the bedding. Mice were transferred from the habituation cage to the prepared new cage. A 5-min timer was started when the mouse was introduced in the testing cage, and time was recorded when the mouse found the cereal and began eating it. If the mouse did not uncover the pellet within 5 min, 300 s was recorded for that mouse. The buried cereal test was performed 5 d in a row, and the surface test was performed on the 6th test (cereal was put on the surface of bedding). Wild-type (n = 14) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 10). ns represents no significant difference between wild types and Stoml3 knock-outs using Mann–Whitney U test. The time to uncover the buried pellet from day 3 to 5 was averaged and then compared between wild-type and mutant mice using a Mann–Whitney U test, similarly, time to uncover the surface pellet from 6th day was compared between groups.

Habituation/dishabituation test

Mice were isolated in a clean cage overnight before testing. A tissue cartridge holding a nonscented cotton ball was placed in the cage to let mice get used to the novel item. On the testing day, mice were moved to the testing area without water and food for 1-h habituation. After habituation, a scented tissue cartridge (Fig. 4D, noted as odor 1) was placed into cage. Animals were recorded from the front side of the cage for 30 s. This was repeated for six consecutive trials with odor 1, with intertrial intervals of 5 min. On the 7th trial, a novel odor (Fig. 4D, odor 2) was introduced. Time sniffing was measured on each trial during the 30-s test period. Almond extract (5 μl) was used as odor 1, banana extract (5 μl) was used as odor 2. Almond and banana scent were selected because they were considered as neutral odors for mice (Yang and Crawley, 2009). Wild-type mice (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 12). Mean time investigating a scented cartridge during 30-s test period across seven trials. Almond extract was used on trials 1–6, and banana extract was introduced as a novel scent on trial 7. Significant difference of sniffing time in wild types is observed when novel scent is introduced on trial 7 (**p < 0.01), Mann–Whitney U test.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Olfactory assays for wild-type and Stoml3 knock-out mice. A, B, Innate olfactory attraction tests. Mean time sniffing a scented block in wild-type mice (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 11) during the 3-min test period. Water was used as a control scent. Peanut butter and mouse urine were used as attractive scents. Both wild-type and Stoml3 knock-out mice showed attraction response to peanut butter and mouse urine. Asterisk indicates p < 0.05. C, Buried cereal test. Mean time wild-type (n = 14) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 10) took to find the pellet. Surface pellet test is a positive control, confirming that the cereal pellet is attractive to the mice. ns represents no significant difference between wild types and Stoml3 knock-outs using Mann–Whitney U test. D, Habituation/dishabituation test. Mean time sniffing a scented cartridge in wild-type mice (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 12) during 30-s test period across seven trials. Almond extract was used on trials 1–6, and banana extract was introduced as a novel scent on trial 7. Significant difference of sniffing time in wild types is observed when novel scent is introduced on trial 7 (**p < 0.01), while no significant difference is observed between trial 6 and trial 7 in Stoml3 knock-out mice. E, Block test. Mean time sniffing home cage blocks A–C and novel block E in wild-type mice (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 12). Wild types spend more than 2-fold time exploring novel block (E) than Stoml3 knock-out mice (**p < 0.01, Mann–Whitney U test, error bars represent SEM). See also Extended Data Figure 4-1.

Block test

To measure ability to discriminate social odors, we performed the block test as previously described (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). Each mouse was individually housed in a clean cage with bedding and five wood blocks labeled A–E at least 24 h before testing. On the test day, all five blocks were removed from cage and placed into a sealed bag. Mice were transferred to testing area without food and water for 1-h habituation. After habituation, blocks A–D from the same mouse were placed back into the cage. Mice were video recorded from front side of the cage for 30 s. This procedure was repeated six times with at least 5-min interval. On the seventh trial, the same procedure was performed, but block D was replaced with block E from another mouse’s cage so that A, B, C were home-cage blocks and block E was from other mouse’s cage. Sniffing time of each block during the 30-s test period from seventh trial was measured. Wild-type (n = 16) and Stoml3 knock-out mice (n = 12). **p < 0.01, Mann–Whitney U test, error bars represent SEM.

Results

Conserved and species-specific stomatin domain genes

We recently showed that the C. elegans stomatin domain protein MEC-2 is expressed in olfactory neurons in addition to its previously-described expression in mechanosensory neurons (Liang et al., 2022). We also showed that mec-2 mutants are deficient in both mechanosensory and olfactory behaviors (Liang et al., 2022). The worm genome contains nine additional stomatin domain genes (Fig. 1B), and while mec-2 has been extensively characterized in C. elegans, many of the other stomatin domain genes remain uncharacterized. To test whether additional stomatin domain genes are involved in olfaction, we first determined phylogenetic relationships among stomatin domain proteins in C. elegans and other well-studied metazoan genomes (Fig. 1B), with the aim of determining whether closely related genes are co-expressed in specific neuron types (Fig. 1C,D).

We noticed a striking expansion of specific stomatin domain gene members in both Drosophila and C. elegans. For example, a single Stoml2 orthologue exists in humans, mice, flies, and worms. On the other hand, the Stomatin/Stoml3 subfamily contains two genes in humans and mice, three in flies, and eight in worms (Fig. 1B). This observation led us to examine whether there is evidence for recent expansion of stomatin domain genes in Drosophila or C. elegans. Indeed, three of the eight C. elegans Stomatin/Stoml3 homologues reside in a cluster on chromosome X (Fig. 1E). Such clustering is a hallmark of recently-duplicated genes (Katju and Lynch, 2003; Thomas, 2006). The remaining C. elegans Stomatin/Stoml3 homologues do not reside in genomic clusters, but are all located on the X chromosome, while the more distantly-related Stomatin domain genes are distributed across autosomes.

Drosophila stomatin domain genes also exhibit genomic clustering: two of the three Stomatin/Stoml3 homologues are located immediately next to each other on chromosome X (Fig. 1F), as are two of the three closely-related Drosophila homologues of Podocin on chromosome 3 (Extended Data Fig. 1-1B). Together, these results indicate that C. elegans and Drosophila genomes contain expanded repertoires of stomatin domain genes, most notably in genes related to Stomatin/Stoml3.

Subset of stomatin domain genes expressed in C. elegans olfactory neurons

We analyzed RNA Seq data generated from specific FACS-sorted neuron types (Taylor et al., 2021) to determine which of the C. elegans stomatin domain genes are expressed in olfactory neurons. We focused on the AWA and AWB neurons, which are involved in detecting attractive and repulsive odors, respectively (Ferkey et al., 2021). AWA and AWB cell bodies are located near the terminal pharyngeal bulb. Their dendrites extend anteriorly to the nose tip, ending in sensory cilia, and their axons synapse onto downstream interneurons. It was previously shown that mec-2 is required for detection of both AWA-specific and AWB-specific odorants (Liang et al., 2022), and cell-specific RNA Seq confirms that mec-2 is highly expressed in both AWA and AWB neurons (Fig. 1C; Extended Data Fig. 1-1A). As a comparison, we also assayed expression of stomatin domain genes in two classes of neurons that are not sensory neurons, the VD and DD motor neurons. In contrast to olfactory neurons, mec-2 expression is nearly undetectable in motor neurons (Fig. 1D). This demonstrates that mec-2 is expressed in specific subsets of neurons, including the AWA and AWB olfactory neurons.

We also detected substantial levels of expression for both sto-4 and unc-1 in AWA and AWB olfactory neurons (Fig. 1C). In contrast to mec-2, however, sto-4 and unc-1 are also highly expressed in motor neurons, suggestive of a broader function in various neuron types (Fig. 1D). Indeed, using both transcriptional and translational reporters, unc-1 was previously shown to be expressed and active in a variety of muscles and neurons, including motor neurons (Chen et al., 2007; Rajaram et al., 1999). Finally, we note that within the functional classes of neurons we tested, stomatin domain genes tend to exhibit similar expression levels between individual cell sub-types (e.g., mec-2 is highly expressed in both AWA and AWB). The one exception is sto-2, which is highly expressed in motor neurons, but with consistent >3-fold higher expression in DD than in VD motor neurons (Fig. 1D). Together, these data reveal a diversity of stomatin domain genes in the C. elegans genome, which exhibit cell-type specific gene expression within the nervous system.

Specific stomatin domain genes required for C. elegans olfactory behavior

We next tested whether the Stomatin domain genes expressed in olfactory neurons are required for olfactory behavior. Using standard chemotaxis assays (Fig. 2A) we previously showed that mec-2 is required for chemotaxis responses to a variety of volatile odorants, both attractive and repulsive (Liang et al., 2022; Fig. 2B,C). We obtained multiple loss-of-function alleles for both of the olfactory-neuron-expressed stomatin domain genes, unc-1 and sto-4. We found that mutations in both genes cause reduced locomotion, complicating the interpretation of standard chemotaxis assays, which require coordinated locomotion over time toward or away from the olfactants. Only worms that move from the origin in which they are deposited (Fig. 2A) are assayed, to control for animals that are unable to locomote. The majority of unc-1 and sto-4 animals remain within the origin, in contrast to wild types or mec-2, indicating an inability to locomote effectively (Extended Data Fig. 2-1A). Even so, among the minority of animals locomoting beyond the origin, both unc-1 and sto-4 mutants exhibit chemotaxis defects (Fig. 2B,C), although we reasoned that this could still be attributable to locomotory deficits.

To further disentangle the effects of unc-1 and sto-4 on olfaction versus locomotion, we turned to a “smell on a stick” assay, in which individual worms are directly presented with an aversive odor, causing wild-type worms to quickly perform a locomotion reversal to avoid the odor (Fig. 2D). This assay requires much less coordination of locomotion, allowing us to focus on the olfactory abilities of the mutants. mec-2 mutants exhibit strong defects in this assay when presented with the repellant odor octanol, while sto-4 and unc-1 mutations cause only minor olfactory defects (Fig. 2E). From these results, we conclude that mec-2 is the primary stomatin domain gene required for olfaction in C. elegans. While sto-4 and unc-1 are expressed in olfactory neurons, they seem to play a minor role in olfaction itself, and their requirement for chemotaxis stems largely from locomotory defects. This is consistent with their high levels of expression in motor neurons as well (Fig. 1C).

In mechanosensation, mec-2 has been shown to affect activity of mechanosensory channels, but not the development or structure of the mechanosensory neurons themselves (Zhang et al., 2004). We likewise wanted to test whether mec-2 plays a role in the development or activity of olfactory neurons. We examined the structure of the AWA neuron and found no differences between wild-type and mec-2 mutants: their cell bodies, dendrites, and cilia are all positioned appropriately, as is the olfactory receptor ODR-10 (Fig. 2F). We likewise found no morphologic differences in AWB or AWC neurons (Extended Data Fig. 2-1C,D) We therefore conclude that mec-2 is not required for the development or morphology of the AWA olfactory neuron.

We also tested the activity of olfactory neurons in wild-type and mec-2 animals, using the well-characterized str-2::GCaMP5 reporter of calcium activity in AWC(on) olfactory neurons (Chalasani et al., 2007; Akerboom et al., 2012). As previously described, in wild-type neurons odor presentation results in a robust decrease in calcium, and odor removal results in a robust increase in calcium (Chalasani et al., 2007; Akerboom et al., 2012; Fig. 2G). In mec-2 mutants, the calcium response is broadly similar, but the magnitude of the calcium response is decreased (Fig. 2G). The magnitude of the decrease is modest, but statistically significant (Extended Data Fig. 2-1B). From these experiments we conclude that mec-2 has a modest effect on olfactory neuron activity in response to olfactory stimuli.

Finally, to test whether mec-2 acts cell autonomously in olfactory neurons, we performed the smell on a stick assay in mec-2 mutants with cell-specific mec-2 re-expression. We used the str-1 promoter to re-introduce mec-2 to the AWB neuron, which is involved in sensing the aversive odor octanol (Chao et al., 2004). We previously demonstrated that mec-2 encodes three different isoforms with distinct functional properties in different cell types (Fig. 2H,I). In agreement with previous work using reporters (Liang et al., 2022), cell-specific RNA Seq indicates that the mec-2B isoform predominates in AWA/AWB olfactory neurons (Fig. 2J). We generated cDNAs for each of the three isoforms and tested whether each could rescue the olfactory response to octanol when artificially expressed in AWB. Indeed, transgenic re-introduction of any of the three mec-2 isoforms in the AWB neuron partially rescues octanol avoidance responses in mec-2 mutants (Fig. 2K). This is in contrast with previous work in mechanosensory neurons, which showed that mec-2B is not functional, while mec-2A and mec-2E must be co-expressed for proper mechanosensory behavior (Liang et al., 2022). Together, these experiments indicate that all three mec-2 isoforms are functional when ectopically expressed in AWB, and that mec-2 acts cell-autonomously in the AWB olfactory neuron.

Subset of stomatin domain genes expressed in mouse olfactory neurons

Previous work in C. elegans demonstrated a critical role for mec-2 in mechanosensory behavior, and inspired follow-up work in mice demonstrating a role for Stoml3 in mammalian mechanosensory behavior (Huang et al., 1995; Wetzel et al., 2007). We similarly wished to determine whether the role for mec-2 in olfaction represents a novel evolutionarily-conserved function for stomatin domain genes.

We first tested whether specific stomatin domain genes are selectively expressed in mouse olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs). To do so we analyzed RNA Seq data in which mouse olfactory receptor neurons were GFP labeled and FACS sorted, and compared this with similar data obtained from mouse motor neurons (Bandyopadhyay et al., 2013; Kanageswaran et al., 2015; Fig. 3A–D). We confirmed that marker genes for the respective neuron classes are expressed in the expected cell types (Fig. 3A,C), and found that stomatin domain genes exhibit distinct expression patterns (Fig. 3B,D). We observed high expression of both Stoml3 and Stomatin in ORNs, but not in motor neurons, while Stoml1 and Stoml2 are expressed in motor neurons but not olfactory neurons (Fig. 3A,B). Because Stoml3 displays the highest level of expression in ORNs, and because of previous work demonstrating striking localization of Stoml3 to ORN sensory cilia (Kobayakawa et al., 2002; Goldstein et al., 2003), we prioritized Stoml3 for further genetic and behavioral analysis.

Stoml3 is required for a subset of olfactory behaviors in mice

Stoml3 KO mice have previously been generated (Wetzel et al., 2007), but we were unable to obtain these mice, so we used CRISPR/Cas9 to generate a similar Stoml3 KO mouse (Fig. 3E–G). We removed the same two exons as in the published Stoml3 KO, as well as two additional downstream exons. The deletion results in the loss of the conserved stomatin domain of Stoml3 and introduction of downstream premature stop codons. We found that Stoml3−/− mice are born at Mendelian ratios and do not display overt defects in development, behavior, grooming, or body weight. This is in agreement with the previously-generated Stoml3−/− mice, which were further shown to possess normal motor performance (Wetzel et al., 2007).

To test whether Stoml3 mutant mice are defective in olfactory behavior, we performed a number of standard olfactory behavioral assays on Stoml3−/− mice and their wild-type littermates. We first tested the ability to detect and respond to olfactory stimuli that elicit innate behavioral responses in wild-type mice (Kobayakawa et al., 2007; Qiu et al., 2021). We measured time spent sniffing attractive odors compared with controls and found that both wild-type and Stoml3 mutant mice tend to spend more time sniffing attractive odors, although the trend only reaches statistical significance for urine scent in wild type (Fig. 4A,B). Time spent investigating each scent does not significantly differ between wild-type and Stoml3 mutants (Extended Data Fig. 4-1A), suggesting that Stoml3 mutants are not grossly different from wild type in their ability detect odors. We next measured latency to find and eat a buried cereal pellet, because mice with impaired olfaction have been demonstrated to take longer to uncover buried food (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014; Nathan et al., 2004). As with the innate olfactory assays, we found no differences between Stoml3 and wild-type mice in latency to uncover the buried pellet (Fig. 4C, S3B, p > 0.01). Together, these results suggest that loss of Stoml3 does not strongly affect innate olfactory response to attractive stimuli such as food odors.

Extended Data Figure 4-1

Stoml3 behavior and mec-2 expression. A, Latency to uncover pellet in cereal test measured on a daily basis, showing no significant differences between dynamics of wild-type and Stoml3 KO mice. B, In trial 6 of the block test, where no novel odor is present, time spent exploring the blocks is minimal. C, Single-cell sequencing data from CenGEN consortium data on mec-2 expression in representative neuron populations. Size of dot represents proportion of single cells in which mec-2 was detected, and heatmap represents scaled TPM of the mec-2 gene. A few sensory cell types are highlighted. Download Figure 4-1, TIF file.

To test whether Stoml3 knock-out mice are able to distinguish between different odors, we performed a habituation/dishabituation test in which mice are habituated with one scent for six consecutive trials, then switched to a novel scent on the seventh trial (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). We measured the time spent sniffing the odor cartridge during each trial. Both wild-type and Stoml3 mutant mice display an initial interest in the odor followed by a period of habituation and decreased interest in the odor, as previously described (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). On the seventh trial, the odor was changed to a novel scent. Wild-type mice spend more time sniffing the novel scent compared with a familiar scent (Fig. 4D, p < 0.01), as previously described (Wesson et al., 2008). In contrast, Stoml3 mutant mice do not exhibit an increased interest in the novel scent in the seventh trial (p > 0.01) and spend the same amount of time investigating the novel scent in trial 7 as with the habituated scent in trial 6. This suggests that Stoml3 mice, unlike wild types, are unable to (or unmotivated to) discriminate between familiar and novel scents (Fig. 4D).

Further evidence for this hypothesis comes from experiments on social odors in the “block test.” In this test, we housed mice individually with several wooden blocks for 24 h, allowing the blocks to acquire the odors of the home cage (Lehmkuhl et al., 2014). The next day, we removed and reintroduced these home cage blocks for six consecutive trials to measure investigation time of the familiar blocks. Mice became habituated to the presence of the blocks and spent minimal time investigating them by the sixth trial (Extended Data Fig. 4-1C). On the seventh trial, one of the blocks was swapped with a block from the cage of a different mouse, thus introducing a block with novel social odors. Wild-type mice spend a substantial amount of time sniffing of the novel-odor block, as previously described (Wesson et al., 2008; Fig. 4E, p < 0.01). Stoml3 mutants also spend more time sniffing the novel-odor block, but spend significantly less time sniffing the novel-odor block when compared with wild-type mice (Fig. 4E, p < 0.01). This result indicates a deficit in responding to self versus nonself social odors, which could arise from reduced interest/motivation, discriminatory ability, or a combination of factors.

Together, these experiments demonstrate that Stoml3 knock-out mice do not completely lose the ability to detect odors (Fig. 4A–C). Rather, loss of Stoml3 impairs the behavioral response to odor discrimination, including familiar versus novel odors (Fig. 4D), and self versus nonself odors (Fig. 4E).

Discussion

Here, we demonstrate that worms and mice express a small subset of stomatin domain genes in olfactory neurons. Of these, mec-2 and Stoml3 are required for proper olfactory behavior in both worms and mice. In worms, mec-2 is required for chemotaxis, acts in a cell-autonomous manner, and multiple alternatively spliced mec-2 isoforms can substitute for one another. In mice, we find Stoml3 to be highly expressed in olfactory neurons. Our trancscriptomic analysis agrees with previous work showing that Stoml3 expression is highly restricted (Kobayakawa et al., 2002; Goldstein et al., 2003). It is undetectable in most tissues, but strongly expressed in olfactory neurons and in mechanosensory neurons of the dorsal root ganglia (Goldstein et al., 2003; Wetzel et al., 2007). Given this highly restricted expression, we speculate that in mice, as in worms, the function of Stoml3 in olfactory behavior is cell-autonomous. We show that Stoml3 KO mice are able to detect odors, but unable to efficiently distinguish between odors. Together, these experiments suggest that in addition to their well-established roles in mechanosensory behavior, mec-2 and Stoml3 have an additional shared role in olfactory behavior.

The precise biochemical role of stomatin domain proteins remains somewhat mysterious. A common theme is the modulation of ion channel function (Price et al., 2004; Huber et al., 2006), but the molecular mechanisms have yet to be fully elucidated. A model consistent with our data are that mec-2 and Stoml3 modulate ion channel activity in olfactory neurons in a manner required for their normal levels of activity. It will be interesting to test whether mec-2/Stoml3 exhibit similar or distinct interactions with ion channels or other membrane proteins in mechanosensory versus olfactory neurons. In a recent study, electrophysiological recordings from olfactory epithelium slices revealed that Stoml3 KO ORNs exhibit modest reductions in spontaneous firing frequency (Agostinelli et al., 2021, p. 3). Odor-evoked firing frequency is not affected, but spike duration and number are modestly reduced (Agostinelli et al., 2021, p. 3). These electrophysiological properties of Stoml3 KO OSNs could underly the deficits in olfactory behavior we describe here.

We now know that mec-2 and Stoml3 are required for at least two different sensory behaviors mediated by two different sensory neuron types (mechanosensory and olfactory). Are there additional neuronal roles yet to be uncovered? We note that in C. elegans, mec-2 transcripts are expressed not only in the AWA and AWB olfactory neurons described here, but at comparably high levels in gustatory, thermosensory, and other specific neuronal types as well (Extended Data Fig. 4-1D). It will be interesting to determine whether mec-2 and/or Stoml3 also play a role in these sensory activities as they do in mechanosensation and olfaction.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: We thank the entire Norris Lab, and to Megan Norris for helpful discussions, feedback, and critical reading/review of this manuscript.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • A.N. was supported by grants from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke of the National Institutes of Health (NIH; R01NS111055) and the Welch Foundation (N-2042-20200401). Some strains were provided by the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center, which is funded by the NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs Grant P40 OD010440.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Agostinelli E, Gonzalez-Velandia KY, Hernandez-Clavijo A, Kumar Maurya D, Xerxa E, Lewin GR, Dibattista M, Menini A, Pifferi S (2021) A role for STOML3 in olfactory sensory transduction. eNeuro 8:ENEURO.0565-20.2021. https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0565-20.2021
    OpenUrl
  2. ↵
    Akerboom J, et al. (2012) Optimization of a GCaMP calcium indicator for neural activity imaging. J Neurosci 32:13819–13840. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2601-12.2012 pmid:23035093
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Bandyopadhyay U, Cotney J, Nagy M, Oh S, Leng J, Mahajan M, Mane S, Fenton WA, Noonan JP, Horwich AL (2013) RNA-Seq profiling of spinal cord motor neurons from a presymptomatic SOD1 ALS mouse. PLoS One 8:e53575. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053575 pmid:23301088
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Buck L, Axel R (1991) A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor recognition. Cell 65:175–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-8674(91)90418-x pmid:1840504
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Chalasani SH, Chronis N, Tsunozaki M, Gray JM, Ramot D, Goodman MB, Bargmann CI (2007) Dissecting a circuit for olfactory behaviour in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 450:63–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06292 pmid:17972877
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Chao MY, Komatsu H, Fukuto HS, Dionne HM, Hart AC (2004) Feeding status and serotonin rapidly and reversibly modulate a Caenorhabditis elegans chemosensory circuit. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:15512–15517. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0403369101 pmid:15492222
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Chen B, Liu Q, Ge Q, Xie J, Wang ZW (2007) UNC-1 regulates gap junctions important to locomotion in C. elegans. Curr Biol 17:1334–1339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.060
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Choudhary B, Marx O, Norris AD (2021) Spliceosomal component PRP-40 is a central regulator of microexon splicing. Cell Rep 36:109464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109464 pmid:34348142
    OpenUrlPubMed
  9. ↵
    Cullinan MM, Klipp RC, Bankston JR (2021) Regulation of acid-sensing ion channels by protein binding partners. Channels (Austin) 15:635–647. https://doi.org/10.1080/19336950.2021.1976946
    OpenUrl
  10. ↵
    Del Mármol J, Yedlin MA, Ruta V (2021) The structural basis of odorant recognition in insect olfactory receptors. Nature 597:126–131. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03794-8 pmid:34349260
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Dibattista M, Al Koborssy D, Genovese F, Reisert J (2021) The functional relevance of olfactory marker protein in the vertebrate olfactory system: a never-ending story. Cell Tissue Res 383:409–427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00441-020-03349-9
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. ↵
    Dobin A, Davis CA, Schlesinger F, Drenkow J, Zaleski C, Jha S, Batut P, Chaisson M, Gingeras TR (2013) STAR: ultrafast universal RNA-seq aligner. Bioinformatics 29:15–21. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/bts635
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Ferkey DM, Sengupta P, L’Etoile ND (2021) Chemosensory signal transduction in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 217:iyab004. https://doi.org/10.1093/genetics/iyab004
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    Genetet S, Desrames A, Chouali Y, Ripoche P, Lopez C, Mouro-Chanteloup I (2017) Stomatin modulates the activity of the anion exchanger 1 (AE1, SLC4A1). Sci Rep 7:46170. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep46170 pmid:28387307
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Goldstein BJ, Kulaga HM, Reed RR (2003) Cloning and characterization of SLP3: a novel member of the stomatin family expressed by olfactory receptor neurons. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 4:74–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162-002-2039-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Goodman MB, Ernstrom GG, Chelur DS, O’Hagan R, Yao CA, Chalfie M (2002) MEC-2 regulates C. elegans DEG/ENaC channels needed for mechanosensation. Nature 415:1039–1042. https://doi.org/10.1038/4151039a pmid:11875573
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Green JB, Young JPW (2008) Slipins: ancient origin, duplication and diversification of the stomatin protein family. BMC Evol Biol 8:44. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-8-44
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Hart AC, Chao MY (2010) From odors to behaviors in Caenorhabditis elegans. In: The neurobiology of olfaction (Menini A, ed). Boca Raton: CRC/Taylor and Francis.
  19. ↵
    Huang M, Gu G, Ferguson EL, Chalfie M (1995) A stomatin-like protein necessary for mechanosensation in C. elegans. Nature 378:292–295. https://doi.org/10.1038/378292a0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Huber TB, et al. (2006) Podocin and MEC-2 bind cholesterol to regulate the activity of associated ion channels. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:17079–17086. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0607465103
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    Kanageswaran N, Demond M, Nagel M, Schreiner BSP, Baumgart S, Scholz P, Altmüller J, Becker C, Doerner JF, Conrad H, Oberland S, Wetzel CH, Neuhaus EM, Hatt H, Gisselmann G (2015) Deep sequencing of the murine olfactory receptor neuron transcriptome. PLoS One 10:e0113170. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0113170
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Katada S, Hirokawa T, Oka Y, Suwa M, Touhara K (2005) Structural basis for a broad but selective ligand spectrum of a mouse olfactory receptor: mapping the odorant-binding site. J Neurosci 25:1806–1815. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4723-04.2005
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    Katju V, Lynch M (2003) The structure and early evolution of recently arisen gene duplicates in the Caenorhabditis elegans genome. Genetics 165:1793–1803. https://doi.org/10.1093/genetics/165.4.1793 pmid:14704166
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    Kobayakawa K, Hayashi R, Morita K, Miyamichi K, Oka Y, Tsuboi A, Sakano H (2002) Stomatin-related olfactory protein, SRO, specifically expressed in the murine olfactory sensory neurons. J Neurosci 22:5931–5937. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-14-05931.2002 pmid:12122055
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Kobayakawa K, Kobayakawa R, Matsumoto H, Oka Y, Imai T, Ikawa M, Okabe M, Ikeda T, Itohara S, Kikusui T, Mori K, Sakano H (2007) Innate versus learned odour processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature 450:503–508. pmid:17989651
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Lehmkuhl AM, Dirr ER, Fleming SM (2014) Olfactory assays for mouse models of neurodegenerative disease. J Vis Exp (90):e51804 https://doi.org/10.3791/51804 pmid:25177842
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Liang X, Calovich-Benne C, Norris A (2022) Sensory neuron transcriptomes reveal complex neuron-specific function and regulation of mec-2/stomatin splicing. Nucleic Acids Res 50:2401–2416. https://doi.org/10.1093/nar/gkab1134 pmid:34875684
    OpenUrlPubMed
  28. ↵
    Nathan BP, Yost J, Litherland MT, Struble RG, Switzer PV (2004) Olfactory function in apoE knockout mice. Behav Brain Res 150:1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00219-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Price MP, Thompson RJ, Eshcol JO, Wemmie JA, Benson CJ (2004) Stomatin modulates gating of acid-sensing ion channels. J Biol Chem 279:53886–53891. https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M407708200 pmid:15471860
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    Qiu Q, Wu Y, Ma L, Xu W, Hills M, Ramalingam V, Yu CR (2021) Acquisition of innate odor preference depends on spontaneous and experiential activities during critical period. Elife 10:e60546. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60546
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    Rajaram S, Spangler TL, Sedensky MM, Morgan PG (1999) A stomatin and a degenerin interact to control anesthetic sensitivity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 153:1673–1682. https://doi.org/10.1093/genetics/153.4.1673
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    Ruan J, et al. (2008) TreeFam: 2008 update. Nucleic Acids Res 36:D735–D740. pmid:18056084
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Rungaldier S, Umlauf E, Mairhofer M, Salzer U, Thiele C, Prohaska R (2017) Structure-function analysis of human stomatin: a mutation study. PLoS One 12:e0178646. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178646 pmid:28575093
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Taylor SR, et al. (2021) Molecular topography of an entire nervous system. Cell 184:4329–4347.e23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2021.06.023
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Thomas JH (2006) Analysis of homologous gene clusters in Caenorhabditis elegans reveals striking regional cluster domains. Genetics 172:127–143. https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.104.040030 pmid:16291650
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Wang Q, Rio DC (2018) JUM is a computational method for comprehensive annotation-free analysis of alternative pre-mRNA splicing patterns. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:E8181–E8190.
    OpenUrl
  37. ↵
    Wesson DW, Donahou TN, Johnson MO, Wachowiak M (2008) Sniffing behavior of mice during performance in odor-guided tasks. Chem Senses 33:581–596. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjn029
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Wetzel C, Hu J, Riethmacher D, Benckendorff A, Harder L, Eilers A, Moshourab R, Kozlenkov A, Labuz D, Caspani O, Erdmann B, Machelska H, Heppenstall PA, Lewin GR (2007) A stomatin-domain protein essential for touch sensation in the mouse. Nature 445:206–209. pmid:17167420
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Yang M, Crawley JN (2009) Simple behavioral assessment of mouse olfaction. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter 8:Unit 8.24.
    OpenUrl
  40. ↵
    Zhang S, Arnadottir J, Keller C, Caldwell GA, Yao CA, Chalfie M (2004) MEC-2 is recruited to the putative mechanosensory complex in C. elegans touch receptor neurons through its stomatin-like domain. Curr Biol 14:1888–1896. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.10.030
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Christina Zelano, Northwestern University - Chicago

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Keerthi Krishnan, Adam Dewan.

Reviewers agreed that this manuscript is potentially significant and impactful, and will be of interest to a wide audience. It presents comparable findings across two different species on a novel role for a gene family. Both reviewers raised important concerns that need to be addressed:

Reviewer 1:

The manuscript titled “A conserved role for stomatin domain genes in olfactory behavior” characterizes a new role for mec-2/Stoml3 gene in olfactory behavior. The comparative approach of using C. elegans and mice is a definite plus, in uncovering a novel role, in this study. This reviewer would encourage the authors of the study to clarify/elaborate on the following points, and provide stronger rationale for the explanation of some interpretations in the study.

1. Page 5, line 104 and Figure 1C: mec-2 is considered as “highly expressed in both AWA and AWB neurons”. What is a typical number of FKPM to be considered “highly expressed?” A positive control for high expression or already published reports for high expression would be helpful.

2. Are mRNA levels congruent with protein levels for these genes in C. elegans?

3. Page 5, line 105: What is the connection between AWA, AWB, VD and DD neurons? Are they from the same lineage? Or, work in a circuit together? Why are the VD and DD neurons used for comparison? Please explain.

4. Figure 2K - Is it feasible to “rescue” with double or triple mec-2 isoform re-expressions? Is this a technically difficult experiment to perform?

5. Pg. 10: Where else is Stoml3 RNA and protein expressed in the mouse? Is it possible that Stoml3 is not only expressed in olfactory neurons, while interpreting the olfactory-motor assays? Please discuss.

6. Figure 4. Were the same mice used for all the olfactory assays in a battery of behavioral tests? Please clarify in the “Methods”. A scatter plot would be more useful graphical representations, to see individual variation and response to different stimuli. In Figure 4D, what is the explanation for the rise and dip in time sniffing for both WT and KO between days 1-3? Also, for interpretation of the Results here, is it possible that the Stoml3KO do not care or are not interested in olfactory discrimination or lack of preference between scents, rather than have “impaired ability to distinguish”? Please discuss.

7. Page 15: In the “buried cereal test”, is there a rationale for averaging time across days? The dynamics in unearthing the buried cereal over days would be useful. For example, do the WT get faster each day? Do the KO show similar time every day?

Reviewer 2:

This manuscript provides evidence for a role for stomatin genes in olfactory behavior. This conclusion is not surprising given STOML-3 is expressed in cilia of olfactory sensory neurons and its genetic removal alters transduction (Agostinelli et al, 2021). However, the data from C. elegans and the behavior experiments in mice are interesting and would provide further support for the contribution of this gene to olfactory perception. Thus, I feel that the topic is significant and would be impactful to a broad audience.

I could not find the statistical analysis for the odor attraction assay. The authors have compared the wrong data in the block experiment.

This manuscript provides evidence for a role for stomatin genes in olfactory behavior. This conclusion is not surprising given STOML-3 is expressed in cilia of olfactory sensory neurons and its genetic removal alters transduction (Agostinelli et al, 2021). However, the data from C. elegans and the behavior experiments in mice are interesting and would provide further support for the contribution of this gene to olfactory perception. Thus, I feel that the topic is significant and would be impactful to a broad audience. However, there are several prominent issues with their behavioral experiments in mice that I hope the authors can address.

1. Since mec-2 affect the activity of the mechanosensory channels, could the authors provide evidence that mec-2 knockout worms exhibit normal locomotor activity? Further, the authors also state that unc-1 and sto-4 mutants exhibit reduced locomotion, so a panel showing overall locomotor ability compared to a wt would help support the narrative.

2. Could the authors please explain why morphology was examined in the AWA neuron only, while transduction was only tested in AWC neurons? What about AWB neurons? Further, since this paper could have a broad audience a very brief description of the olfactory circuitry of C. elegans would be helpful.

3. The authors conclude that mec-2 causes a small, but statistically significant decrease in calcium response to isoamyl acetate. Could the authors provide more data on the number of worms tested? Further, this decrease in olfactory response would be better supported with a second odor. However, I do not deem this additional experiment necessary.

4. I have several concerns about the behavioral experiments in mice.

Major 1: In the habituation dishabituation assay, why is there a noticeable increase in the second presentation of the odor? This is highly unusual. An animal should initially investigate a novel odor for the greatest time period upon the first presentation and then subsequently decrease their investigation. This is a very key point because without the second presentation (which is unusual) the investigation of odor 1 and 2 by the mutant animal is flat. This would denote an overall lack of interest in the odor/task (or another confounding issue) not necessarily a discrimination deficit.

Major 2: The authors have incorrectly interpreted their block experiment. A significant change in investigation time between two presentations denotes discrimination, not a difference between the investigation time between two genotypes. Thus, the mutant mice appear to be able to discriminate between a novel scent and their own scent as the statistical comparison would appear to be significant between blocks C and E. The difference between the investigation time between the wild types and mutants denotes a difference of interest (not discrimination), which again could be due to any number of reasons.

Major 3: Is the investigation time for peanut butter and mouse urine significantly different from water? I don’t see any statistical tests for this comparison. This is another key point for the authors conclusions.

Minor 1: Could the authors please provide more details about the analysis of this data? Was the data analyzed by an observer blind to genotype etc? Did the authors measure nasal contact or just X distance from the source? Some of these details are included for the innate attraction test but are missing from the other behavioral assays. The number of animals should be included in the behavioral descriptions.

Minor 2: The Y axis for Figure 4 should be investigation time, not sniffing. Mice are also sniffing when they are not near the stimulus. The error bars are not mentioned - are they stdev? For panels A,B,C, and E individual data points would be more impactful.

Minor 3: Given the widespread expression of stoml3, could the authors provide some behavioral evidence that these animals are relatively normal.

Minor Concerns

Line 30 - FYI - more recent work has called into question the ability of humans to discriminate 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. Gerkin and Castro, 2015 eLife and others.

Line 134 - Although I don’t deem this experiment necessary - I wonder whether the authors could provide more evidence for or against the minimal contribution of unc-1 and sto-4 for olfactory behavior by performing an analogous experiment to “smell the stick” with a non-olfactory stimulus (e.g., heat or light?). A similar reduction in locomotion would support that these genes are making a larger contribution to locomotor ability, while a similar locomotor response to wts could provide evidence for a role in olfactory behavior.

Author Response

We thank the reviewers for their useful comments and suggestions. We have accordingly performed a number of additional experiments and analyses, and altered and/or elaborated on a number of interpretations and conclusions. We believe these changes enhance the methods presentation, data analysis, and data interpretation. Newly-added text in the manuscript, and response to reviewers here, are denoted by blue font.

Synthesis Statement for Author (Required):

Reviewers agreed that this manuscript is potentially significant and impactful, and will be of interest to a wide audience. It presents comparable findings across two different species on a novel role for a gene family. Both reviewers raised important concerns that need to be addressed:

Reviewer 1:

The manuscript titled “A conserved role for stomatin domain genes in olfactory behavior” characterizes a new role for mec-2/Stoml3 gene in olfactory behavior. The comparative approach of using C. elegans and mice is a definite plus, in uncovering a novel role, in this study. This reviewer would encourage the authors of the study to clarify/elaborate on the following points, and provide stronger rationale for the explanation of some interpretations in the study.

1. Page 5, line 104 and Figure 1C: mec-2 is considered as “highly expressed in both AWA and AWB neurons”. What is a typical number of FKPM to be considered “highly expressed?” A positive control for high expression or already published reports for high expression would be helpful.

We have now added a positive control: osm-6, which is often used as a marker of ciliated sensory neurons (including olfactory neurons). We detect expression of osm-6 in olfactory neurons but not motor neurons as expected (Fig S1A), and the FPKM values are on par with those of the Stomatin domain genes tested in olfactory neurons.

2. Are mRNA levels congruent with protein levels for these genes in C. elegans?

We have now added text discussing that for both unc-1 and mec-2, gene-specific translational reporters are congruent with the mRNA levels tested here (lines 60-62 and 113). For sto-4 and other genes, this remains to be determined.

3. Page 5, line 105: What is the connection between AWA, AWB, VD and DD neurons? Are they from the same lineage? Or, work in a circuit together? Why are the VD and DD neurons used for comparison? Please explain.

We have now added a brief explanation as to our rationale in choosing which neurons to examine (lines 106-108). The main goal was to test olfactory sensory neurons (AWA and AWB) and compare them to control neuron types that are not sensory neurons. Deep single-cell transcriptome data are currently only available for a handful of neurons, restricting our possible choices, and we settled on olfactory neurons versus motor neurons. We also felt this would provide a nice parallel to available cell-specific sequencing data from mouse, in which olfactory and motor neuron RNA Seq data is available.

4. Figure 2K - Is it feasible to “rescue” with double or triple mec-2 isoform re-expressions? Is this a technically difficult experiment to perform?

This experiment would be modestly difficult to perform, but possible. We speculate, however, that the reason for only partial rescue with each cDNA is not that all three isoforms are simultaneously required, but rather that ectopic transgenic rescue is not fully efficient. We suspect this because (1) The AWB neuron normally expresses only a single mec-2 isoform, and (2) we previously showed that endogenous editing of the genome (as opposed to transgenic overexpression) to provide only the mec-2B isoform results in nearly 100% rescue.

5. Pg. 10: Where else is Stoml3 RNA and protein expressed in the mouse? Is it possible that Stoml3 is not only expressed in olfactory neurons, while interpreting the olfactory-motor assays? Please discuss.

We have now added some discussion on this point, including that Stoml3 has previously been shown (and in agreement with our transcriptomic data) to be highly restricted in expression, detectable specifically in olfactory neurons and mechanosensory neurons of the DRG. Which suggests that the olfactory behavior defects in mice, as demonstrated in worms, are likely (though not 100% conclusively) to be cell-autonomous in nature (lines 290-295).

6. Figure 4. Were the same mice used for all the olfactory assays in a battery of behavioral tests? Please clarify in the “Methods”.

We have now added this information in the Methods section, that the same mice were used across all behavioral assays.

A scatter plot would be more useful graphical representations, to see individual variation and response to different stimuli.

We have now added individual data points overlaid on these graphs.

In Figure 4D, what is the explanation for the rise and dip in time sniffing for both WT and KO between days 1-3? Also, for interpretation of the Results here, is it possible that the Stoml3KO do not care or are not interested in olfactory discrimination or lack of preference between scents, rather than have “impaired ability to distinguish”? Please discuss.

We speculate that this phenomenon is due to the fact that each trial was allotted 30 seconds (as in PMID 25177842) as opposed to 2 minutes (as in PMID 19575474 17991495 and others), and the fact that mice were housed 24 hours prior to testing (as in PMID 25177842 17623023) rather than being transferred to fresh cages prior to testing (as in PMID 9575474 34458470). Together we believe this resulted in a delay for the mice (both wild-type and mutant) to begin investigating on the 1st trial. We noticed anecdotally in reviewing the behavioral videos that mice took longer to begin investigation during the 1st trial than in the 2nd trial. However, once the mice began to investigate during the 1st trial, they continued to investigate robustly. As such, we speculate that if the trials were designed for longer duration, the investigation time of the 1st trial would equal or surpass that of the 2nd trial.

Importantly, the increase in investigation time from trial 1 to trial 2 is seen in both wild-type controls as well as Stoml3 mutants, and therefore should not alter the conclusion that wild-types strongly dishabituate from trial 6 to 7, while Stoml3-/- do not. Nevertheless, we have also added some additional discussion as to the possibility that Stoml3 mice could be uninterested, as opposed to unable, to distinguish and respond to olfactory cues (lines 252-253 and 264-265).

7. Page 15: In the “buried cereal test”, is there a rationale for averaging time across days? The dynamics in unearthing the buried cereal over days would be useful. For example, do the WT get faster each day? Do the KO show similar time every day?

We now present both the original averaged data, and also the dynamics (Fig 4C and S4B). We did not identify any significant differences between wild-type and KO. There is a trend, but not statistically significant, toward faster unearthing of the pellet over time in WT.

Reviewer 2:

This manuscript provides evidence for a role for stomatin genes in olfactory behavior. This conclusion is not surprising given STOML-3 is expressed in cilia of olfactory sensory neurons and its genetic removal alters transduction (Agostinelli et al, 2021). However, the data from C. elegans and the behavior experiments in mice are interesting and would provide further support for the contribution of this gene to olfactory perception. Thus, I feel that the topic is significant and would be impactful to a broad audience.

I could not find the statistical analysis for the odor attraction assay. The authors have compared the wrong data in the block experiment.

This manuscript provides evidence for a role for stomatin genes in olfactory behavior. This conclusion is not surprising given STOML-3 is expressed in cilia of olfactory sensory neurons and its genetic removal alters transduction (Agostinelli et al, 2021). However, the data from C. elegans and the behavior experiments in mice are interesting and would provide further support for the contribution of this gene to olfactory perception. Thus, I feel that the topic is significant and would be impactful to a broad audience. However, there are several prominent issues with their behavioral experiments in mice that I hope the authors can address.

1. Since mec-2 affect the activity of the mechanosensory channels, could the authors provide evidence that mec-2 knockout worms exhibit normal locomotor activity? Further, the authors also state that unc-1 and sto-4 mutants exhibit reduced locomotion, so a panel showing overall locomotor ability compared to a wt would help support the narrative.

We have now added a panel specifically measuring locomotory ability (Fig S2A) which shows poor locomotory ability in unc-1 and sto-4 mutants compared to wild type, and better locomotory ability in the mec-2 and chemotaxis-negative che-2 mutants.

2. Could the authors please explain why morphology was examined in the AWA neuron only, while transduction was only tested in AWC neurons? What about AWB neurons? Further, since this paper could have a broad audience a very brief description of the olfactory circuitry of C. elegans would be helpful.

We have now added a brief description of the olfactory circuitry of C. elegans (lines 100-105). We have also now added a new experiment in which we examined morphology of AWB and AWC neurons as well (Figure S2C-D) and find no obvious morphological defects in mec-2 mutants, similar to our findings in the AWA neuron.

3. The authors conclude that mec-2 causes a small, but statistically significant decrease in calcium response to isoamyl acetate. Could the authors provide more data on the number of worms tested? Further, this decrease in olfactory response would be better supported with a second odor. However, I do not deem this additional experiment necessary.

We have now added this information in the figure legend (Figure 2G legend).

4. I have several concerns about the behavioral experiments in mice.

Major 1: In the habituation dishabituation assay, why is there a noticeable increase in the second presentation of the odor? This is highly unusual. An animal should initially investigate a novel odor for the greatest time period upon the first presentation and then subsequently decrease their investigation. This is a very key point because without the second presentation (which is unusual) the investigation of odor 1 and 2 by the mutant animal is flat. This would denote an overall lack of interest in the odor/task (or another confounding issue) not necessarily a discrimination deficit.

[Same text as response to Reviewer 1, above]: We speculate that this phenomenon is due to the fact that each trial was allotted 30 seconds (as in PMID 25177842) as opposed to 2 minutes (as in PMID 19575474 17991495 and others), and the fact that mice were housed 24 hours prior to testing (as in PMID 25177842 17623023) rather than being transferred to fresh cages prior to testing (as in PMID 9575474 34458470). Together we believe this resulted in a delay for the mice (both wild-type and mutant) to begin investigating on the 1st trial. We noticed anecdotally in reviewing the behavioral videos that mice took longer to begin investigation during the 1st trial than in the 2nd trial. However, once the mice began to investigate during the 1st trial, they continued to investigate robustly. As such, we speculate that if the trials were designed for longer duration, the investigation time of the 1st trial would equal or surpass that of the 2nd trial.

Importantly, the increase in investigation time from trial 1 to trial 2 is seen in both wild-type controls as well as Stoml3 mutants, and therefore should not alter the conclusion that wild-types strongly dishabituate from trial 6 to 7, while Stoml3-/- do not. Nevertheless, we have also added some additional discussion as to the possibility that Stoml3 mice could be uninterested, as opposed to unable, to distinguish and respond to olfactory cues (lines 252-253 and 264-265).

Major 2: The authors have incorrectly interpreted their block experiment. A significant change in investigation time between two presentations denotes discrimination, not a difference between the investigation time between two genotypes. Thus, the mutant mice appear to be able to discriminate between a novel scent and their own scent as the statistical comparison would appear to be significant between blocks C and E. The difference between the investigation time between the wild types and mutants denotes a difference of interest (not discrimination), which again could be due to any number of reasons.

We have now updated the analysis and interpretation (Fig 4E and lines 262-265). We show that indeed the mutant mice are able to significantly distinguish between novel and self scents, although the magnitude of the effect is significantly smaller than that of wild type. We have accordingly added additional interpretation and discussion that this could be due to reduced interest/motivation, discriminatory ability, or a combination of factors (lines 262-265).

Major 3: Is the investigation time for peanut butter and mouse urine significantly different from water? I don’t see any statistical tests for this comparison. This is another key point for the authors conclusions.

We have now added two statistical analyses: first, comparing wild-type to Stoml3 mutant investigation times, in which no significant differences are observed. Second, comparing within genotypes the investigation times of peanut butter or urine to water. Since some of the trends in these comparisons do not reach statistical significance, we have accordingly altered the corresponding interpretation of these experiments (Lines 232-236).

Minor 1: Could the authors please provide more details about the analysis of this data? Was the data analyzed by an observer blind to genotype etc? Did the authors measure nasal contact or just X distance from the source? Some of these details are included for the innate attraction test but are missing from the other behavioral assays. The number of animals should be included in the behavioral descriptions.

These details have now been added uniformly for each behavioral assay, with information provided in both the figure legend for Figure 4 and in each respective methods section. For details common to all assays (e.g. that the data was analyzed by an observer blind to genotype), these details are provided in lines 353-357.

Minor 2: The Y axis for Figure 4 should be investigation time, not sniffing. Mice are also sniffing when they are not near the stimulus. The error bars are not mentioned - are they stdev? For panels A,B,C, and E individual data points would be more impactful.

Figure 4 Y axes have now been updated, and Figure 4 legend now reports that error bars represent S.E.M. We have also overlaid individual data points for panels 4A, B, C, and E.

Minor 3: Given the widespread expression of stoml3, could the authors provide some behavioral evidence that these animals are relatively normal.

We have now added some additional observational evidence that the Stoml3 mutant animals are normal, both from our observations and from more targeted in-depth behavioral analysis in previous analyses (Lines 224-227).

Minor Concerns

Line 30 - FYI - more recent work has called into question the ability of humans to discriminate 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. Gerkin and Castro, 2015 eLife and others.

We have now edited the phrasing in the introduction to account for these points (lines 28-29).

Line 134 - Although I don’t deem this experiment necessary - I wonder whether the authors could provide more evidence for or against the minimal contribution of unc-1 and sto-4 for olfactory behavior by performing an analogous experiment to “smell the stick” with a non-olfactory stimulus (e.g., heat or light?). A similar reduction in locomotion would support that these genes are making a larger contribution to locomotor ability, while a similar locomotor response to wts could provide evidence for a role in olfactory behavior.

We have done some preliminary experiments very similar to those suggested, out of curiosity, using heat as a stimulus. Indeed, both unc-1 and sto-4 are poor at thermotaxis as well, suggesting an overall locomotory ability defect. However, we are still working to perfect this experimental paradigm in the lab.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 10 (3)
eNeuro
Vol. 10, Issue 3
March 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A Conserved Role for Stomatin Domain Genes in Olfactory Behavior
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
A Conserved Role for Stomatin Domain Genes in Olfactory Behavior
Xiaoyu Liang, Morgan Taylor, Rebekah Napier-Jameson, Canyon Calovich-Benne, Adam Norris
eNeuro 1 March 2023, 10 (3) ENEURO.0457-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0457-22.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
A Conserved Role for Stomatin Domain Genes in Olfactory Behavior
Xiaoyu Liang, Morgan Taylor, Rebekah Napier-Jameson, Canyon Calovich-Benne, Adam Norris
eNeuro 1 March 2023, 10 (3) ENEURO.0457-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0457-22.2023
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
    • Author Response
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • behavior
  • bhemotaxis
  • msec-2
  • stomatin
  • stoml3
  • olfaction

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Characterization of the Tau Interactome in Human Brain Reveals Isoform-Dependent Interaction with 14-3-3 Family Proteins
  • The Mobility of Neurofilaments in Mature Myelinated Axons of Adult Mice
Show more Research Article: New Research

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Different control strategies drive interlimb differences in performance and adaptation during reaching movements in novel dynamics
  • The nasal solitary chemosensory cell signaling pathway triggers mouse avoidance behavior to inhaled nebulized irritants
  • Taste-odor association learning alters the dynamics of intra-oral odor responses in the posterior piriform cortex of awake rats
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.