Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: New Research, Development

Development of the Functional Connectome Topology in Adolescence: Evidence from Topological Data Analysis

Zeus Gracia-Tabuenca, Juan Carlos Díaz-Patiño, Isaac Arelio-Ríos, Martha Beatriz Moreno-García, Fernando A. Barrios and Sarael Alcauter
eNeuro 30 January 2023, 10 (2) ENEURO.0296-21.2022; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0296-21.2022
Zeus Gracia-Tabuenca
1Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Querétaro 76230, México
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Juan Carlos Díaz-Patiño
1Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Querétaro 76230, México
2Centro de Investigación Científica y de Educación Superior de Ensenada, Ensenada 22860, México
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Isaac Arelio-Ríos
1Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Querétaro 76230, México
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Martha Beatriz Moreno-García
1Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Querétaro 76230, México
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Martha Beatriz Moreno-García
Fernando A. Barrios
1Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Querétaro 76230, México
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Fernando A. Barrios
Sarael Alcauter
1Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Querétaro 76230, México
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Sarael Alcauter
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Adolescence is a crucial developmental period in terms of behavior and mental health. Therefore, understanding how the brain develops during this stage is a fundamental challenge for neuroscience. Recent studies have modeled the brain as a network or connectome, mainly applying measures from graph theory, showing a change in its functional organization, such as an increase in its segregation and integration. Topological Data Analysis (TDA) complements such modeling by extracting high-dimensional features across the whole range of connectivity values instead of exploring a fixed set of connections. This study inquires into the developmental trajectories of such properties using a longitudinal sample of typically developing human participants (N = 98; 53/45 female/male; 6.7–18.1 years), applying TDA to their functional connectomes. In addition, we explore the effect of puberty on individual developmental trajectories. Results showed that the adolescent brain has a more distributed topology structure compared with random networks but is more densely connected at the local level. Furthermore, developmental effects showed nonlinear trajectories for the topology of the whole brain and fronto-parietal networks, with an inflection point and increasing trajectories after puberty onset. These results add to the insights into the development of the functional organization of the adolescent brain.

  • adolescence
  • functional connectivity
  • persistence homology
  • puberty
  • resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging
  • topological data analysis

Significance Statement

Topological Data Analysis (TDA) may be used to explore the topology of the brain along the whole range of connectivity values instead of selecting only a fixed set of connectivity thresholds. Here, we explored some properties of the topology of the brain’s functional connectome and how they develop in adolescence. We show that developmental trajectories are nonlinear and better adjusted by puberty status than chronological age, with an inflection point around the onset of puberty. In particular, the results show that the topology of the fronto-parietal network is the one that drives the functional connectome changes in the adolescent period.

Introduction

Adolescence is a critical development period that substantially impacts the body and behavior. Notably, the brain undergoes structural and functional changes influenced by pubertal hormones (Vijayakumar et al., 2018; Laube et al., 2020). Moreover, these changes occur along with a consolidation of cognitive and executive performance (Baum et al., 2017; Chai et al., 2017).

These insights have been addressed by modeling the brain as a complex network of interacting nodes at task or rest conditions (Biswal et al., 1995; Smith et al., 2009). In this framework, the functional connectome is described by its system properties in biologically plausible terms, mainly using measures from graph theory (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). Although graph metrics provide an effective framework to describe brain organization properties, they also have some limitations. The main limitation is that graph theory relies on pairwise connections as the basic unit of the network; in this regard high-order interactions within the network cannot be properly handled (Lord et al., 2016). Another potential limitation is the common step of thresholding, which is to discard a subset of the network connections based on several criteria (e.g., statistically, low values, sign, etc.). There is no consensus about this step, and functional connectomes tend to be unstable at different thresholds (Garrison et al., 2015), although group inferences may dramatically change based on thresholding (Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020). Nevertheless, other methods have recently been applied to address high-dimensional data, such as Topological Data Analysis (TDA; Sizemore et al., 2018; Expert et al., 2019). TDA models the connectome as a topological space and characterizes its interaction patterns as geometric features, allowing it to simplify complex structures at different scales (Giusti et al., 2015; Santos et al., 2019; Centeno et al., 2022). In particular, TDA applied to functional connectomes is not affected by the potential biases of connectivity thresholding nor brain segmentation (H. Lee et al., 2012; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020); additionally, TDA is a suitable tool to address longitudinal data since it can extract invariant topological features into longitudinal designs (Fülöp et al., 2020). This methodology has been used for over a decade in neuroimaging (Chung et al. 2009; H. Lee et al., 2011, 2012, 2019; Choi et al., 2014; M.H. Lee et al., 2015; Chung et al., 2019) but is not the standard procedure in neuroimaging. Furthermore, it explores different properties to those explored with graph theory or other network approaches that typically explore the fully connected network [when only one single component is present, i.e., when Betti-0 (B0) = 1].

In terms of the functional organization of the brain, previous cross-sectional studies have shown that the adolescent period is characterized by an increase in modularity and specialization (Fair et al., 2009; Satterthwaite et al., 2013a; Gu et al., 2015), with prominent effects in frontal and parietal systems, along with executive performance (Marek et al., 2015; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2021). However, as far as we are concerned, TDA in human connectomes has mainly been applied to neuropsychiatric disorders (H. Lee et al., 2012, 2017; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020; Li et al., 2021) but not to characterize the typical development. There is still a huge degree of incertitude in this field because of the great variability between samples, sexes, and cultures (Sawyer et al., 2018), with special emphasis on the fact that some individuals have faster or slower pubertal development even when they have the same chronological age (Blakemore et al., 2010; Vijayakumar et al., 2018). In this regard, longitudinal trajectories and pubertal markers are highly valuable in describing adolescent development.

Therefore, this study focuses on how the topology structure of the neurotypical functional connectome changes during the adolescent period. In order to achieve that, TDA features were extracted from the functional brain connectivity of a longitudinal sample of typically developing subjects, and (non)linear trends were tested from chronological age and pubertal status.

Materials and Methods

Sample

A general invitation was sent to local schools describing the study protocols and the inclusion/exclusion criteria. Inclusion criteria consisted of a full-term gestation (≥37 weeks). Exclusion criteria included academic year repetition and any neurologic or psychiatric disorder identified with the MINI semi-structured interview. Signed informed consent for parents and verbal assent for minors was required. The study protocols followed the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the Institutional Ethics Board.

The sample comprised 98 typically developing participants (53 females, 45 males; age range: 6.7–18.1 years old). Of those, 41 returned for a second session, and 16 for a third. Initially, the study was designed as a cross-sectional study. However, to include intraindividual development in the growth charts, some participants agreed to participate in one or two follow-up sessions. As a result, follow-ups occurred after five years and the second after two years, respectively (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A, Longitudinal plot: each dot represents a subject at the age of assessment. Lines represent longitudinal assessments of a subject. B, Pubertal Development Scale scores (PDS) along age. Thin lines represent individual trajectories; thick lines represent locally estimated scatterplot smoothing (LOESS) curves per sex group (with 95% confidence-interval shadow) after controlling for intraindividual trends. F, female; M, male; QC, session with excessive motion artifact. Figures were modified from Gracia-Tabuenca et al. (2021) under CC BY 4.0 license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Pubertal status assessment

Participants fulfilled the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen et al., 1988). PDS averages the response of five self-reported questions about growth spurt in height, pubic hair, and skin change for both sexes; plus breast growth and menarche for females and facial hair growth and voice change for males. Responses are absence (1), first signs (2), evident (3), and finished (4) pubertal spurt. Those participants under 10 years old were set to PDS level 1, following similar values in previous studies (Hibberd et al., 2015; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2019). In addition, 8 missing values (four females) were estimated via generalized additive mixed model (GAMM) with age-sex interaction locally estimated scatterplot smoothing (LOESS) curves (according to Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2021).

Imaging

Participants underwent an magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) protocol for each session, including a whole-brain functional MRI (fMRI) sequence plus high-resolution T1-weighted images for anatomic reference. After five “dummy” volumes for scan stabilization, a total of 150 fMRI volumes were obtained using a gradient recalled T2* echoplanar imaging sequence (TR/TE = 2000/40 ms, voxel size 4 × 4 × 4 mm3). Participants were instructed to lay down, close their eyes, and not fall asleep. In order to ease participants to remain awake, the fMRI scan was applied at the beginning of the MRI session and always in the morning. T1 images were obtained using a 3D spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR) acquisition (TR/TE = 8.1/3.2 ms, flip angle = 12.0, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm3). All brain imaging was acquired with a 3T MR GE750 Discovery scanner (General Electric) using an 8-channel-array head coil. However, 20 sessions were acquired with a 32-channel coil; thus, a covariate was included in the subsequent analyses.

Preprocessing

Structural T1 volumes were denoised with nonlocal means (Manjón et al., 2010) and N4 bias field correction (Tustison et al., 2010). fMRI datasets were preprocessed using FSL v.5.0.6 (Jenkinson et al., 2012; RRID:SCR_002823). Preprocessing steps included slice timing, head motion correction, brain extraction, intensity normalization, confound regression, spatial normalization, and 0.01- to 0.08-Hz bandpass filtering.

Considering that the pediatric population tends to move more inside the scanner (Satterthwaite et al., 2012), we implemented a strident strategy of confounding variables regression (Satterthwaite et al., 2013b). A total of 36 parameters were regressed out from the fMRI time series, including the six head-motion estimated parameters plus the average time series of the global signal, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. The derivatives of these nine variables were also added, and the quadratic terms of those eighteen. Additionally, the volumes with a framewise displacement (FD-RMS; Jenkinson et al., 2002) >0.25 mm (“spikes”) were included as confounds as well. This approach overpowers other widely used motion-mitigation methods (Ciric et al., 2017; Parkes et al., 2018; Taymourtash et al., 2019; Graff et al., 2022). Eighteen sessions with <4 min without spike-volumes were discarded (Satterthwaite et al., 2013b; Parkes et al., 2018); therefore, the final sample consisted of 89 participants (39 male, age range: 6.7–18.1 years old), of whom 37 and 11 had two and three longitudinal sessions, respectively.

In addition, fMRI datasets were co-registered to their T1 volume with six degrees of freedom and then warped twice using nonlinear SyN transformation (Avants et al., 2008; RRID:SCR_004757) to a pediatric template (NIHPD4.5–18.5; Fonov et al., 2011) and then to the MNI-152 standard template.

Functional connectomes

Brain networks were defined based on 264 regions of interest (ROIs) as nodes (Power et al., 2011). Pairwise edges were calculated through Pearson’s correlation between the average fMRI preprocessed signal of every pair of ROIs.

These ROIs consist of 5-mm radius spheres with high consistency in task and rest tested in large fMRI databases (Power et al., 2011). Moreover, this set of ROIs can be grouped in thirteen functional networks. This segmentation has been applied in numerous pediatric studies (Satterthwaite et al., 2013a; Gu et al., 2015; Marek et al., 2015; Chai et al., 2017; Ciric et al., 2017; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020, 2021).

Topological data analysis

The functional connectome can be modeled as a topological space using the Rips complex, defined as Rips(F, ε). F stands for the set of nodes (same as the connectome nodes), and ε stands for the filtration value that indicates if a pair of nodes of F are connected (those with a distance lower than ε, see below the definition of distance). Thus, the set of connected nodes of the Rips complex varies as a function of ε. Additionally, topological features can be extracted from the Rips complexes, the so-called Betti numbers. Specifically, Betti numbers of order zero or Betti-0 (B0) accounts for the number of components (i.e., the number of groups of connected nodes and isolated nodes), Betti-1 (B1) accounts for the number of 1-dimensional holes which correspond to cycles in the network (Fig. 2), and so on (for an extensive review on TDA, we suggest Sizemore et al., 2019; Edelsbrunner and Harer, 2010).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Betti-0 (B0) and Betti-1 (B1) for different filtration values. Illustrative example with 20 nodes, five filtration values ε, represented as the circle diameter and their corresponding B0 and B1 values. At ε = 0, the number of components (B0) equals the number of nodes, and there are no holes. As the filtration value increases, the number of components (B0) decreases until they form a single component containing all the nodes. Meanwhile, the number of holes (B1) starts at 0, increases, and finally goes back to 0 again. A hole of dimension 1 is a cycle with four or more edges (highlighted with green edges); the simplices of dimension >1 are colored in light blue for each filtration value ε.

In this study, we focused exclusively on B0 and B1. When ε = 0, B0 equals the number of nodes. As ε increases, B0 decreases and eventually will reach a single component where every node of F is connected (Fig. 2). In contrast, at low values of ε, there are no holes in the connected pattern of the topological space because there are not enough connections to build them. Similarly, at high values of ε, the holes are “filled” because all pairwise connections within the component are accomplished. These holes represent serially distributed connections of nodes without shortcuts, while a filled hole means that those nodes are densely connected between them (Sizemore et al., 2018). Therefore, the greater amount of holes (i.e., B1) is reached at intermediate values of ε. Both processes can be characterized as Betti curves as a function of ε (Fig. 3).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Sample intercepts for the B0 (A) and B1 (B) curves are in blue. Average of 1000 bootstrapped connectomes with random edge-rewiring curves in gray. Every average with 95% confidence interval. The blue bars are centered at the sample intercept filtration for starting/ending values with 95% confidence interval in dashed red. Extended Data Figure 3-1 also shows that the persistence of topological holes (B1) shown in B is directly related to the number of holes.

Extended Data Figure 3-1

Scatter-plot of Betti-1 area under the curve (B1-AUC) against the number (#) of topological holes, the red line corresponds to the fitted line with its corresponding 95% confidence interval in gray. Intercept at 60.16 and slope of 14.7, R2 = 0.89 (p < 2.2e-16). Intrasubject random-effects were regressed-out from both variables. Download Figure 3-1, TIF file.

The distance between nodes was set as one minus their corresponding Pearson’s correlation (i.e., their functional connectivity edge), following H. Lee et al. (2012): d(ξ, xj) = 1 − r(ξ, xj), r being the Pearson’s correlation between nodes ξ and xj. B0 and B1 curves were computed using the TDA R-package (Fasy et al., 2014), and were summarized employing the area under the curve (AUC). This metric, also known as a Persistence Indicator Function and Total Persistence, is a norm associated with the persistence diagrams (Rieck et al., 2020). In addition, stability properties have been proved (Cohen-Steiner et al., 2010), and; it also has been used for the classification of subjects in fMRI datasets (Caputi et al., 2021). The AUC accounts for the overall process of the Betti numbers along all possible values of ε. Low scores of B0-AUC can be interpreted as a fast transition to the single component. In contrast, higher scores imply a more distributed configuration of the nodes that impedes a rapid transition to a single component. Meanwhile, low scores of B1-AUC mean that nodes rapidly bind to one another, fulfilling the topological holes, and higher scores imply an increase in the persistence of such holes, but also in the number of holes within the network (i.e., a less densely connected network; see Extended Data Fig. 3-1).

Furthermore, to discard that the observed results can be obtained by chance, a null distribution of Betti curves was generated by bootstrapping 1000 connectomes extracted from the original sample whose edges were randomly rewired (Giusti et al., 2015; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020).

Developmental trajectories

Developmental effects were tested using linear mixed-effects (LME) and nonlinear generalized additive mixed models (GAMM). We opted for GAMM because it allows for testing several nonlinear trends simultaneously and can address nonparametric regression estimators (Lin et al., 2004). Additionally, given that PDS is an ordinal and not continuous variable, GAMM is a suitable modeling approach. Eight models were applied: two LME for age and age-sex interaction, six GAMM fitting smooth splines for age and age-by-sex, PDS and PDS-by-sex, and Age-PDS interaction and Age-PDS interaction-by-sex. The longitudinal dimensions of the sample were modeled by intraindividual intercepts, which accounted for the random-effects of the models, and were estimated via maximum likelihood. Additionally, every model included the average head motion (FD-RMS) and coil as confounds. Models were implemented using R libraries: LME via lme4 (Bates et al., 2007; RRID:SCR_015654), GAMM via gamm4 (Wood et al., 2017). The selection of the best model was set by the lowest Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1974). The AIC evals a model by the trade-off between its complexity and its goodness of fit. That is, the subtraction between the number of parameters (k) and the log-likelihood function (lnL) by a factor of two (i.e., AIC = 2k − 2lnL). Thus, based on information theory, the best model is the one with the minimum AIC value. Model assumptions were evaluated with Shapiro–Wilk normality tests on the residuals and random-effects (Verbeke and Lesaffre, 1996). Developmental terms within the model with lower AIC were tested via F tests (Wood, 2013).

In addition, the model with lower AIC at the whole-brain level was applied for the thirteen functional networks of the Power et al. (2011) segmentation, where their corresponding significance was corrected for multiple testing using a false discovery rate (FDR) q < 0.05 (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

Code accessibility

All preprocessed data and the code described in this study are freely available online at https://github.com/BrainMapINB/Pubertal_TDA. Also, the code is available as Extended Data 1. Present results were computed with an Intel Core i7-4790 CPU @ 3.60 GHz × 8 with Ubuntu 18.04.3 LTS 64-bit.

Extended Data 1

Code and materials. Download Extended Data 1 ZIP file

Results

The sample intercept B0 curve, the representative curve for the whole sample, showed an inverse sigmoid pattern with a slower transition to the single component compared with the permuted data (Fig. 3A), which means that the brain topology is more complex compared with a random network. On the other hand, the B1 curve shows a bell shape with a maximum of 104.69 “holes” at 0.432 filtration value, while the permuted data shows a maximum of 183.92 at ε = 0.438 (Fig. 3B). This implies that the brain network has a more densely connected pattern, compared with random networks, and therefore less number of topological holes.

In addition, the average filtration value where B0 curves get to the value of 1 is 0.5474, with a 95% confidence interval of [0.527, 0.5677] (Fig. 3A). The average filtration value where B1 curves begin to increase above 0 is 0.1627, with a 95% confidence interval of [0.1554, 0.1699]. Similarly, the average filtration value where B1 curves return to 0 is 0.718, with a 95% confidence interval of [0.7118, 0.7242] (Fig. 3B). This information means that both the B0 and the B1 curves start and end around the same filtration values. That is, two Betti curves with similar areas will not have extremal differences in their curve profiles.

Regarding model selection for the developmental effects, the GAMM for the PDS showed the lowest AIC for B0-AUC (858.3) and B1-AUC (625.6; Table 1). Additionally, the normality assumption was rejected (based on the nominal α = 0.05) only in the residuals of the linear mixed-models of the B1-AUC, but the assumption remains for all of the nonlinear models as well as all B0-AUC models tested. The B0-AUC and B1-AUC trends along the PDS show an initial increase from level 1 to 2, followed by a soft decline after that (Fig. 4). Both effects were significant (B0-AUC: F = 6.13, EDF = 2.61, p = 0.0064; B1-AUC: F = 5.22, EDF = 2.42, p = 0.012).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Shapiro–Wilk test (SW) for the residuals (e) and random-effects (RE) for Betti-0 (B0) and Betti-1 (B1) areas under the curve (AUC) at every developmental model

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Scatter-plots of the GAMM PDS (left) and age (right) models for the B0 (top) and B1 (bottom) area under de curve (AUC) in relation to the pubertal scale (PDS) or age (in years). Thin lines represent individual trajectories; thick black lines represent the sample curve (with 95% confidence-interval shadow). Smooth spline F statistics are depicted. GAMM-age adjustments have higher AIC than GAM-PDS, but are depicted as a reference. a.u., arbitrary units. The relationship between the PDS or Age and the TDA features residuals after regressing out average motion, head coil, and intraindividual slopes covariates is included in Figure Extended Data Figure 4-1. F, females; M, males.

Extended Data Figure 4-1

Scatter-plots of the GAMM PDS (left) and age (right) models for the B0 (top) and B1 (bottom) area under de curve (AUC) residuals (e) in relation to the pubertal scale (PDS) or age (in years). Average motion, head coil, and intraindividual slopes were regress-out from the dependent variable. Thin lines represent individual trajectories; thick black lines represent the sample curve (with 95% confidence-interval shadow). Smooth spline F statistics are depicted. GAMM-age adjustments have higher AIC than GAM-PDS, but are depicted as a reference. Download Figure 4-1, TIF file.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Brain maps of the smooth spline’s F value of the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS) GAMM term for B0-AUC at the functional networks level. Mapping was based on ROI corresponding to the consensus area according to Power et al. (2011), using BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013; RRID:SCR_009446).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Scatter-plot of the GAMM model for the B0-AUC of the fronto-parietal network (FPN) in relation to the pubertal scale (PDS). Thin lines represent individual trajectories; thick black lines represent the sample’s smooth spline curve (with 95% confidence-interval shadow). Smooth spline F statistics are depicted. a.u., arbitrary units. The relationship between the PDS and the B0-AUC residuals after regressing out average motion, head coil, and intraindividual slopes covariates is included in Extended Data Figure 6-1. EDF, effective degrees of freedom.

Extended Data Figure 6-1

Scatter-plot of the GAMM model for the B0-AUC residuals (e), after regressing out in-scanner motion and head-coil, of the fronto-parietal network (FPN) in relation to the pubertal scale (PDS). Thin lines represent individual trajectories; thick black lines represent the sample’s smooth spline curve (with 95% confidence-interval shadow). Smooth spline F statistics are depicted. Download Figure 6-1, TIF file.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

Model performance at the RSN, effective degrees of freedom (EDF), F and p values for the development component

Discussion

In this study, we have applied Topological Data Analysis (TDA) on the functional connectomes of a longitudinal sample of typically developing children and adolescents. TDA features show a more distributed connectivity structure compared with random networks. However, when assessing the connectivity structure within the connected nodes, the brain connectomes exhibit a more densely connected pattern than the random networks. Furthermore, this topology develops nonlinearly through adolescence, better adjusted to pubertal status than chronological age. This nonlinear effect exhibits a stronger connectivity of the whole-network and within the fronto-parietal network just after the onset of the pubertal signs.

Regarding the average Betti curves, the sample intercept for the B0 curve showed an inverse sigmoid pattern that replicates previous findings in functional connectivity fMRI (Liang and Wang, 2017; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020; Li et al., 2021) and PET (H. Lee et al., 2012) studies. Furthermore, the average B0 curve of the randomized data reached the single component faster than the observed data, evincing a less distributed network. This random pattern was also replicated in another pediatric sample showing the same faster transition to a single component (Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020). Concerning the B1, both the sample intercept and randomized curves exhibit a bell-shaped curve with an approximately similar filtration value at their maxima but at a lower area for the observed data. This information evinces a connectivity topology structure of a lower number of holes or more densely connected at the local level in the real data compared with the random networks. Thus, B0 tells how fast the whole-network goes from isolated to all-connected nodes, while B1 reflects how densely connected those elements are already connected. It is noticeable that, on average, B0 tends to reach the single component at filtration values of ε = 0.5, but at that point, B1 curves display their peak number of “holes.” This information implies that these TDA features not only reflect different levels of connectivity structure but also occur at different connectivity strengths.

Regarding the developmental effects of the TDA features, several models were tested to address the area under the B0 and B1 curves. Nonlinear additive models show greater goodness of fit compared with linear ones, even controlling for the extra number of parameters. Specifically, when considering the pubertal status (assessed by the PDS) without its sex interaction is the model that better fits the AUC for both B0 and B1. Hence, the development of the functional connectome topology better adjusts pubertal status than chronological age. This is a relevant finding considering that the pubertal status takes into account noncontinuous changes as well as more subtle sex effects than a nonlinear age-sex interaction. No previous studies have addressed the adolescent connectome via TDA, but recent studies have shown a better adjustment with the pubertal status for the developmental trends of the functional connectome (based on graph theory; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2021) or the frontostriatal functional connectivity (van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2019). Previous studies in animal models have shown brain plasticity associated with puberty-related hormonal changes (Sisk and Foster, 2004). Neuroimaging studies controlling for the age effects have also revealed structural changes associated with the puberty stage in humans, mainly showing decreased gray matter density but increased white matter density in later stages (Peper et al., 2009; Perrin et al., 2009; Bramen et al., 2011; Herting et al., 2012). This work contributes to the emerging evidence that puberty onset dramatically influences the development of functional brain connectivity (van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2019; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2021).

Concerning whole-brain inferences, the AUC for both B0 and B1 showed an initial increase from PDS level 1 to 2, but decreased afterward. In contrast, when focusing on the chronological age, the turning point is ∼12 years old, but showing smoother trends compared with PDS. This means a faster transition to the single component for the B0 while a lower rate of geometric holes for the B1 at the end of adolescence when considering PDS or age. But only B0-AUC effects were significant, which evinces that those changes were more prominent at lower filtration values, i.e., edges with higher functional connectivity (see Materials and Methods, Topological data analysis). Previous work on brain functional organization during this period has shown increases along age in the within-network connectivity (Fair et al., 2009; Satterthwaite et al., 2013a; Gu et al., 2015), while others failed to replicate that pattern showing an increase in the between-network functional connectivity instead (Hwang et al., 2013; Marek et al., 2015). In addition, when considering the PDS, it has been shown that functional centrality, segregation, efficiency, and integration increase at the end of adolescence (Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2021). Such divergent results in neurotypical samples can be explained by the use of different samples and methods, such as selection of regions/networks of interest, thresholding, and/or connectome features. However, the present study uses longitudinal nonlinear modeling with features from the TDA framework, which is resilient thresholding and can extract high-order patterns at the local and global levels at the same time. All these studies demonstrate the change in the configuration of the brain’s functional organization during adolescence.

Likewise, the B0-AUC along the PDS effect was stronger in the Fronto-Parietal Network (FPN), showing a similar nonlinear trend as that for the whole brain network (Table 2; Figs. 5 and 6). This demonstrates a faster binding of the FPN nodes at the end of adolescence in terms of functional connectivity. The FPN is a key module of the connectome that is involved in the response to high-demanding tasks (Zanto and Gazzaley, 2013) and is a fundamental system for the consolidation of executive behavior in the adolescent period (Baum et al., 2017; Chai et al., 2017). Other works on functional connectomes have reported an increase in the FPN connectivity along with other attention-related systems in late adolescence (Hwang et al., 2013; Marek et al., 2015). In fact, when applying the graph theory measures to this sample (in a previous study from our group, Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2021), we found diverse connectome changes in several functional networks, including attention-related but also primary and subcortical, but when using TDA features the central role of the FPN is clearly stated among the other networks.

Some limitations of this work should be taken into account. We used relatively short scans, which may affect the quality of the data; nonetheless, it was considered sufficient at the time of the first acquisition (Van Dijk et al., 2010). Furthermore, we applied a strident quality control of the data discarding those datasets with <80% good quality data in terms of motion artifact. Nonetheless, Topological Data Analysis (TDA) complements other network modeling strategies by extracting high-dimensional features across the whole range of connectivity values, instead of exploring a fixed set of connections.

In conclusion, the present study focused on the characterization of functional connectomes as topological spaces in a longitudinal sample of typically developing children and adolescents. Observed Topological Data Analysis (TDA) features showed a more distributed structure but with denser local connections compared with random networks. However, during adolescence, this effect changes with a nonlinear trend for the whole-brain and the fronto-parietal network, particularly after the onset of the pubertal signs. These results provide evidence of the nonlinear, puberty-dependent developmental trajectories of the topology of the brain network. With the advantage that these properties arise when exploring the whole range of connectivity strengths instead of focusing on a small set of them. Although, as far as we are concerned, this is the first implementation of TDA into neurotypical development, we have shown that this approach can handle complex data in a multi-session design and effectively detect meaningful changes in the adolescent functional connectome. Being adolescence a critical period for the appearance of the first signs of mental health disorders, we expect these trajectories may be of interest for studying both normal and altered development.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: We are incredibly grateful to the children and parents who participated in this study voluntarily. We thank Nuri Aranda López, Leonor Casanova Rico, César Arturo Dominguez Frausto, Deisy Gasca Martínez, Leopoldo González Santos, Ma. de Lourdes Lara Ayala, Juan J. Ortiz, and Erick H. Pasaye for their technical support. We also thank Rafael Fajardo, Roberto E. Mercadillo, and Edna Navarrete for their support in the psychological assessment and Michael C. Jeziorski for editing this manuscript. We thankfully acknowledge the imaging resources and support provided by the Laboratorio Nacional de Imagenología por Resonancia Magnética, Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (CONACYT) network of national laboratories.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This work was supported by the Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnologia (CONACYT) Fellowship 330142 (to Z.G.-T.) for being a doctoral student of Programa de Doctorado en Ciencias Biomédicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México (UNAM), a Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México-Dirección General de Asuntos del Personal Académico (UNAM-DGAPA) postdoctoral fellowship, and UNAM-Programa de Apoyo a Proyectos de Investigación e Innovación Tecnológica (UNAM-PAPIIT) Grants IN208622 and IN212219 (to S.A.).

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Akaike H (1974) A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Automat Contr 19:716–723. doi:10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, Gee JC (2008) Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med Image Anal 12:26–41. doi:10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004 pmid:17659998
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Bates D, Sarkar D, Bates MD, Matrix L (2007) The lme4 package. R package version 2:74. Available at https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
  4. ↵
    Baum GL, Ciric R, Roalf DR, Betzel RF, Moore TM, Shinohara RT, Kahn AE, Vandekar SN, Rupert PE, Quarmley M, Cook PA, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Gur RE, Gur RC, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD (2017) Modular segregation of structural brain networks supports the development of executive function in youth. Curr Biol 27:1561–1572.e8. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.051 pmid:28552358
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 57:289–300. doi:10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34:537–541. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340409 pmid:8524021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Blakemore S-J, Burnett S, Dahl RE (2010) The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping 31:926–933.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Bramen JE, Hranilovich JA, Dahl RE, Forbes EE, Chen J, Toga AW, Dinov ID, Worthman CM, Sowell ER (2011) Puberty influences medial temporal lobe and cortical gray matter maturation differently in boys than girls matched for sexual maturity. Cereb Cortex 21:636–646. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq137 pmid:20713504
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Caputi L, Pidnebesna A, Hlinka J (2021) Promises and pitfalls of topological data analysis for brain connectivity analysis. Neuroimage 238:118245. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118245 pmid:34111515
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Centeno EGZ, Moreni G, Vriend C, Douw L, Santos FAN (2022) A hands-on tutorial on network and topological neuroscience. Brain Struct Funct 227:741–762. doi:10.1007/s00429-021-02435-0
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    Chai LR, Khambhati AN, Ciric R, Moore TM, Gur RC, Gur RE, Satterthwaite TD, Bassett DS (2017) Evolution of brain network dynamics in neurodevelopment. Netw Neurosci 1:14–30. doi:10.1162/NETN_a_00001 pmid:30793068
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Choi H, Kim YK, Kang H, Lee H, Im H-J, Hwang DW, Kim EE, Chung JK, Lee DS (2014) Abnormal metabolic connectivity in the pilocarpine-induced epilepsy rat model: a multiscale network analysis based on persistent homology. Neuroimage 99:226–236. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.039 pmid:24857713
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Chung MK, Bubenik P, Kim PT (2009) Persistence diagrams of cortical surface data. Inf Process Med Imaging 21:386–397.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    Chung MK, Huang SG, Gritsenko A, Shen L, Lee H (2019) Statistical inference on the number of cycles in brain networks. Proc IEEE Int Symp Biomed Imaging 2019:113–116. doi:10.1109/ISBI.2019.8759222 pmid:31687091
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Ciric R, Wolf DH, Power JD, Roalf DR, Baum GL, Ruparel K, Shinohara RT, Elliott MA, Eickhoff SB, Davatzikos C, Gur RC, Gur RE, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD (2017) Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. Neuroimage 154:174–187. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020 pmid:28302591
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Cohen-Steiner D, Edelsbrunner H, Harer J, Mileyko Y (2010) Lipschitz functions have LP-stable persistence. Found Comput Math 10:127–139. doi:10.1007/s10208-010-9060-6
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  17. ↵
    Edelsbrunner H, Harer J (2010) Computational topology: An introduction. American Mathematical Soc. Available at https://bookstore.ams.org/view?ProductCode=MBK/69.
  18. ↵
    Expert P, Lord LD, Kringelbach ML, Petri G (2019) Editorial: topological neuroscience. Netw Neurosci 3:653–655. doi:10.1162/netn_e_00096
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NUF, Church JA, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2009) Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e1000381. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381 pmid:19412534
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Fasy BT, Kim J, Lecci F, Maria C (2014) Introduction to the R package TDA. arXiv 1411.1830. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1411.1830.
  21. ↵
    Fonov V, Evans AC, Botteron K, Almli CR, McKinstry RC, Collins DL; Brain Development Cooperative Group (2011) Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage 54:313–327. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033 pmid:20656036
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Fülöp T, Desroches M, Cohen AA, Santos FAN, Rodrigues S (2020) Why we should use topological data analysis in ageing: towards defining the “topological shape of ageing.” Mech Ageing Dev 192:111390. doi:10.1016/j.mad.2020.111390 pmid:33127442
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Garrison KA, Scheinost D, Finn ES, Shen X, Constable RT (2015) The (in) stability of functional brain network measures across thresholds. Neuroimage 118:651–661. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.046 pmid:26021218
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Giusti C, Pastalkova E, Curto C, Itskov V (2015) Clique topology reveals intrinsic geometric structure in neural correlations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:13455–13460. doi:10.1073/pnas.1506407112 pmid:26487684
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Gracia-Tabuenca Z, Díaz-Patiño JC, Arelio I, Alcauter S (2020) Topological data analysis reveals robust alterations in the whole-brain and frontal lobe functional connectomes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. eNeuro 7:ENEURO.0543-19.2020. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0543-19.2020
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Gracia-Tabuenca Z, Moreno MB, Barrios FA, Alcauter S (2021) Development of the brain functional connectome follows puberty-dependent nonlinear trajectories. Neuroimage 229:117769. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117769 pmid:33482398
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Graff K, Tansey R, Ip A, Rohr C, Dimond D, Dewey D, Bray S (2022) Benchmarking common preprocessing strategies in early childhood functional connectivity and intersubject correlation fMRI. Dev Cogn Neurosci 54:101087. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101087 pmid:35196611
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Gu S, Satterthwaite TD, Medaglia JD, Yang M, Gur RE, Gur RC, Bassett DS (2015) Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:13681–13686. doi:10.1073/pnas.1502829112 pmid:26483477
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    Herting MM, Maxwell EC, Irvine C, Nagel BJ (2012) The impact of sex, puberty, and hormones on white matter microstructure in adolescents. Cereb Cortex 22:1979–1992. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr246 pmid:22002939
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Hibberd EE, Hackney AC, Lane AR, Myers JB (2015) Assessing biological maturity: chronological age and the pubertal development scale predict free testosterone in adolescent males. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab 28:381–386. doi:10.1515/jpem-2014-0187 pmid:25332291
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Hwang K, Hallquist MN, Luna B (2013) The development of hub architecture in the human functional brain network. Cereb Cortex 23:2380–2393. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs227 pmid:22875861
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17:825–841. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 pmid:12377157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM (2012) FSL. Neuroimage 62:782–790. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015 pmid:21979382
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Laube C, van den Bos W, Fandakova Y (2020) The relationship between pubertal hormones and brain plasticity: implications for cognitive training in adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci 42:100753. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100753 pmid:32072931
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Lee H, Chung MK, Kang H, Kim BN, Lee DS (2011) Discriminative persistent homology of brain networks. 2011 IEEE international symposium on biomedical imaging: from nano to macro, pp 841–844, Chicago, IEEE. doi:10.1109/ISBI.2011.5872535
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  36. ↵
    Lee H, Kang H, Chung MK, Kim BN, Lee DS (2012) Persistent brain network homology from the perspective of dendrogram. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 31:2267–2277. doi:10.1109/TMI.2012.2219590 pmid:23008247
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Lee H, Kang H, Chung MK, Lim S, Kim BN, Lee DS (2017) Integrated multimodal network approach to PET and MRI based on multidimensional persistent homology. Hum Brain Mapp 38:1387–1402. doi:10.1002/hbm.23461 pmid:27859919
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Lee H, Chung MK, Kang H, Choi H, Ha S, Huh Y (2019) Coidentification of group-level hole structures in brain networks via Hodge Laplacian. In: Medical Image Computing and Computer Assisted Intervention – MICCAI 2019 Lecture Notes in Computer Science (Shen D, Liu T, Peters TM, Staib LH, Essert C, and Zhou S, eds). Cham: Springer International Publishing.
  39. ↵
    Lee MH, Kim DY, Lee SH, Kim JU, Chung MK (2015) Comparisons of topological properties in autism for the brain network construction methods. In: Medical imaging 2015: image processing (SPIE), pp 559–564. SPIE Medical Imaging, 2015, Orlando, Florida, United States. SPIE. doi:10.1117/12.2081133
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. ↵
    Li J, Bian C, Luo H, Chen D, Cao L, Liang H (2021) Multi-dimensional persistent feature analysis identifies connectivity patterns of resting-state brain networks in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neural Eng. Advance online publication. Retrieved Feb 16, 2021. https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-2552/abc7ef.doi:10.1088/1741-2552/abc7ef
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  41. ↵
    Liang H, Wang H (2017) Structure-function network mapping and its assessment via persistent homology. PLoS Comput Biol 13:e1005325. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005325 pmid:28046127
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Lin X, Wang N, Welsh AH, Carroll RJ (2004) Equivalent kernels of smoothing splines in nonparametric regression for clustered/longitudinal data. Biometrika 91:177–193. doi:10.1093/biomet/91.1.177
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  43. ↵
    Lord LD, Expert P, Fernandes HM, Petri G, Van Hartevelt TJ, Vaccarino F, Deco G, Turkheimer F, Kringelbach ML (2016) Insights into brain architectures from the homological scaffolds of functional connectivity networks. Front Syst Neurosci 10:85. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2016.00085 pmid:27877115
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Manjón JV, Coupé P, Martí‐Bonmatí L, Collins DL, Robles M (2010) Adaptive non‐local means denoising of MR images with spatially varying noise levels. J Magn Reson Imaging 31:192–203. doi:10.1002/jmri.22003 pmid:20027588
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Marek S, Hwang K, Foran W, Hallquist MN, Luna B (2015) The contribution of network organization and integration to the development of cognitive control. PLoS Biol 13:e1002328. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002328 pmid:26713863
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Parkes L, Fulcher B, Yücel M, Fornito A (2018) An evaluation of the efficacy, reliability, and sensitivity of motion correction strategies for resting-state functional MRI. Neuroimage 171:415–436. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.073 pmid:29278773
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Peper JS, Schnack HG, Brouwer RM, Van Baal GCM, Pjetri E, Székely E, van Leeuwen M, van den Berg SM, Collins DL, Evans AC, Boomsma DI, Kahn RS, Hulshoff Pol HE (2009) Heritability of regional and global brain structure at the onset of puberty: a magnetic resonance imaging study in 9-year-old twin pairs. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2184–2196. doi:10.1002/hbm.20660 pmid:19294640
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Perrin JS, Leonard G, Perron M, Pike GB, Pitiot A, Richer L, Veillette S, Pausova Z, Paus T (2009) Sex differences in the growth of white matter during adolescence. Neuroimage 45:1055–1066. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.023 pmid:19349224
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, Boxer A (1988) A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. J Youth Adolesc 17:117–133. doi:10.1007/BF01537962 pmid:24277579
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Vogel AC, Laumann TO, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2011) Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72:665–678. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006 pmid:22099467
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Rubinov M, Sporns O (2010) Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52:1059–1069. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 pmid:19819337
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Rieck B, Sadlo F, Leitte H (2020) Topological machine learning with persistence indicator functions. In: Topological methods in data analysis and visualization V. Mathematics and visualization (Carr H, Fujishiro I, Sadlo F, and Takahashi S, eds), pp 87–101. Cham: Springer International Publishing.
  53. ↵
    Santos FA, Raposo EP, Coutinho-Filho MD, Copelli M, Stam CJ, Douw L (2019) Topological phase transitions in functional brain networks. Phys Rev E 100:e032414. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.100.032414 pmid:31640025
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Loughead J, Ruparel K, Elliott MA, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE (2012) Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. Neuroimage 60:623–632. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063 pmid:22233733
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Ruparel K, Erus G, Elliott MA, Eickhoff SB, Gennatas ED, Jackson C, Prabhakaran K, Smith A, Hakonarson H, Verma R, Davatzikos C, Gur RE, Gur RC (2013a) Heterogeneous impact of motion on fundamental patterns of developmental changes in functional connectivity during youth. Neuroimage 83:45–57. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.045 pmid:23792981
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Gerraty RT, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Calkins ME, Eickhoff SB, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Wolf DH (2013b) An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. Neuroimage 64:240–256. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052 pmid:22926292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Sawyer SM, Azzopardi PS, Wickremarathne D, Patton GC (2018) The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc Health 2:223–228. doi:10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  58. ↵
    Sisk CL, Foster DL (2004) The neural basis of puberty and adolescence. Nat Neurosci 7:1040–1047. doi:10.1038/nn1326
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Sizemore AE, Giusti C, Kahn A, Vettel JM, Betzel RF, Bassett DS (2018) Cliques and cavities in the human connectome. J Comput Neurosci 44:115–145. doi:10.1007/s10827-017-0672-6 pmid:29143250
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    Sizemore AE, Phillips-Cremins JE, Ghrist R, Bassett DS (2019) The importance of the whole: topological data analysis for the network neuroscientist. Netw Neurosci 3:656–673. doi:10.1162/netn_a_00073
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR, Beckmann CF (2009) Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:13040–13045. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905267106 pmid:19620724
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    Taymourtash A, Schwartz E, Nenning KH, Sobotka D, Diogo M, Kasprian G, Prayer D, Langs G (2019) Quantifying residual motion artifacts in fetal fMRI data. In: Smart ultrasound imaging and perinatal, preterm and paediatric image analysis, pp 171–180. Cham: Springer.
  63. ↵
    Tustison NJ, Avants BB, Cook PA, Zheng Y, Egan A, Yushkevich PA, Gee JC (2010) N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 29:1310–1320. pmid:20378467
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Van Dijk KR, Hedden T, Venkataraman A, Evans KC, Lazar SW, Buckner RL (2010) Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol 103:297–321. pmid:19889849
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    van Duijvenvoorde ACK, Westhoff B, de Vos F, Wierenga LM, Crone EA (2019) A three-wave longitudinal study of subcortical–cortical resting-state connectivity in adolescence: testing age-and puberty-related changes. Hum Brain Mapp 40:3769–3783. pmid:31099959
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    Verbeke G, Lesaffre E (1996) A linear mixed-effects model with heterogeneity in the random-effects population. J Am Stat Assoc 91:217–221.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  67. ↵
    Vijayakumar N, de Macks ZO, Shirtcliff EA, Pfeifer JH (2018) Puberty and the human brain: insights into adolescent development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 92:417–436. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.004 pmid:29972766
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    Wood SN (2013) On p-values for smooth components of an extended generalized additive model. Biometrika 100:221–228.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  69. ↵
    Wood S, Scheipl F, Wood MS (2017) Package ‘gamm4.’ Am Stat 45:339. Available at https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gamm4.
    OpenUrl
  70. ↵
    Xia M, Wang J, He Y (2013) BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One 8:e68910. pmid:23861951
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    Zanto TP, Gazzaley A (2013) Fronto-parietal network: flexible hub of cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci 17:602–603. pmid:24129332
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Christie Fowler, University of California, Irvine

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: NONE.

The study investigates changes in the brain functional connectome that occur during adolescence using algebraic topology, specifically connectivity of simplicial complexes (Betti numbers b0 & b1). The methodological framework is original and may be helpful to derive information about brain networks segregation and integration. Some other aspects are commendable such as the longitudinal sample and the quite high sample size. The conclusions stipulate that as adolescence goes the brain becomes more segregated at the global level and more densely connected at the local level, and that the trends are nonlinear.

The study may make an interesting contribution to the field of human brain mapping, although these finding are not particularly new. However, the conclusions may not be sufficiently supported by the results. Moreover, the study is not strongly motivated (no question, no hypothesis

Specific comments:

1. The article suffers from a lack of insight on what TDA reveals about the longitudinal growth patterns that other tools don’t reveal.

2. Parametric statistical approaches like LME have stringent model assumptions. No model checking details are included.

3. I don’t feel confident that the topological metrics are really about networks segregation and integration, i.e. independent processing in specialized subsystems and global cooperation between different subsystems, respectively. As far as I understand them they likely relate to these definitions, but not sure to which extent. My concern applies particularly to integration, which cannot be considered by applying TDA per network (and accordingly ignoring interaction between networks). This naturally leads to questionable interpretations.

4. The absence of any research question and hypothesis (as noted above) makes the relevance and impact of the study outcomes difficult to evaluate. In particular, it’s hard to judge whether the statistical framework is the most appropriate. The authors should expose clearly the rational behind model selection (especially why nonlinear trajectories have been examined using GAMM models and not more specific model-driven approaches).

5. Several points about LME/GAMM modelling need to be addressed:

(i) the authors discuss the goodness of fit of the different classes of models but terms of most of them are not significant. This approach seems questionable to me.

(ii) a conclusion is that nonlinear models surpass linear ones, and that development effects show nonlinear developmental trajectories. However, results did not demonstrate it for age and age.sex, and only nonlinear models have been implemented for PDS and PDS.sex. I have difficulty to understand from where this conclusion come from.

(iii) the respective impact of age and puberty on network topological characteristics is difficult to established since both variables largely covary (I guess) and have not been tested jointly in the models (e.g. controlling for one while testing the other). Statements as “the connectome topology better adjusts the pubertal status than chronological age” appears overstated to me for now.

6. Overall, data are not described with enough details. The authors should better describe the longitudinal sample, the way the longitudinal dimension of the sample have been modelled (how repeated measurements have been taken into accounts), the relationship between PDS and age, the extent of censored brain data, the output of all models (LRT, intercept, p-values, etc).

7. Outputs from the models are represented as standardized residuals so that it’s tricky to get an idea of the real relationships between AUC-B0/B1 and PBS or age (beyond saying that the trend is nonlinear). It would be important to provide further details on the exact nature of the relationships between AUC-B0/B1 and PBS or age.

8. It would be important to improve Figure 1 and text related to it to make easier the intuition about B0 and B1 in terms of brain functioning.

9. The result section is very short and needs to be revised to present outcomes in a more exhaustive and comprehensive way.

10. The author should discuss in more details what is original in their findings with respect to existing literature. As the discussion stands currently, it seems that their study only confirms to some extent already familiar results.

11. Technical details related to TDA need to be improved, e.g.

- Line 148: “which nodes [of F]” should be “which pairs of nodes”;

- Line 152: “number of components” should be “number of connected components”;

- Line 165: “Blue triangles are not holes as they are surfaces between triads of nodes” is inaccurate in the context of Figure 1 as the two-dimensional complex doesn’t contain tetrahedrons;

- Line 169: “in favor of the connected nodes” should be “as the number of connected components, B0, increases”.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 10 (2)
eNeuro
Vol. 10, Issue 2
February 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Development of the Functional Connectome Topology in Adolescence: Evidence from Topological Data Analysis
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Development of the Functional Connectome Topology in Adolescence: Evidence from Topological Data Analysis
Zeus Gracia-Tabuenca, Juan Carlos Díaz-Patiño, Isaac Arelio-Ríos, Martha Beatriz Moreno-García, Fernando A. Barrios, Sarael Alcauter
eNeuro 30 January 2023, 10 (2) ENEURO.0296-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0296-21.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Development of the Functional Connectome Topology in Adolescence: Evidence from Topological Data Analysis
Zeus Gracia-Tabuenca, Juan Carlos Díaz-Patiño, Isaac Arelio-Ríos, Martha Beatriz Moreno-García, Fernando A. Barrios, Sarael Alcauter
eNeuro 30 January 2023, 10 (2) ENEURO.0296-21.2022; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0296-21.2022
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • adolescence
  • functional connectivity
  • persistence homology
  • puberty
  • resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging
  • topological data analysis

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: New Research

  • Characterization of the Tau Interactome in Human Brain Reveals Isoform-Dependent Interaction with 14-3-3 Family Proteins
  • The Mobility of Neurofilaments in Mature Myelinated Axons of Adult Mice
  • Capacity Limits Lead to Information Bottlenecks in Ongoing Rapid Motor Behaviors
Show more Research Article: New Research

Development

  • Heterozygous Dab1 null mutation disrupts neocortical and hippocampal development
  • The Mobility of Neurofilaments in Mature Myelinated Axons of Adult Mice
Show more Development

Subjects

  • Development

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.