Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Research ArticleResearch Article: Confirmation, Cognition and Behavior

Hippocampal Neuronal Activity Preceding Stimulus Predicts Later Memory Success

Soyeon Jun, June Sic Kim and Chun Kee Chung
eNeuro 31 January 2023, 10 (2) ENEURO.0252-22.2023; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0252-22.2023
Soyeon Jun
1Neuroscience Research Institute, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea, 03080
4Department of Neurosurgery, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, South Korea, 03080
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
June Sic Kim
3Research Institute of Basic Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea, 03080
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Chun Kee Chung
2Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea, 08826
4Department of Neurosurgery, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, South Korea, 03080
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Chun Kee Chung
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Hippocampal neuronal activity at a time preceding stimulus onset affects episodic memory performance. We hypothesized that neuronal activity preceding an event supports successful memory formation; therefore, we explored whether a characterized encoding-associated brain activity, viz. the neuronal activity preceding a stimulus, predicts subsequent memory formation. To address this issue, we assessed the activity of single neurons recorded from the hippocampus in humans, while participants performed word memory tasks. Human hippocampal single-unit activity elicited by a fixation cue preceding words increased the firing rates (FRs) and predicted whether the words are recalled in a subsequent memory test; this indicated that successful memory formation in humans can be predicted by a preceding stimulus activity during encoding. However, the predictive effect of preceding stimulus activity did not occur during retrieval. These findings suggest that the preparative arrangement of brain activity before stimulus encoding improves subsequent memory performance.

  • hippocampus
  • memory
  • neuronal activity
  • prediction
  • single-unit activity

Significance Statement

Hippocampal neuronal activity at a time preceding stimulus onset affects episodic memory performance. This predictive effect may reflect encoding attentional selection, providing an excitatory advantage to the hippocampal cells involved in coding the cue.

Introduction

Human brain activity elicited by initially faced stimulus events is a critical determinant of whether an item will be successfully memorized later (Sanquist et al., 1980; Rugg and Coles, 1995; Paller and Wagner, 2002). In previous studies, researchers have focused on memory-predictive brain activation during stimulus presentation using the “subsequent memory effect” (SME) approach. It is classically known as the neural activity difference between items subsequently remembered and items are forgotten while encoding during stimulus presentation (Paller et al., 1987; Paller and Wagner, 2002). In this approach, the recorded neural activity during the encoding period is classified based on whether these items are remembered or forgotten in the subsequent memory test. Brain activities differentiating between subsequently remembered and forgotten items encoding-related activity and fleeting neural activity induced by individual items are used as a neural marker in successful memory formation. Although findings from this approach have yielded important insights into the neural mechanisms underlying successful memory encoding (Jun et al., 2019, 2020, 2021), studies are limited concerning the exploration of encoding-correlated brain activity lined up by stimulus events.

Learning from novel events is dependent on various brain processes and mechanisms. Thus, in reality, experiences and memorandum do not exist separately; rather, they proceed on a sequential continuum. Specifically, present events are, in some measure, a consequence of past events and could provide cues regarding future events. Thus, at any given moment in time, the neural activity resulting from experiences will influence the memory of the events faced. The use of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) suggests that brain activity occurring immediately before encoding can predict subsequent memory performance in semantic preparatory task sets, improving the encoding of words processed deeply (Otten et al., 2006; Guderian et al., 2009).

Additionally, neural activity is tonically maintained across a succession of stimulus events involved in successful episodic memory encoding (Otten et al., 2002). In addition, Park and Rugg (2010) investigated human neuronal activity in the hippocampus and found that attention to encoding predicts subsequent memory in the medial temporal lobe in the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. These results show that the hippocampal predictive preonset spiking effect is much more robust (Urgolites et al., 2020). Further investigation is needed to create additional knowledge concerning the diverse neural mechanisms supporting hippocampus-dependent memory encoding. However, only a few studies have investigated the effect of predictive neuronal activity’s effect on subsequent memory performance and successful memory formation, specifically in hippocampal-dependent activity.

The present study explored whether a characterized encoding-associated brain activity predicts subsequent memory formation. We used the “preceding stimulus subsequent memory effect” (pre-SME) procedure in human hippocampal single neuronal activity during encoding and retrieval stimulus interval. Additionally, we investigated whether there is a characterization of hippocampus-dependent task effects in word item memory or word associative memory (Staresina et al., 2019; Jun et al., 2020) on hippocampal neuronal activities preceding a stimulus.

Materials and Methods

Subjects and microelectrode recording

Four male patients with epilepsy aged 25–64 year (M = 39.7, SE = 17.3) were implanted with hybrid depth electrodes for seizure monitoring. We assessed all individuals using a typical neuropsychological test (Table 1). Nine microwires (eight high-impedance recording electrodes and one low-impedance reference; Ad-Tech) projecting from the shaft of the depth electrodes were used to record signals from medial temporal lobe neurons. Electrode locations (Fig. 1) were selected following the clinical criteria. Target electrode locations were confirmed using a human hippocampal atlas created using a postoperative computed tomography (CT) image that was reconstructed from individual preoperative magnetic resonance (MR) images using CURRY 8.0 (Compumedics Neuroscan). MR images were collected using a Sigma 1.5-tesla scanner (GE). A neuroradiologist identified each electrode contact using a thin postimplant CT scan section. The brain model and implanted electrodes were reconstructed from individual preoperative MR and postoperative CT images using CURRY 7.0 (Compumedics Neuroscan). A neuroradiologist and neurosurgeon then confirmed the contact of the hippocampal electrodes. We further investigated an automatic segmentation for the hippocampal subfield using an open-source software package (Fischl, 2012) with T1-weighted images (Iglesias et al., 2015) and annotations using Freeview. Electrodes that could be contained in the hippocampus or amygdala were included in this experiment. All individuals had adequate recognition and distinguishable neuronal spiking activities.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

List of patient demographics, pathology, and neuropsychological evaluation

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Electrode localization with structural magnetic resonance images. Examples of hippocampal microelectrodes from two patients are shown (see Table 1 for demographic information). The patient’s left brain is shown on the left side in the axis plane. All images are T1 images recorded using Sigma 3-tesla scanner (GE). Green arrows indicate electrodes inserted in the hippocampal subfield of Cornu Ammonis (CA3 (above) and CA1 (bottom)). a, Sagittal image of the left hippocampal CA3. b, Coronal hippocampal CA3. c, Axial hippocampal CA3. d, Sagittal image of the left hippocampal CA1. e, Coronal CA1 image. f, Axial CA1 image.

Experimental design and single-unit identification

We recorded single neuronal activities in four individuals and had individuals encode visually presented word memory tasks using STIM2 software (Compumedics Neuroscan). The patients had the same memory task (Fig. 2) as our previous study using the word memory tasks (Jun et al., 2019, 2020, 2021). Behavioral data were analyzed with SPSS 23 (IBM). Extracellular neural activity was recorded using 40-μm micro-wires implanted in a depth electrode and inserted in the hippocampus and amygdala. Spiking and intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG) field potentials were recorded using a 32-channel Neuralynx ATLAS system. Spike data from the same electrode recording with macroelectrode were acquired by bandpass filtering the raw signal from 600 to 9000 Hz. For the spike, the sampling rate was 32 kHz; the signals were referenced against one low-impedance reference electrode, and spikes were detected from the raw trace by applying a 30-μV threshold. Spike sorting was performed using wave clus (Quiroga et al., 2004). Once the sorting was completed, all clusters were categorized according to noise, multi-unit, or single-unit activity based on the waveform size, the shape of waveform compared with noise, the absence of power line interference, and an indication of a refractory interval through the procedures used in a previous study (Valdez et al., 2013). To visualize the spikes aligned with the memory task, a raster plot was created by aligning spike timestamps regarding to the timestamp for the visual stimuli presentation (bin size = 200 ms, time window = 1 s before and 3 s after stimuli). We then subdivided the entire prestimulus and during-stimulus periods into time-resolved windows using 200-ms sliding windows with 150-ms overlap (Urgolites et al., 2020).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Word memory tasks. a, Example of the timeline of the visual item memory task and (b) associative memory task. Memory tasks comprised three successive stages encoding, distractor, and retrieval. Each patient sequentially completed words items during encoding, which was followed by a white fixation cue with a black screen for 1 s. The green dashed line box indicates the preceding stimulus interval.

Unit identification by the prestimulus subsequent memory effect

We categorized pre-SME neurons that were statistically defined by comparing the distribution of each neuron’s firing rate (FR) similar to previously applied methods (Yoo et al., 2021). Neurons were classified as (1) “pre-SME” cells if the FR was higher in subsequently remembered words compared with subsequently forgotten words, (2) “anti-pre-SME” if the FR was higher to subsequently forgotten words relative to subsequently remembered words, or (3) “non-pre-SME,” if FR was not a significant response to recognition success. For the individual statistic test, we generated a p-value by identifying the position of the true rank sum statistic from the test applied to real data with 1000 rank sum values. Thus, permutation-based p-values were calculated to assess the significant difference between the conditions (i.e., remembered vs forgotten trials) in the prestimulus period by a null distribution of data samples. First, we collected the trials of the two conditions in a single set, then randomly partitioned the trials into two subsets. Next, we calculated the test statistics on this random partition and repeated steps (i.e., 1000 iterations) second and third analysis a large number of times and constructed a histogram of the test statistics (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). Finally, we used the Wilcoxon signed-rank test for the group-level significant test and assessed the statistical significance of changes. All spike data analysis was conducted using the Statistics Toolbox in MATLAB software (version 2018b, MathWorks).

Human participants

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul National University Hospital (H-1407-115-596). All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the study.

Results

Memory and reaction time: results showed that patients remembered well as the mean frequency of correct responses across all trials was 80.0 ± 3.95%, mean ± SEM) and the average sensitivity, d, was 1.7 ± 0.27 (p < 0.05), which indicated that they had adequate memory. The overall reaction time for recognition was 1220.23 ± 239.73 ms for correctly remembered words and 1717.18 ± 885.66 ms for forgotten words (mean ± SEM).

Firing rate of hippocampal and amygdala neurons

We recorded spiking activities of single units in the hippocampus and amygdala as individuals performed word memory tasks. Thirty-two single neurons were isolated from the hippocampus and amygdala (30 from the hippocampus and two from the amygdala: 29 single units and three multiunits). Only single units that met the isolation criteria, including the interspike intervals (ISIs) ≤ 3 ms and the cluster number of neurons ≤0.5% (n = 29), were used in the final analyses. Ninety-three percent of most units (n = 27 out of 29) were regular-spiking neurons, and others were unclassified neurons (2/29) based on autocorrelograms (Barthó et al., 2004). Unit recordings were made from the hippocampal CA3 for patients 1 and 3. On average, the isolated single neurons fired 1.63 spikes per second. The mean firing rate of the hippocampal neurons was 1.71, which was higher than that of 1.47 in the amygdala (Fig. 3).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Numbers of neurons and mean firing rates from different brain areas. a, Units’ numbers from each brain area were as follows, from left to right: 20, 10, and 2. b, The mean firing rates of units, respectively. All units are included, regardless of firing rates. The mean firing rates of the hippocampal and amygdala are 1.71 and 1.47, respectively. Error bars are ±SEM. RH, right hippocampus; LH, left hippocampus; RA, the right amygdala.

Firing rate of encoding and activity

We examined how the encoding activity during the presentation of words (i.e., task period) differed from the resting states. We analyzed the mean firing rates in the hippocampus during resting and memory encoding states for a given session. Results showed that the spiking activity for memory encoding was significantly higher than that for resting states. The mean firing rates across neurons during resting states versus memory encoding showed a significant increase in firing rates for memory encoding, which indicates that neurons exhibited task specificity (Fig. 4A, group-level analysis, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p < 0.001).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Group-level comparison of spiking activity in the hippocampus during an item memory task. a, The mean firing rates across neurons (n = 11) during resting states versus memory encoding showed a significant increase in firing rates for memory encoding compared with resting states (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, ***p < 0.001). b, Group-level comparison of pre-SME and during-SME hippocampal mean firing rates. During the preceding stimulus, hippocampal firing rates were higher for subsequent remembered than forgotten words (left, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, **p < 0.01). However, the trend during-SME hippocampal activity was not significant (right, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p = 0.058). c, Pre-SME and during-SME mean firing rates for an operation of words subsequently remembered (left) versus forgotten (right). The difference between pre-SME and during-SME was not significant in both remembered and forgotten words (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p = 0.0518, p = 0.082, respectively).

Firing rate of pre-SME predicts encoding success

To quantify the pre-SME, the mean firing rates were measured across the 1-s interval of the fixation cue period between remembered and forgotten trials. As we functionally defined proportions of each type of neuron, we then considered neurons as preceding stimulus activity if the firing rate responded to either (1) a fixation cross cue during the prestimulus interval and showed, or (2) a consistent elevated pattern of firing in all trials of that fixation cross. Behavioral classification of neurons indicated that 11 neurons (40%, 10 in the hippocampus, and one in the amygdala) showed a significant preceding stimulus activity and consistently increased firing rate in all trials in at least a 200-ms segment. These neurons exhibited significantly elevated FR during pre-encoding of successfully remembered compared with unsuccessfully remembered items, with consistently increased firing rate. The firing rates substantially differed before word stimulus onset corresponding to subsequent memory performance (pre-SME, p < 0.01, permutation-based p-value, shuffling 1000 times across units; Fig. 5, raster plot). Therefore, mean firing rates of neurons elicited by fixation cues preceding encoding words showed significantly increased patterns with the firing rates of subsequently remembered words compared with that of forgotten words. Three neurons showed a significantly decreased FR that predicted encoding success (anti-pre-SME). The rest 13 neurons did not show significant FR changes related to encoding success (non-pre-SME).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Hippocampal neuronal responses to subsequently remembered and forgotten word items. Shown are representative examples of hippocampal neurons that significantly increase firing rates across trials. Preceding stimulus and during-stimulus raster plot of firing rates during encoding sessions. The red lines mark stimulus onset at 0; the offset is at 3 s (x-axis). Raster rows represent single trials, and each dot represents an action potential. Next, the preceding stimulus and during-stimulus raster plot of firing rates for the remembered and forgotten conditions during encoding (**p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, permutation-based p-value).

We compared the SME procedure between preceding stimulus and during-stimulus intervals. We found the hippocampal activity for subsequently remembered words was relatively larger than that for subsequently forgotten words before word stimuli (Fig. 4B, left, group-level analysis, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, df = 10, p < 0.01). Although the group-level analysis was not significant, hippocampal activity for subsequently remembered words was relatively larger than that for subsequently forgotten words after word stimuli (Fig. 4B, right, group-level analysis, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, df = 10, p = 0.058, see also Fig. 6 for a representative electrode site). These comparisons showed higher hippocampal neuronal activity for subsequently remembered versus forgotten words for both preceding and during-stimulus presentation. However, the spiking data showed no difference between the preceding stimulus and during-stimulus spiking activity in subsequently remembered and forgotten words (Fig. 4C, group-level analysis, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p = 0.518, see also Fig. 6 for a representative electrode site). The difference was not significant, as the hippocampal spiking activity indicated merely a trend of higher during-stimulus spiking levels for the subsequently remembered words versus that of subsequently forgotten words. These results suggest that the during-stimulus activity was a continuation of the preceding stimulus activity, indicating that the during-stimulus neuronal activity was a prolongation of the preceding stimulus activity.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Hippocampal neuronal responses to subsequently remembered and forgotten word items. a, The mean firing rates between resting states and encoding. Mean firing rates for the encoding duration are significantly higher than the resting state. b, The hippocampal preceding stimulus neuronal activity (pre-SME) is significantly higher in subsequently remembered items compared with subsequently forgotten items (***p < 0.001, *p < 0.05, permutation-based p-value). This neuron was selective to only preceding stimulus with similar increases in activity in the during-stimulus condition. c, The hippocampal spiking activity indicated the difference between preceding stimulus and during-stimulus activity was negligible in both remembered and forgotten conditions (n.s., p = 0.0524, respectively, permutation-based p-value). n.s. indicates not significant.

First presentation effect of unit activity

To further examine how the preceding stimulus activity of the first presentation of word stimulus compares with the performance at the other presentation, we compared the mean spike activity from the first preceding stimulus activity with the mean spike activity from the other stimulus preceding activity. We showed that the first stimulus and other stimulus’s hippocampal preceding activity were higher for subsequently remembered but not for forgotten words (permutation-based rank sum test, p < 0.01), yielding hippocampal spiking activity before the presentation of predicted to-be-learned words.

Spiking activity of task dependency and retrieval

We also analyzed the group-level comparison of the associative memory task by an illustration in the item memory task, to confirm whether spiking activity would differ between the two different tasks (Jun et al., 2020). Results showed that the hippocampal activity in associative memory task also exhibited higher values for remembered words and lower in forgotten words either preceding stimulus or during-stimulus periods (Fig. 7, group-level analysis, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p < 0.001 and p < 0.01, respectively). It indicates that the hippocampal preceding stimulus effect was exhibited in both conditions. To further study the relation between activity around the encoding and retrieval, we examined whether activity during the retrieval predicts memory performance on that stimulus. Results show that in retrieval, although during-stimulus hippocampal activity predicted whether memory performance would be correct (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p < 0.001), the predictive effect was not observed in the preceding stimulus (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p = 0.147).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Group-level comparison of spiking activity in the hippocampus during an associative memory task. a, Presubsequent memory effect (pre-SME) and (b) during-SME mean firing rates as an operation of words subsequently remembered versus forgotten words.

Discussion

In this study, we evaluated whether spiking activity preceding and during stimulus predicted subsequent successful memory formation in word memory tasks in patients with epilepsy. The hippocampal neurons, presented by a fixation cue preceding a word, increased the firing rates based on whether the word would be subsequently remembered. The during-stimulus activity also showed the trend of predicting subsequent successful memory formation. However, the firing rates were merely a prolongation of preceding stimulus firing rates in the item memory task. However, during retrieval, this predictive effect was not exhibited. These findings suggest that the hippocampal preceding stimulus activity during encoding may modulate successful memory formation and predict later memory success.

The brain activity that generates complex patterns even in the absence of sensory involvement or task is referred to as “prestimulus,” as opposed to activity following sensory input (Iemi et al., 2019). Studies exhibiting prestimulus activity have primarily investigated event-related potentials (ERPs), showing successful memory encoding (i.e., pre-SME). For example, neural activity in the prestimulus onset interval varied with items that were subsequently recalled or subsequently forgotten. It supports the idea that the preceding stimulus neural activity is sensitive to episodic memory performance (Klimesch et al., 1999; Fell et al., 2011; Gruber et al., 2013; Merkow et al., 2014; Salari and Rose, 2016). Although our data showed a similar hippocampal preceding stimulus effect in both word memory tasks, prior fMRI studies showed that pre-SMEs have been affected by task features, for instance, stimulus modality (Wagner et al., 1998a; Golby et al., 2001; Otten et al., 2006; Park and Rugg, 2010). Accordingly, it is considered to reflect, to some extent, the preparative arrangement of material-specific or task-specific and domain-general processes that influence memory performance.

What might be a rationale for modulating preceding stimulus hippocampal activity? One possibility is that neural activity reflects an attentional or preparative status in which the preceding stimulus cue recruits in anticipation of the upcoming stimulus. For example, the experimental task required participants to switch unpredictably from set up for a coming visual stimulus. In this case, an arrangement is needed to switch from the attentional concentration of the task fixation cue to the forthcoming stimulus. Therefore, hippocampal activity preceding stimulus was enhanced on those stimuli where processes arranged for the presentation of the fixation cue and resulted in a reasonably optimal preparative status. A possible neurochemical mechanism underlying these attentional shifts is the activation of the scheme by modulating the “reward value” of the preceding stimulus cue (Adcock et al., 2006), which described that the hippocampal pre-SMEs function together being activated in the ventral tegmental area, the origin of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system. Pre-SME in this system incites or elicits cues indicating the novelty of upcoming to-be-studied items (Wittmann et al., 2007). In this sense, hippocampal neurons may reflect the fixation cue operated by triggering the mesolimbic dopaminergic system, which may contribute to manipulating preceding stimulus hippocampal activity in conditions of novel study items.

Since the preceding stimulus and during-stimulus activity in encoding may be related, how would preceding stimulus activity regulate encoding eligibility? As depicted in Figure 4C, the raw spiking data revealed that during-stimulus firing, there is an extension of the preceding stimulus firing rates for each remembered and forgotten condition. In an earlier study, cumulative excitability in a single unit follows those units being partially influenced to correspond to a novel memory (Cai et al., 2016). More excitable units primarily activate with the upcoming items, and synaptic variations in those neurons create the memory trace (Rogerson et al., 2014). The neuronal firing of specific neurons in the hippocampal CA1 and other hippocampal subfields is critical in building a behavioral response that actuates “engram” neurons labeled when the cue is presented. This effect conjures up our experiences, and memories may be conveyed by simple ensemble activity without sequential structure (Dutta et al., 2020). Overall, the present study suggests that the more excitable neurons were already working at the time the item was presented and created well-built memory traces (Han et al., 2007).

Our findings reveal a cellular process in the human hippocampus representing preparatory activity and identify candidate neurons that may support the attentional shift for memory performance. Moreover, our results advance previous findings that the chance of an item being successfully encoded is related either to the pattern of neuronal activity stimulated by the item itself or to the neuronal activity that instantly heads (or that neuronal activity that immediately precedes) the item. Therefore, when the brain, specifically the hippocampal neurons, is gearing up to encode into episodic memory and hippocampal-mediated episodic memory, success may be augmented by high levels of preceding stimulus neuronal response.

The data indicated that words subsequently remembered induced more positive-going event-related potential (ERP) than words subsequently forgotten (Otten et al., 2006). It may reflect encoding attentional selection, providing an excitatory advantage to the hippocampal cells involved in coding the cue. Concerning the alternations in timing and expression of pre-SMEs, it is apparent that neuronal or oscillatory activity from iEEG field potentials is more sensitive than that from noninvasive measures for detecting pre-SMEs which may be missed with ERPs or BOLD signals. However, pre-SMEs using direct invasive electrophysiological studies need to be better understood. Our data provide neuronal evidence of pre-SMEs with a markedly higher temporal resolution, essential for noninvasive studies examining memory. These findings are applicable for the interpretation of studies employing indirect brain investigation [i.e., fMRI and electroencephalography (EEG)] of successful memory formation (Wagner et al., 1998b; Paller and Wagner, 2002; Otten et al., 2006) and an vital precursor to better understanding human memory. The present experiment investigated a small number of participants; hence, we cannot claim with any certainty that the statistical power was sufficient. Despite this limitation, we observed a consistent preparatory neuronal effect on each of the 11 neurons. In future research, we aim to recruit more extensive samples of patients and perform in-depth analyses considering variable hippocampal-subfield characteristics.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements: We thank Robert T. Knight for constructive discussions, Florian Mormman for helpful comments and Seokyun Ryun for data collection.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • This research was supported by Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by Ministry of Education Grants NRF-2021M3E5D2A01019538, NRF-2021R1A4A2001803, and NRF-2022R1I1A1A01073860.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Adcock RA, Thangavel A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Knutson B, Gabrieli JD (2006) Reward-motivated learning: mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron 50:507–517. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.036 pmid:16675403
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Barthó P, Hirase H, Monconduit L, Zugaro M, Harris KD, Buzsáki G (2004) Characterization of neocortical principal cells and interneurons by network interactions and extracellular features. J Neurophysiol 92:600–608. doi:10.1152/jn.01170.2003 pmid:15056678
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Cai DJ, et al. (2016) A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature 534:115–118. pmid:27251287
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Dutta S, Gao S, Chu JP, Kemere C (2020) Tracing a path for memory in the hippocampus. Neuron 107:199–201. pmid:32702341
    OpenUrlPubMed
  5. ↵
    Fell J, Ludowig E, Staresina BP, Wagner T, Kranz T, Elger CE, Axmacher N (2011) Medial temporal theta/alpha power enhancement precedes successful memory encoding: evidence based on intracranial EEG. J Neurosci 31:5392–5397. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3668-10.2011 pmid:21471374
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Fischl B (2012) FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62:774–781. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021 pmid:22248573
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Golby Aj, Poldrack RA, Brewer JB, Spencer D, Desmond JE, Aron AP, Gabrieli JD (2001) Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain 124:1841–1854. doi:10.1093/brain/124.9.1841 pmid:11522586
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Gruber MJ, Watrous AJ, Ekstrom AD, Ranganath C, Otten LJ (2013) Expected reward modulates encoding-related theta activity before an event. Neuroimage 64:68–74. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.064 pmid:22917987
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Guderian S, Schott BH, Richardson-Klavehn A, Düzel E (2009) Medial temporal theta state before an event predicts episodic encoding success in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:5365–5370. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900289106 pmid:19289818
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Han JH, Kushner SA, Yiu AP, Cole CJ, Matynia A, Brown RA, Neve RL, Guzowski JF, Silva AJ, Josselyn SA (2007) Neuronal competition and selection during memory formation. Science 316:457–460. doi:10.1126/science.1139438 pmid:17446403
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Iemi L, Busch NA, Laudini A, Haegens S, Samaha J, Villringer A, Nikulin VV (2019) Multiple mechanisms link prestimulus neural oscillations to sensory responses. Elife 8:e43620. doi:10.7554/eLife.43620
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Iglesias JE, Augustinack JC, Nguyen K, Player CM, Player A, Wright M, Roy N, Frosch MP, Mckee AC, Wald LL, Fischl B, Van Leemput K; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2015) A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. Neuroimage 115:117–137. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.042 pmid:25936807
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Jun S, Lee SA, Kim JS, Jeong W, Chung CK (2020) Task-dependent effects of intracranial hippocampal stimulation on human memory and hippocampal theta power. Brain Stimul 13:603–613. pmid:32289685
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    Jun S, Kim JS, Chung CK (2019) Direct stimulation of human hippocampus during verbal associative encoding enhances subsequent memory recollection. Front Hum Neurosci 13:23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Jun S, Kim JS, Chung CK (2021) Prediction of successful memory encoding based on lateral temporal cortical gamma power. Front Neurosci 15:517316. pmid:34113226
    OpenUrlPubMed
  16. ↵
    Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Schwaiger J, Auinger P, Winkler T (1999) ‘Paradoxical’ alpha synchronization in a memory task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 7:493–501. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00056-1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Maris E, Oostenveld R (2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods 164:177–190. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 pmid:17517438
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Merkow MB, Burke JF, Stein JM, Kahana MJ (2014) Prestimulus theta in the human hippocampus predicts subsequent recognition but not recall. Hippocampus 24:1562–1569. doi:10.1002/hipo.22335
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Otten LJ, Henson RN, Rugg MD (2002) State-related and item-related neural correlates of successful memory encoding. Nat Neurosci 5:1339–1344. doi:10.1038/nn967 pmid:12402040
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Otten LJ, Quayle AH, Akram S, Ditewig TA, Rugg MD (2006) Brain activity before an event predicts later recollection. Nat Neurosci 9:489–491. doi:10.1038/nn1663 pmid:16501566
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Paller KA, Wagner AD (2002) Observing the transformation of experience into memory. Trends Cogn Sci 6:93–102. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01845-3 pmid:15866193
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Paller KA, Kutas M, Mayes AR (1987) Neural correlates of encoding in an incidental learning paradigm. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 67:360–371. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(87)90124-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Park H, Rugg MD (2010) Prestimulus hippocampal activity predicts later recollection. Hippocampus 20:24–28. doi:10.1002/hipo.20663 pmid:19499585
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Quiroga RQ, Nadasdy Z, Ben-Shaul Y (2004) Unsupervised spike detection and sorting with wavelets and superparamagnetic clustering. Neural Comput 16:1661–1687. doi:10.1162/089976604774201631 pmid:15228749
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Rogerson T, Cai DJ, Frank A, Sano Y, Shobe J, Lopez-Aranda MF, Silva AJ (2014) Synaptic tagging during memory allocation. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:157–169. doi:10.1038/nrn3667 pmid:24496410
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Rugg MD, Coles MGH (1995) Electrophysiology of mind: event-related brain potentials and cognition. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.
  27. ↵
    Salari N, Rose M (2016) Dissociation of the functional relevance of different pre-stimulus oscillatory activity for memory formation. Neuroimage 125:1013–1021. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.037 pmid:26484828
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Sanquist TF, Rohrbaugh JW, Syndulko K, Lindsley DB (1980) Electrocortical signs of levels of processing: perceptual analysis and recognition memory. Psychophysiology 17:568–576. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1980.tb02299.x pmid:7443924
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Staresina BP, Reber TP, Niediek J, Boström J, Elger CE, Mormann F (2019) Recollection in the human hippocampal-entorhinal cell circuitry. Nat Commun 10:1503. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-09558-3 pmid:30944325
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Urgolites ZJ, Wixted JT, Goldinger SD, Papesh MH, Treiman DM, Squire LR, Steinmetz PN (2020) Spiking activity in the human hippocampus prior to encoding predicts subsequent memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 117:13767–13770. doi:10.1073/pnas.2001338117 pmid:32482860
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    Valdez AB, Hickman EN, Treiman DM, Smith KA, Steinmetz PN (2013) A statistical method for predicting seizure onset zones from human single-neuron recordings. J Neural Eng 10:e016001. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/10/1/016001 pmid:23220865
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Wagner AD, Poldrack RA, Eldridge LL, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (1998a) Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroreport 9:3711–3717. pmid:9858384
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Dale AM, Rosen BR, Buckner RL (1998b) Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science 281:1188–1191. doi:10.1126/science.281.5380.1188 pmid:9712582
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Wittmann BC, Bunzeck N, Dolan RJ, Düzel E (2007) Anticipation of novelty recruits reward system and hippocampus while promoting recollection. Neuroimage 38:194–202. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.038 pmid:17764976
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Yoo HB, Umbach G, Lega B (2021) Neurons in the human medial temporal lobe track multiple temporal contexts during episodic memory processing. Neuroimage 245:118689. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118689 pmid:34742943
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Christina Zelano, Northwestern University - Chicago

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Salman Qasim.

The reviewers and I agree that this study has potential to contribute rare insight into human hippocampal function, and to our understanding of pre-stimulus brain states. The approach, including human single-unit recordings, is novel and the authors found evidence in support of their hypotheses. However, both reviewers raised substantial concerns about the statistics and felt that additional methods details were needed in order to assess the validity of the reported findings. See reviewers’ detailed comments which need to be addressed below:

Reviewer 1:

Here, the authors propose that increases in hippocampal firing rates preceding stimulus onset were predictive of stimulus memory in humans performing a word memory task, without reinstatement of this activity during retrieval. The motivation for this study is well-described, and the approach (human single-unit recordings) is novel and could provide a rare insight into human hippocampal function.

However, there are substantial weaknesses in the reported analytical and statistical methods that prevent me from assessing the validity of the stated conclusions. I thus cannot recommend the manuscript for publication in its current state. I describe these below and make prescriptive recommendations.

1. There is no statistical test or threshold provided to characterize significant increases in firing rate. This is critical, as the authors seem to cherry-pick 12 units showing “significant” activity preceding stimulus, but do not describe how this was determined relative to any null hypothesis of chance-level spiking. This is especially problematic as these 12 units are then analyzed at the group-level in Figures 4 and 5, making it difficult to assess these subsequent results.

a. To address this, the authors should provide a statistically determined threshold mean firing rate that these units exceed, or a statistical method for identifying timepoints at which the peak firing rate is significantly elevated.

b. A completely alternative (and, in my opinion, superior) approach might be to compute a true subsequent memory-effect as done in prior literature: compute a within-cell comparison of firing rate between remembered and forgotten trials, yielding a single test-statistic per cell. The authors do this just for Figure 3, but seemingly abandon the approach for categorizing “pre-SME” cells. An example of this approach is clearly demonstrated in PMID: 34742943.

2. More rigorous (permutation-based) statistics are required for between-condition comparisons to account for the possibility that the group results are caused by the very large difference in sample sizes between remembered and forgotten conditions that are exacerbated by the very small amount of data (1 second, low firing rates) analyzed.

3. In addition, due to the overall low sample size, it is critical that the authors provide raster plots of all of the neurons identified as having the identified pre-SME effects. The most compelling figure in this manuscript is Figure S2, where the authors provide one such example. This will build confidence in the results, which is important because the small sample size inherently limits certain statistical approaches that are norms in the field.

Reviewer 2:

This manuscript investigates preparatory hippocampal activity for memory encoding and tests whether neural firing prior to memory encoding predicts subsequent memory. The authors perform single neuron recordings in four patients with epilepsy during word memory tasks. The authors find evidence in support of their hypothesis. Overall, the manuscript may add to our understanding of pre-stimulus brain states but I suggest several revisions prior to publication.

1. This manuscript relies critically on the timing of activity, yet no information is provided as to how neural and behavioral data were synchronized. The authors should provide these details, along with any estimate of timing uncertainty (e.g. synchronization error)?

2. There are numerous grammatical mistakes throughout. I am sympathetic to those writing in a secondary language but I would nonetheless respectfully suggest they consult with a writing professional at their institution to clean up such errors.

3. The data come from a relatively small number of neurons (29 total). The authors then do “population” statistics on 12 units, which I am not sure is valid, particularly if unit isolation is imperfect and “neurons” are not truly independent. At the very least this should be mentioned as a study limitation.

4. Please provide example waveforms and or raw data to evaluate spike sorting and data signal quality.

5. Aside from Figure S2, I would prefer if the authors provided more single-trial raster plots for those neurons showing pre-stimulus and post-stimulus subsequent memory effects.

6. Hippocampal subfield segmentation with fMRI is notoriously tricky. The authors claim the recordings are from CA3 without providing sufficient detail to evaluate this claim.

7. Figure 5 suggests that pre-stimulus effects are larger than during-stimulus effects, which is very surprising.

Minor points

8. The authors should cite the substantial evidence of pre-stimulus (oscillatory) brain states on memory in the introduction, such as Guderian et al 2009. Other work using fMRI or during retrieval (work by Richard Addante, Juergen Fell and others) is also relevant and could be added.

Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 10 (2)
eNeuro
Vol. 10, Issue 2
February 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Hippocampal Neuronal Activity Preceding Stimulus Predicts Later Memory Success
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Hippocampal Neuronal Activity Preceding Stimulus Predicts Later Memory Success
Soyeon Jun, June Sic Kim, Chun Kee Chung
eNeuro 31 January 2023, 10 (2) ENEURO.0252-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0252-22.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
Hippocampal Neuronal Activity Preceding Stimulus Predicts Later Memory Success
Soyeon Jun, June Sic Kim, Chun Kee Chung
eNeuro 31 January 2023, 10 (2) ENEURO.0252-22.2023; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0252-22.2023
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • hippocampus
  • memory
  • neuronal activity
  • prediction
  • single-unit activity

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Article: Confirmation

  • Glycolytic System in Axons Supplement Decreased ATP Levels after Axotomy of the Peripheral Nerve
  • Manipulating the Rapid Consolidation Periods in a Learning Task Affects General Skills More than Statistical Learning and Changes the Dynamics of Learning
  • Emergent Low-Frequency Activity in Cortico-Cerebellar Networks with Motor Skill Learning
Show more Research Article: Confirmation

Cognition and Behavior

  • Environment Enrichment Facilitates Long-Term Memory Consolidation Through Behavioral Tagging
  • Effects of cortical FoxP1 knockdowns on learned song preference in female zebra finches
  • The genetic architectures of functional and structural connectivity properties within cerebral resting-state networks
Show more Cognition and Behavior

Subjects

  • Cognition and Behavior

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.