Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
eNeuro

eNeuro

Advanced Search

 

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Blog
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • TOPICS
    • Cognition and Behavior
    • Development
    • Disorders of the Nervous System
    • History, Teaching and Public Awareness
    • Integrative Systems
    • Neuronal Excitability
    • Novel Tools and Methods
    • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Editorial Board
    • For the Media
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact Us
    • Feedback
  • SUBMIT
PreviousNext
Opinion, Sensory and Motor Systems

From Intention to Action: Hierarchical Sensorimotor Transformation in the Posterior Parietal Cortex

He Cui
eNeuro 12 November 2014, 1 (1) ENEURO.0017-14.2014; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0017-14.2014
He Cui
Brain and Behavior Discovery Institute and Department of Psychiatry and Health Behavior, Medical College of Georgia, Georgia Regents University, Augusta, Georgia 30912
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Cover Figure
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Cover Figure

Although both the PRR and area 5d have been found to be intimately involved in visually guided reach, several recent studies of reference frame, decision making, and sequential movement suggest that they might play distinct roles in planning and control of reach at different levels of complexity, indicating a hierarchical circuitry participating in translation from abstract intention into concrete action plan.

  • action selection
  • decision making
  • reference frame
  • sensorimotor integration
  • sequential movement

Significance Statement

While there is a large body of evidence in favor of a clustering of representations of body effectors in the posterior parietal cortex, recent advances have revealed a hierarchical organization beyond a flat intentional map composed of functionally distinct subdivisions operating in parallel at same level. In particular, the parietal reach region (PRR) and dorsal area 5 (area 5d) have been found to play distinct roles in visually-guided reach. Based on three lines of neurophysiological studies on nonhuman primates that utilize sophisticated behavioral tasks, including reference frame, effector choice, and sequential reach, this essay proposes that the PRR and area 5d are involved in translation of general motor intentions into detailed motor programs at different stages.

Introduction

The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) has historically been considered a typical association cortex, important for spatial attention and multisensory integration in the generation of a unitary map whose output is relayed to the frontal lobe to guide behavior (Critchley, 1953; Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982; Colby and Goldberg, 1999). Since the 1970s, tremendous progress in neurophysiology, neuroanatomical tracing, functional imaging, and experimental intervention has yielded evidence of a variety of distinct functional subareas in the PPC, as opposed to a homogeneous area that constructs a single unified perceptual representation (Rizzolatti et al., 1997; Mountcastle, 1998; Andersen and Buneo, 2002). Furthermore, the PPC has been shown to be involved in movement planning in a number of different contexts (Andersen and Cui, 2009; Rizzolatti and Kalaska, 2013). The PPC seems to be composed of a mosaic of intentional maps, each of which is thought to be specialized for a different kind of movement for a particular body part (Andersen and Buneo, 2002; Scherberger et al., 2013). Recent advances have further suggested that the individual effector-specific regions are heterogeneous (Heed et al., 2011; Leone et al., 2014), with a hierarchical organization among different subdivisions preferring the same effector (Cui and Andersen, 2011; Verhagen et al., 2013), indicating that some subareas in PPC are involved in sensorimotor integration at multiple levels, instead of working in parallel within a flat intentional map. This essay will focus on two reach-related areas in Brodmann’s area 5 (Fig. 1A), the parietal reach region (PRR), and dorsal area 5 (area 5d), and discuss the functional relationship between them based on recent neurophysiological studies using a variety of sensorimotor contingencies.

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1

Anatomical and functional distinctions between two reach-related areas in the PPC. A, Locations of the PRR (MIP) and area 5d. B, Three paradigms to distinguish general motor intention from detailed motor programs. In the reference frame task, general motor intention is represented in extrinsic space, while physical movement is prepared in intrinsic space. In the effector-choice task, multiple potential action plans can be formed prior to specification of the concrete motor parameters controlling the end-effector. In the sequential reach task, an action sequence encompassing all motor components at a cognitive level is formed before it is unfolded into a series of detailed programs of element movements.

The PRR was originally defined as an area medial and posterior to the lateral intraparietal area (LIP), including more than one cytotectonic area, whose cells are more active for reaching arm movements than for saccadic eye movements (Snyder et al., 1997; Quian Quiroga et al., 2006; Cui and Andersen, 2007). Many subsequent neurophysiological studies of the PRR have targeted the medial bank of the IPS (MIP) (Baldauf et al., 2008; Cui and Andersen, 2007, 2011; Pesaran et al., 2006, 2008; Scherberger and Andersen, 2007). Reversibly inactivating the PRR in monkeys causes miss-reach, similar to the optic ataxia reported in human patients (Hwang et al. 2012). In addition, pre-movement activity is not simply related to the cue, because the PRR has been found to encode the desired movement goal during an anti-reach task (Gail and Andersen, 2006). Moreover, the reaching plan embedded in the PRR includes not only target locations or end-points, but also high-level kinematics such as movement trajectories (Torres et al., 2013), supporting the view that movement trajectories are geometrically formed, independently of physical motor commands (Torres and Zipser, 2002; Torres and Andersen, 2006).

Area 5d, which is located caudal to the primary somatosensory cortex (SI) and medial to the IPS, is involved in representing spatial information for limbs and control of reaching arm movements (Lacquaniti et al. 1995; Kalaska et al. 1997). It receives major cortical inputs from SI (Jones et al. 1978; Pearson and Powell 1985), and is reciprocally interconnected with primary and premotor cortex (Strick and Kim, 1978). It was previously thought of as a higher somatosensory area that codes posture and joint positions (Sakata et al., 1973). However, studies on behaving monkeys demonstrate that cells in area 5 became much more active during arm movements (Mountcastle et al., 1975), with strong directional tuning (Kalaska et al., 1983). During delayed reach, many area 5 cells exhibit sustained activity far before the earliest increase in electromyographic activity (Kalaska and Crammond, 1995). Compared with primary and premotor cortex, area 5 seems to be less directly involved in control of musculoskeletal dynamics: it appears to be insensitive to external force load, and to encode movement kinematics instead of dynamics (Kalaska et al., 1990). Also, many area 5 neurons show strong firing patterns that continuously covary with hand trajectory (Archambault et al., 2011; Hauschild et al., 2012), a manually/brain controlled cursor (Mulliken et al., 2008), or even the seen position of a realistic fake arm (Graziano et al., 2000).

Although both the PRR and area 5d have been found to be intimately involved in planning of reach, several recent studies of reference frame, decision making, and sequential movement suggest that they might play distinct roles in planning and control of reaching arm movement at different (abstract vs concrete) levels of complexity (Fig. 1B), indicating a hierarchical circuitry that is involved in translation from cognitive intention into detailed motor programs to implement a key step of the complex sensorimotor transformations from receptors to muscle activation patterns.

Reference Frame

The first findings in favor of a hierarchical PRR−area 5d circuitry emerged from experiments characterizing sensorimotor coordinates with a variety of combinations of target, eye, and hand positions (Andersen at al., 1997). Unlike an oculomotor plan in eye‐centered coordinates that is always registered to its sensory target in a retinotopic map, goal-directed reach initially planned in visual space (Flanagan and Rao, 1995; Wolpert et al., 1995) requires a transformation from extrapersonal to joint/muscle coordinates. Recent studies demonstrate that different coordinate systems are employed in the PRR and area 5d (Batista et al., 1999; Bremner and Andersen, 2012), suggesting that these areas are involved in reach planning in extrinsic and intrinsic spaces, respectively.

Neurophysiological recordings during a delayed-reach task revealed a gradient distribution in the medial wall of the IPS, with deep (superficial) neurons tending to be more modulated in the cue (movement) epoch (Johnson et al., 1996), suggesting that they are involved in different stages of the sensorimotor transformation (Battaglia-Mayer et al., 2003). Reaching goals have been found to be represented in retinal coordinates (Batista et al., 1999; Pesaran et al. 2006) in the PRR, indicating a cognitive plan at a more conceptual level than at the subsequent stages of motor planning. That is, the PRR only conveys abstract information (i.e., the spatial goal) about upcoming movements, without specifying physical movement parameters (i.e., joint angles). Nonetheless, accurate motor control must take into account intrinsic variables, such as posture, hand position, the dynamics and structure of muscles, etc. Therefore, abstract motor intentions ultimately must be converted into detailed physical motor plans, with movement parameters specified in muscle/joint coordinates to activate musculoskeletal system (Kalaska et al., 1997). The first step in converting target location in gaze-centered coordinates into limb motor commands for reaching is to incorporate proprioceptive and efference copy information about starting hand position and intrinsic arm posture. It seems likely that area 5d plays a key role in this stage by providing target information with respect to initial hand position to specify action parameters in musculoskeletal coordinates (Buneo et al., 2002). From the PRR to area 5d, neuronal representations of the reaching goal progressively shift from eye-centered to hand-centered along the ventral−dorsal axis of the medial bank of the IPS (Buneo and Andersen, 2006), so that pre-movement activity in area 5d predominantly encodes the reach vector (Bremner and Andersen, 2012). However, this hand-centered coding is not static, but gradually emerges during the reach planning period (Bremner and Andersen, 2014), supporting the view that area 5d integrates the intrinsic biomechanics of the musculoskeletal system by dynamically incorporating information about intrinsic arm posture.

Decision Making

Another approach to elucidating parietal sensorimotor circuitry is to identify distinct functional areas for decision making and motor planning. Decision making initially was assumed to be a neural process separate from action planning (Miller et al., 1960; Keele, 1968). Nevertheless, attempts to distinguish decision making from motor planning with target selection tasks have proven frustrating, because virtually all motor-related areas seem to convey potential movements to candidate targets, and multiple potential plans exist concurrently before the final action is chosen (Riehle and Requin, 1989; Platt and Glimcher, 1997; Basso and Wurtz, 1998; Cisek and Kalaska, 2005). Consequentially, it is commonly believed that target selection and movement preparation involve overlapping brain circuits, and are performed in an integrated manner, as opposed to a serial model in which decision making occurs before action planning (Shadlen and Newsome, 2001; Wang, 2008; Cisek and Kalaska, 2010). From the evolutionary point of view, forming multiple potential action plans prior to choosing among them might not only benefit reward prediction, but might also reduce reaction time (Andersen and Cui, 2009; Cisek and Kalaska, 2010). Nevertheless, the idea of integrated circuitry for decision making and motor planning has only been tested for spatial target selection (Cisek, 2007), which involves spatial attention, and in turn engages numerous cortical areas (Desimone and Duncan, 1995). It is unclear if plan selection and action preparation are embedded in segregated areas for other kinds of decision making, such as nonspatial effector choice.

To examine anatomical overlap for decision making and motor planning in nonspatial action selection, an effector-choice task was designed in which monkeys autonomously chose between a saccade and a hand reach to the same visual stimulus (Cui and Andersen, 2007). Monkeys were required to play a mixed-strategy game against a computer (Barraclough et al., 2004) to compensate for a potential bias due to movement costs. The effector-choice trials were pseudo-randomly interleaved with effector-instructed trials in which the monkeys were explicitly cued to make either a saccade or a reach to the target in the middle of a trial to discourage premature decisions. The effector-choice task allows potential motor plans to be formed without immediate specification of concrete motor parameters controlling a particular end-effector, providing an ideal paradigm to determine whether action selection and motor preparation are encoded in overlapping or in distinct areas in nonspatial decision making. If a brain region is involved in effector decision formation, then it should encode potential motor plans prior to effector selection. Conversely, if an area only carries selected motor plans, then it should only reflect the decision outcome after the effector is unambiguously specified. Interestingly, recent studies demonstrate remarkable qualitative differences between LIP/PRR and area 5d (Cui and Andersen, 2007, 2011). Whereas the LIP and PPR encode potential motor plans, area 5d encodes only selected reach plans after the arm is chosen as the effector, suggesting that it is downstream to the PRR in a serial visuomotor cortical circuit (Cui and Andersen, 2011). While the PRR and area 5d presumably work in concert with premotor and primary motor areas, respectively (Wise et al., 1997), the functional relationship between the parietal and frontal areas in effector choice remains unclear.

Sequential Planning

In contrast to the prefrontal cortex (Tanji, 2001), the PPC has historically received little attention in studies of sequential planning. Nonetheless, neurological studies have suggested that the PPC is crucial for serial behaviour (Buxbaum, 1998; Zadikoff and Lang, 2005). Patients with damage to the left parietal lobe appear normal in performing elementary movements, but are impaired in generating complex action sequences, indicating a profound role of the PPC in integrating multiple spatial goals into a motor sequence.

To reveal the functional role of PPC in sequential arm movements, single-neuron activity from the PRR (Baldauf et al., 2008) and area 5d (Li and Cui, 2013) has been recorded from monkeys performing a double-reach task. When the monkeys prepared to reach two simultaneously presented targets with sequential arm movements, most PRR neurons carried information about both the immediate and the subsequent goals (Baldauf et al., 2008). Situated at the early stage of the visuomotor transformation, the PRR presumably is involved in the first step to generate a coherent sequence, integrating information about all component movements into a high-level movement plan at an abstract level. Nevertheless, a conceptual sequential plan encompassing multiple goals in parallel ultimately must be decomposed into serial motor commands to drive the musculoskeletal system. In contrast to the PRR, area 5d has been found to only encode the immediate upcoming reach, and not the subsequent movement (Li and Cui, 2013). Area 5d activity is tightly coupled only to the next upcoming movement, suggesting it might play a key role in integrating the visual goals from the PRR with physical limb information to form a state estimation (Mulliken et al., 2008; Shi et al., 2013). Moreover, the component reach encoded in area 5d, as opposed to the sequential information being conveyed in the PRR and other cortical sensorimotor areas, suggests that unfolding of the movement sequence begins in the parietal-frontal cortex, including the PPC, prefrontal cortex, premotor cortex, primary motor cortex, and supplemental motor cortex, instead of being exclusively conducted by downstream subcortical and spinal circuits. Again, decomposition of the motor sequence appears to engage a larger sensorimotor network via mutual communication between parietal and frontal cortices (Pesaran et al., 2008).

Discussion

A series of recent experiments have been conducted on monkeys performing a variety of sensorimotor tasks to elucidate distinct roles of the PRR and area 5d at different stages of the sensorimotor transformation. First, the reference frame task allowed us to isolate general motor intentions in extrinsic space and physical movement preparation in intrinsic space. Second, the effector-choice task allowed potential action plans to be formed without immediate specification of the concrete motor parameters controlling the end-effector. Third, the sequential reach task enabled us to isolate the cognitive action sequence from the detailed motor program of element movements. Three lines of evidence obtained with these tasks suggest that the PRR and area 5d form a hierarchical sensorimotor circuitry that translates abstract intentions into detailed motor plans.

Despite the markedly different functional roles between PRR and area 5d, we should keep in mind that they are not sequentially involved in converting extrinsic stimuli into intrinsic motor plans in a hardwired fashion. First, although a serial neural process was observed in the sensorimotor tasks mentioned above, effector specificity and information flow might be different in behavioral contexts other than visually guided reach (e.g., Swaminathan et al., 2013; Woloszyn and Shadlen, 2013). Secondly, behavioral and computational studies have suggested that the sensorimotor transformation emerges through an intimate interplay between sensory inflow and motor outflow through paired forward and inverse internal models (Wolpert and Kawato, 1998; Shadmehr and Wise, 2005): the forward model translates motor commands into anticipated sensory outcomes, whereas the inverse model converts desired sensory consequences into motor commands (Franklin and Wolpert, 2011; Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 2012). Although the prevalent view posits that sensorimotor control largely relies on forward prediction of sensory consequences (Wolpert et al., 2011), direct neurophysiological evidence that persuasively links neural firing to predicted sensory consequences is still lacking, because the internal prediction of sensory consequences is inherently different from actual sensorimotor variables, and seems not to be behaviorally measurable. Since most previous studies have used reactive movements to stationary goals predefined by sensory cues, it is difficult to determine whether the neural activity observed reflects sensory stimuli or predicts impending movements. Although corollary discharges have been found in diverse species (Crapse and Sommer, 2008) for gating sensory inputs (e.g., Lee and Malpeli, 1998) and updating perception (e.g., Duhamel et al., 1992; Sommer and Wurtz, 2002; Synofzik et al., 2008), as well as for distinguishing sensory signals between active and passive motion (e.g., Roy and Cullen, 2004), it remains unclear how they are integrated with sensory inflow in recipient structures to form internal models for directing movement. Further elucidation of the PPC sensorimotor circuitry calls for novel behavioral tasks that are highly dependent on predictive spatiotemporal transformations.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Whitehall Foundation and Alfred P. Sloan Research Fellowship. I thank R.A. Andersen, D. Baldauf, D. Blake, Y. Li, A. Saul, Y. Wang, and Z. Yang for helpful discussions, and J.G. Malpeli for critical comments on the manuscript.

Footnotes

  • ↵1 Author reports no conflict of interest.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License Attribution-Noncommercial 4.0 International which permits noncommercial reuse provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    Andersen RA, Buneo CA (2002) Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 25:189–220. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142922 pmid:12052908
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Andersen RA, Cui H (2009) Intention, action planning, and decision making in parietal-frontal circuits. Neuron 63:568–583. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.028 pmid:19755101
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Bradley DC, Xing J (1997) Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Ann Rev Neurosci 20:303–330. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.303 pmid:9056716
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. Archambault PS, Ferrari-Toniolo S, Battaglia-Mayer A (2011) Online control of hand trajectory and evolution of motor intention in the parietofrontal system. J Neurosci 31:742–752. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2623-10.2011 pmid:21228183
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    Baldauf D, Cui H Andersen RA (2008) The posterior parietal cortex encodes in parallel both goals for double-reach sequences. J Neurosci 28:10081–10089. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3423-08.2008 pmid:18829966
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Barraclough DJ, Conroy ML, Lee D (2004) Prefrontal cortex and decision making in a mixed-strategy game. Nat Neurosci 7:404–410. doi:10.1038/nn1209 pmid:15004564
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Basso MA, Wurtz RH (1998) Modulation of neuronal activity in superior colliculus by changes in target probability. J Neurosci 18:7519–7534. pmid:9736670
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    Batista AP, Buneo CA, Snyder LH Andersen RA (1999) Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. Science 285:257–260. pmid:10398603
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Battaglia-Mayer A, Caminiti R, Lacquaniti F, Zago M (2003) Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network. Cereb Cortex 13:1009–1022. pmid:12967918
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Bremner LR, Andersen RA (2012) Coding of the reach vector in parietal area 5d. Neuron 75:342–351. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.041 pmid:22841318
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Bremner LR, Andersen RA (2014) Temporal analysis of reference frames in parietal cortex area 5d during reach planning. J Neurosci 34:5273–5284. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2068-13.2014 pmid:24719105
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    Buneo CA, Andersen RA (2006) The posterior parietal cortex: sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements. Neuropsychologia 44:2594–2606. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.011 pmid:16300804
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Buneo CA, Jarvis MR, Batista AP Andersen RA (2002) Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching. Nature 416:632–636. doi:10.1038/416632a pmid:11948351
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Buxbaum LJ (1998) Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. Cogn Neuropsychol 15:617–643. doi:10.1080/026432998381032 pmid:22448839
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Cisek P (2007) Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis. Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 362:1585–1599. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2054 pmid:17428779
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Cisek P, Kalaska JF (2005) Neural correlates of reaching decisions in dorsal premotor cortex: specification of multiple direction choices and final selection of action. Neuron 45:801–814. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.027 pmid:15748854
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Cisek P, Kalaska JF (2010) Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:269–298. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135409 pmid:20345247
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Colby CL, Goldberg ME (1999) Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 22:319–349. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.319 pmid:10202542
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Crapse TB, Sommer MA (2008) Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:587–600. doi:10.1038/nrn2457 pmid:18641666
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Critchley M (1953) The parietal lobe. London: Arnold.
  21. ↵
    Cui H, Andersen RA (2007) Posterior parietal cortex encodes autonomously selected motor plans. Neuron 56:552–559. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.031 pmid:17988637
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Cui H, Andersen RA (2011) Different representations of potential and selected motor plans by distinct parietal areas. J Neurosci 31:18130–18136. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6247-10.2011 pmid:22159124
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18:193–222. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205 pmid:7605061
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME (1992) The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science 255:90–92. pmid:1553535
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Flanagan JR, Rao AK (1995) Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. J Neurophysiol 74: 2174-2178.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Franklin DW, Wolpert DM (2011) Computational mechanisms of sensorimotor control. Neuron 72:425–442. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006 pmid:22078503
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Gail A, Andersen RA (2006) Neural dynamics in monkey parietal reach region reflect context-specific sensorimotor transformations. J Neurosci 26:9376–9384. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1570-06.2006 pmid:16971521
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. Graziano MS, Cooke DF, Taylor CS (2000) Coding the location of the arm by sight. Science 290:1782–1786. pmid:11099420
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    Hauschild M, Mulliken GH, Fineman I, Loeb GE Andersen RA, (2012) Cognitive signals for brain-machine interfaces in posterior parietal cortex include continuous 3D trajectory commands. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:17075–17080. doi:10.1073/pnas.1215092109 pmid:23027946
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. He SQ, Dum RP, Strick PL (1993) Topographic organization of corticospinal projections from the frontal lobe: motor areas on the lateral surface of the hemisphere. J Neurosci 13:952–980. pmid:7680069
    OpenUrlAbstract
  31. ↵
    Heed T, Beurze SM, Toni I, Roder B, Medendorp WP (2011) Functional rather than effector-specific organization of human posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 31:3066–3076. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4370-10.2011 pmid:21414927
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    Hwang EJ, Hauschild M, Wilke M Andersen RA (2012) Inactivation of the parietal reach region causes optic ataxia, impairing reaches but not saccades. Neuron 76:1021–1029. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.030 pmid:23217749
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Johnson PB, Ferraina S, Bianchi L, Caminiti R (1996) Cortical networks for visual reaching: physiological and anatomical organization of frontal and parietal lobe arm regions. Cereb Cortex 6:102–119. pmid:8670643
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Jones EG, Coulter JD, Hendry SH (1978) Intracortical connectivity of architectonic fields in the somatic sensory, motor and parietal cortex of monkeys. J Comp Neurol 181:291–347. doi:10.1002/cne.901810206 pmid:99458
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Kalaska JF, Crammond DJ (1995) Deciding not to GO: neuronal correlates of response selection in a GO/NOGO task in primate premotor and parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex 5:410–428. pmid:8547788
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Georgopoulos AP (1983) Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex. Exp Brain Res 51:247–260. pmid:6617794
    OpenUrlPubMed
  37. ↵
    Kalaska JF, Cohen DA, Prud'homme M, Hyde ML (1990) Parietal area 5 neuronal activity encodes movement kinematics, not movement dynamics. Exp Brain Res 80:351–364. pmid:2113482
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    Kalaska JF, Scott SH, Cisek P, Sergio LE (1997) Cortical control of reaching movements. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:849–859. pmid:9464979
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Keele SW (1968) Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychol Bull 70:387–403.doi:10.1037/h0026739
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. ↵
    Lacquaniti F, Guigon E, Bianchi L, Ferraina S, Caminiti R (1995) Representing spatial information for limb movement: role of area 5 in the monkey. Cereb Cortex 5:391–409. pmid:8547787
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Lee D, Malpeli JG (1998) Effects of saccades on the activity of neurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurophysiol 79:922–936. pmid:9463453
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    Leone FT, Heed T, Toni I, Medendorp WP (2014) Understanding effector selectivity in human posterior parietal cortex by combining information patterns and activation measures. J Neurosci 34:7102–7112. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5242-13.2014 pmid:24849346
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Li Y, Cui H, (2013) Dorsal parietal area 5 encodes immediate reach in sequential arm movements. J Neurosci 33:14455–14465. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1162-13.2013 pmid:24005297
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    Miller GA, Galanter E, Pribram KH (1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
  45. ↵
    Mountcastle VB (1998) Perceptual neuroscience: the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UP.
  46. ↵
    Mountcastle VB, Lynch JC, Georgopoulos AP, Sakata H, Acuna C (1975) Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. J Neurophysiol 38:871–908. pmid:808592
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    Mulliken GH, Musallam S, Andersen RA (2008) Forward estimation of movement state in posterior parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:8170–8177. doi:10.1073/pnas.0802602105 pmid:18499800
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    Pearson RC, Powell TP (1985) The projection of the primary somatic sensory cortex upon area 5 in the monkey. Brain Res 356:89–107. pmid:3995356
    OpenUrlPubMed
  49. ↵
    Pesaran B, Nelson MJ, Andersen RA (2006) Dorsal premotor neurons encode the relative position of the hand, eye, and goal during reach planning. Neuron 51:125–134. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.025 pmid:16815337
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Pesaran B, Nelson MJ, Andersen RA (2008) Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature. 453:406–409. doi:10.1038/nature06849 pmid:18418380
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Platt ML, Glimcher PW (1997) Responses of intraparietal neurons to saccadic targets and visual distracters. J Neurophysiol 78:1574–1589. pmid:9310444
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    Quian Quiroga R, Snyder LH, Batista AP, Cui H Andersen RA (2006) Movement intention is better predicted than attention in the posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 26:3615–3620. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3468-05.2006 pmid:16571770
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    Riehle A, Requin J (1989) Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. J Neurophysiol 61:534–549. pmid:2709098
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    Rizzolatti G, Kalaska J (2013) Voluntary movement: the parietal and premotor cortex. In: Principles of neural science (Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, Siegelbaum SA, Hudspeth AJ, eds), pp 863–893. New York: McGaw-Hill.
  55. ↵
    Rizzolatti GL, Fogassi L, Gallese V (1997) Parietal cortex: from sight to action. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:562–567. pmid:9287198
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Roy JE, Cullen KE (2004) Dissociating self-generated from passively applied head motion: neural mechanisms in the vestibular nuclei. J Neurosci 24:2102–2111. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3988-03.2004 pmid:14999061
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    Sakata H, Takaoka Y, Kawarasaki A, Shibutani H (1973) Somatosensory properties of neurons in the superior parietal cortex (area 5) of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 64:85–102. pmid:4360893
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. Scherberger H, Andersen RA (2007) Target selection signals for arm reaching in the posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 27:2001–2012. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4274-06.2007 pmid:17314296
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    Scherberger H, Quian Quiroga R, Andersen RA (2013). Coding of movement intentions. In: Principles of neural coding (Quian Quiroga R, Panzeri S, eds), pp 303–321. Boca Raton, FL: CRC.
  60. ↵
    Shadlen MN, Newsome WT (2001) Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 86:1916–1936. pmid:11600651
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi S (2012) Biological learning and control: how the brain builds representations, predicts events, and makes decisions. Cambridge: MIT.
  62. ↵
    Shadmehr R, Wise SP (2005) The computational neurobiology of reaching and pointing: a foundation for motor learning. Cambridge: MIT.
  63. Shi Y, Apker G, Buneo CA (2013) Multimodal representation of limb endpoint position in the posterior parietal cortex. J Neurophysiol 109:2097–2107. doi:10.1152/jn.00223.2012 pmid:23343899
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    Snyder LH, Batista AP Andersen RA (1997) Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex. Nature 386:167–170. doi:10.1038/386167a0 pmid:9062187
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Sommer MA, Wurtz RH (2002) A pathway in primate brain for internal monitoring of movements. Science 296:1480–1482. doi:10.1126/science.1069590 pmid:12029137
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    Strick PL, Kim CC (1978) Input to primate motor cortex from posterior parietal cortex (area 5). I. Demonstration by retrograde transport. Brain Res 157:325–330. pmid:102409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Swaminathan SK, Masse NY, Freedman DJ (2013) A comparison of lateral and medial intraparietal areas during a visual categorization task. J Neurosci 33:13157–13170. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5723-12.2013 pmid:23926269
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    Synofzik M, Lindner A, Their P (2008) The cerebellum updates predictions about the visual consequences of one's behavior. Curr Biol 18:814–818. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.071 pmid:18514520
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    Tanji J (2001) Sequential organization of multiple movements: involvement of cortical motor areas. Annu Rev Neurosci 24:631–651. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.631 pmid:11520914
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    Torres EB, Andersen RA (2006) Space-time separation during obstacle-avoidance learning in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 96:2613–2632. doi:10.1152/jn.00188.2006 pmid:16855113
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  71. ↵
    Torres EB, Zipser D (2002) Reaching to grasp with a multi-jointed arm. I. Computational model. J Neurophysiol 88:2355–2367. doi:10.1152/jn.00030.2002 pmid:12424277
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    Torres EB, Quian Quiroga R, Cui H, Buneo CA (2013) Neural correlates of learning and trajectory planning in the posterior parietal cortex. Front Integr Neurosci 7:39.doi:10.3389/fnint.2013.00039 pmid:23730275
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M (1982) Two cortical visual systems. In: Analysis of visual behavior (Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJ, eds.), pp 549. Cambridge: MIT.
  74. ↵
    Verhagen L, Dijkerman HC, Medendorp WP, Toni I, (2013) Hierarchical organization of parietofrontal circuits during goal-directed action. J Neurosci 33:6492–6503. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3928-12.2013 pmid:23575847
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    Wang XJ (2008) Decision making in recurrent neuronal circuits. Neuron 60:215–234. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.034 pmid:18957215
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    Wise SP, Boussaoud D, Johnson PB, Caminiti R (1997) Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations. Annu Rev Neurosci 20:25–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.25 pmid:9056706
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    Woloszyn L, Shadlen MN (2013) Responses of neurons in cortical areas LIP and MIP during a reaction time perceptual decision-making task communicated with an eye or a hand movement. Soc Neurosci Abstr 13: 668.13.
    OpenUrl
  78. ↵
    Wolpert DM, Kawato M (1998) Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Netw 11:1317–1329. pmid:12662752
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Michael I, Jordan MI (1995) Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? an adaptation study. Exp Brain Res 103:460–470. pmid:7789452
    OpenUrlPubMed
  80. ↵
    Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011) Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751. doi:10.1038/nrn3112 pmid:22033537
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    Zadikoff C, Lang AE (2005) Apraxia in movement disorders. Brain 128:1480–1497. doi:10.1093/brain/awh560 pmid:15930045
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text

Synthesis

The Author did not consent to publish the author response letter

The decision was a result of the Reviewing Editor Ranulfo Romo, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus was reached. A fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision is listed below. The following reviewers agreed to reveal their identity: Christos Constandtinidis and W. Pieter Medendorp

Synthesis:

The reviewers found your review paper of potential interest for publication in eNeuro, provided you address their comments.

In general, the reviewers think that you must lay down the scope of your review in the introduction section: input out-put organization of PPC and 5d. Also, there are many issues regarding what you mean by sensory-motor hierarchy, sensory-to-motor transformation, and integrated circuits for decision making, planning, and the link with frontal cortex. In the absence of a clear lay-out of these terms, it would be very difficult to detect what is new in this topic review.

It would be useful to place the PRR-5d circuit in a broader context, at least in the introduction. It would be worth considering the inputs to the PRR area from other parietal areas and outputs of area 5d to the premotor cortex. The transformations being considered represent just one step of the sensory-motor hierarchy.

There are specific comments:

Line 15: what is a flat intentional map?

Line 19: a new organizing principle?

Line 46: hollow sentence.

Line 77: Please, discuss the effector specificity could change over time, and that the network is not hardwired.

Line 88: be more specific about the involvements of PPC and 5d in the different stages of the sensory-motor transforms. Here and again, what is it a hierarchy and should refer also to Caminiti and colleagues. But, there are so many examples of hierarchy and sensory-to-motor transformation and their roles in planning, and decision making, etc.

Lines 123-125: It is unclear what is meant by integrated circuitry for decision making and motor planning for spatial target selection. The source being cited is a review (and a 20-year old one, as such).

Line 145: what is the implication of this finding and the link with frontal cortices?

Line 170: Clarify the term parietal-frontal cortex, by spelling out the relevant areas.

Line 171: expand communication via synchronization and de-synchronization between PPC and frontal cortices.

Line 174: It is unnecessary to start the Discussion with “In summary”.

Line 195: Could you please explain or consider that sensory consequences are not behaviorally measurable? Duhamel and colleagues should be discussed in this topic.

View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

eneuro: 1 (1)
eNeuro
Vol. 1, Issue 1
November/December 2014
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this eNeuro article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
From Intention to Action: Hierarchical Sensorimotor Transformation in the Posterior Parietal Cortex
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from eNeuro
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in eNeuro.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
From Intention to Action: Hierarchical Sensorimotor Transformation in the Posterior Parietal Cortex
He Cui
eNeuro 12 November 2014, 1 (1) ENEURO.0017-14.2014; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0017-14.2014

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Share
From Intention to Action: Hierarchical Sensorimotor Transformation in the Posterior Parietal Cortex
He Cui
eNeuro 12 November 2014, 1 (1) ENEURO.0017-14.2014; DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0017-14.2014
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Reference Frame
    • Decision Making
    • Sequential Planning
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Synthesis
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • action selection
  • decision making
  • reference frame
  • sensorimotor integration
  • sequential movement

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Opinion

  • Plea for a Simple But Radical Change in Scientific Publication: To Improve Openness, Reliability, and Reproducibility, Let’s Deposit and Validate Our Results before Writing Articles
  • Promoting and Optimizing the Use of 3D-Printed Objects in Spontaneous Recognition Memory Tasks in Rodents: A Method for Improving Rigor and Reproducibility
  • Exposure to Sleep, Rest, or Exercise Impacts Skill Memory Consolidation but so Too Can a Challenging Practice Schedule
Show more Opinion

Sensory and Motor Systems

  • Different control strategies drive interlimb differences in performance and adaptation during reaching movements in novel dynamics
  • The nasal solitary chemosensory cell signaling pathway triggers mouse avoidance behavior to inhaled nebulized irritants
  • Taste-odor association learning alters the dynamics of intra-oral odor responses in the posterior piriform cortex of awake rats
Show more Sensory and Motor Systems

Subjects

  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Opinion

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Issue Archive
  • Blog
  • Browse by Topic

Information

  • For Authors
  • For the Media

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(eNeuro logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
eNeuro eISSN: 2373-2822

The ideas and opinions expressed in eNeuro do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the eNeuro Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in eNeuro should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in eNeuro.