Background

Brief review of epidemiologic findings of MIA

The MIA model of neuropsychiatric disorders was derived from a series of epidemiologic studies, which demonstrated associations between maternal infections during pregnancy and schizophrenia [1,2,3]. The most informative of these studies are based on large population-based birth cohorts employing maternal serum, neonatal blood spots, and other specimens from biobanks. Nested case–control studies in these cohorts have provided evidence supporting the MIA model. Among numerous findings, these studies have revealed associations between schizophrenia and maternal serum antibody to influenza, maternal serum/neonatal antibodies to rubella [4], Toxoplasma gondii [5, 6], and herpes simplex virus type 2 [7, 8] and cytomegalovirus [9], although not all findings have been replicated.

Cohort studies derived from these populations using electronic medical records have revealed associations between several types of maternal infections and schizophrenia [1]; generally, these studies have evaluated broad classes of infections, rather than individual microbes. Other case–control studies have demonstrated relationships between inflammatory biomarkers in maternal sera. These include a positive correlation of maternal levels of the cytokines interleukin-8 [10] and TNF-α [7], and risk of schizophrenia, an inverse correlation between the levels of anti-inflammatory Th2 cytokines and risk of schizophrenia [11], a link between elevated maternal serum CRP and schizophrenia [12] and associations between elevated levels of maternal complement components and schizophrenia [13]. Some correlations, such as the one with exposure to cytomegalovirus, are associated with genetic loci suggesting a role for gene–environmental interactions [9, 14].

More recently, this work has been broadened to include other neuropsychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders. Large epidemiological studies have reported increased risk of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) associated with maternal infection during pregnancy, though results vary depending on gestational timing of the exposure, type of infectious agent, and intensity of the maternal immune response [15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23]. Additional studies have found that maternal CRP levels are increased in pregnancies giving rise to ASD cases [24], though this finding was not replicated in a different cohort [25]. Cytokines and other soluble immune molecules, quantified in specimens such as maternal blood and amniotic fluid, have also been elevated in some pregnancies which led to ASD cases [26,27,28,29]. Maternal influenza has been associated with a fivefold increased risk of bipolar disorder [30, 31] and maternal T. gondii type I was related to affective psychoses [32].

Although there is a clear need for additional epidemiological studies, the current data suggest that, at least for a subset of women, exposure to infection during pregnancy may increase the risk of CNS disorders in offspring [1, 33]. Rather than being a risk factor for a single disease or disorder, MIA may rather be a primer for a range of psychiatric disorders [34, 35]. Moreover, the diversity of infectious agents associated with an increased risk of CNS disorders suggests that the maternal immune response may be one common link between prenatal immune challenge and altered fetal brain development. Finally, infections may be tapping into immune pathways that are shared by other environmental triggers [36, 37].

Overview of MIA model experimental design considerations

Because the genetic, ecological, and behavioral diversity of humans is so remarkably heterogeneous, the development of MIA models in animals has been essential for testing causality, identifying molecular mechanisms, and developing new diagnostic tools and therapeutics. Indeed, rodent and, more recently, nonhuman primate (NHP) animal models of MIA have demonstrated a causal relationship between maternal infection and neuropathological and behavioral abnormalities consistent with a range of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders. In these models, the immune system is activated during pregnancy using a variety of immunogens, and changes in offspring brain and behavioral development that parallel features of human disease are then measured (for reviews, see [35, 38,39,40,41]). The MIA model is emerging as a powerful translational tool to explore the effects of prenatal immune challenge on the developing fetus, though the need for a more consistent model is paramount.

Developing animal models to study complex human brain diseases, such as those associated with maternal infection, pose a major challenge to preclinical research efforts [42,43,44]. Historically, the validity of animal models has been determined by: (i) Construct validity—which accounts for mechanistic similarities between the model and the clinical condition, in addition to the etiological relevance of the model to human disease(s), (ii) Face validity—resemblance of outcome measures of the model to features of the human disease, and (iii) Predictive validity—response of the model to therapeutic agents used to treat the human disease [45]. The MIA models meet all of the criteria for validity: they mimic some known biological and/or behavioral pathology and model a known disease-related risk factor, they exhibit many disease-related phenotypes, and they respond well to therapeutics used to treat CNS disorders. Although MIA models have been used most often to model ASD or SZ, the emerging consensus among leaders in the field is that prenatal infection may be relevant to a number of CNS diseases and restricting interpretation to any given human disorder may limit the utility and relevance of the MIA model [46, 47]. Rather, prenatal immune challenge may serve as a “disease primer” into an altered trajectory of fetal brain development that, in combination with other genetic and environmental factors, may ultimately result in the emergence of ASD, SZ, or other CNS disorders [34, 35, 48].

In fact, the MIA animal models are allowing us to test the hypothesis that it is the combination of the timing of exposure, the type of immune activation (viral vs. bacterial and acute vs. chronic), environmental exposures and co-morbidities (such as stress and diet), and intensity and duration of MIA that may determine the nature of brain and behavioral alterations that manifests in offspring [49, 50]. In spite of the challenges associated with methodological variability, described in detail below, several behavioral phenotypes associated with the model are quite reproducible (Fig. 1). Because the specific MIA induction and postnatal housing parameters appear to dictate the neuropathological and behavioral outcomes in offspring, the emerging view in our field is that we should not continue to ignore the details of how each lab generates their models but rather, we should embrace and explore those details because they may reveal critical information about the specific combination of conditions that cause risk. Moreover, we should not forget that maternal infections in humans do not always cause disease in offspring, suggesting that animal models could reveal why some pregnancies are susceptible, and others resilient, to MIA. Thus, rather than suggest specific recommendations for how MIA models should be generated, we are instead proposing that labs share specific details about their models to enhance their rigor, reproducibility, and translational relevance. Below, we describe several categories of experimental design considerations: (I) immune activation, (II) animals and their environment, and (III) outcome measures, and we propose a checklist for reporting these details. Although the focus here is on maternal models, the resulting guidelines may also be of use to postnatal immune challenge models, as this period is also clinically associated with alterations in development [46, 51, 52].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Reproducibility of behavioral phenotypes that are commonly assessed in mouse models of maternal immune activation, which are based on prenatal exposure to the viral mimetic Poly IC. The bar plots show the number of studies reporting a significant deficit or no deficit in the corresponding behavioral domain after prenatal Poly IC treatment during specific gestational days (GDs). The pie charts illustrate the amount of replication (in percent) between individual studies; the numbers in brackets represent the number of studies. The corresponding studies are listed in Supplementary Table 1, which provide further information regarding basic experimental design, mouse strain, age of testing, Poly IC dosing, and sample sizes. a Reproducibility of prepulse inhibition deficits in Poly IC-based maternal immune activation models. b Reproducibility of impaired social approach behavior (sociability) in Poly IC-based maternal immune activation models.

I. Immune activation

Immune activation compounds

Immune molecules such as cytokines and chemokines, and the cells that produce them within the brain are critical for normal brain development. This recognition has in recent years led to the working hypothesis that inflammatory events during pregnancy, e.g., in response to infection, may disrupt the normal expression of immune molecules during critical stages of neural development and thereby contribute to the risk for neurodevelopmental disorders. The MIA hypothesis has been tested in animal models by activating the immune system during pregnancy using a variety of immunogens and then observing changes in offspring brain and behavioral development that parallel features of human CNS disorders. Although MIA can be induced by many agents [53, 54], here we focus on the two most commonly used models, Poly IC and LPS.

Polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid (Poly IC) is a synthetic double-stranded RNA molecule that is commonly used as a viral mimetic. Poly IC is recognized by the pattern recognition receptor, toll-like receptor (TLR) 3, which specifically recognizes double-stranded RNA, the genetic information for many viruses [55]. Despite the fact that Poly IC is a synthetic analog, variations in its manufacture can have significant effects on its ability to drive an immune response and is emerging as an important consideration for MIA and latent inhibition [56, 57] models of experimental design [58]. These concerns stem, in part, from in vitro data demonstrating that Poly IC length varies among manufactures and that this variability may affect the magnitude of immune responses [59, 60]. These results have recently been extended to in vivo studies [61] that further emphasize the importance of establishing rigorous reporting guidelines for the MIA model. Perinatal Poly IC exposure typically induces a robust febrile response, a profound increase in cytokine and chemokine production (e.g., IL-1β, IL-6, TNFα, IL-10, CXCL1) [62,63,64], and HPA axis activation [65,66,67]. However, these effects are species specific; when mice are housed below thermoneutrality (approx. 20–30 °C), the more robust (i.e., longer-lasting) effect of high-dose Poly IC is hypothermia [68]. Similar to behavioral symptoms seen in individuals with schizophrenia and ASD, offspring that were prenatally treated with Poly IC have been reported to have significant abnormalities in several behaviors, including deficits in the pre-pulse inhibition [62, 63, 69,70,71], acoustic startle response, decreases in exploratory behavior in both open-field and novel-object tests, increased immobility in the forced swim test, and decreases in social interactions and sucrose/saccharine preferences [64, 72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82]. Importantly, many of these effects are dependent on the prenatal timing, Poly IC dosing, and genetic background of animals.

Lipopolysaccharide (LPS), the cell wall component of gram-negative bacteria, has been used to mimic infection in many animal studies because it initiates a well-characterized immune response via the activation of TLR4. The response to LPS also includes fever and is characterized by a robust increase in the expression of many cytokines and chemokines, including IL-1β, IL-6, TNFα, CXCL1, CXCL2, CXCL10, CCL2, and CCL7, among others, as well as a marked increase in circulating corticosterone [83,84,85,86,87]. Treatment of pregnant dams with high doses of LPS is linked to overt white matter damage, decreased oligodendrocyte development, hypo-myelination of neurons [88,89,90], and enhanced behavioral pain responses in adult offspring [91]. Lower doses of LPS given during the perinatal period also induce a number of long-term changes in offspring, both biochemical and behavioral, including social and cognitive deficits [92, 93]. For each of these immunogens, the later-life impact depends on the timing of the injection [64], in addition to multiple variables we highlight below.

All available data suggest that a maternal infection can alter the developmental trajectory of the immune system and the brain. They support the idea that whereas direct neurological injury is often associated with severe neonatal infection (e.g., Borna Disease Virus or high-dose LPS), mild-to-moderate infection and subsequent cytokine exposure during key periods of brain development may result in more subtle alterations, such as altering the response or vulnerability to a subsequent insult [94]. Whether an inflammatory event within the CNS confers either resilience or vulnerability to a later challenge is a key unanswered question and most certainly depends on several factors, including timing (early, mid, or late gestation); route of administration (intra-peritoneal, sub-cutaneous, intra-venous, intra-muscular, etc.); dose, source (vendor: Sigma, InvivogGen, etc.) and serotype of the immunogen; and even storage and administration conditions (time of day, room temperature, etc.) [58, 64, 95, 96]. Finally, stress can significantly modify immune responses; thus, factors such as cage changes at the time of challenge should also be consistent within a study, and reported in methods. We suggest that reporting of each of these variables is critical for future research reports using these MIA models.

Immune activation validation

While it is recognized that the source, type, and administration timing of immunogens used in our inflammatory models can lead to differential behavioral and physiological outcomes, it is important to continually validate our models to ensure that discrepant findings are not due to the use of less potent or inert products. Administration of immunogens such as LPS and Poly IC activate the immune system, resulting in the release of prostaglandins and proinflammatory cytokines which have been directly tied to the induction of a series of behavioral symptoms of sickness (e.g., lethargy, reduced motivation for food and social contact, piloerection/goosebumps) [97,98,99,100,101], indicative of an inflammatory response. Combined, these processes drive the initiation and maintenance of fever and are part of an adaptive strategy to help organisms fight infections [102, 103] (reviewed in [104]). The physiological and behavioral components of this “sickness response” can be quantified in order to validate commonly used animal models of inflammation.

The potency and efficacy of an immunogen can be evaluated by measuring serum or plasma levels of proinflammatory cytokines such as interleukin-1β, interleukin-6, and tumor necrosis factor alpha [83, 85, 105,106,107,108]. Methodological considerations become important here—if one were to take a blood sample from every subject [109]—this introduces handling stress into the experimental protocol. Although the use of satellite animals from each treatment group can dissociate the effects of MIA from handling stress [110, 111] (see Fig. 2), it may be possible to miss individual differences in that some animals may not mount a measurable immune response, potentially leading to variability in the data. However, borrowing from the toxicology literature, the use of satellite groups can be useful if a sufficient number of animals for meaningful scientific interpretation are used, or if recommended guidelines are followed [109,110,111]. While measurement of proinflammatory cytokines can be expensive in terms of animals, reagents, and time, it may be a gold standard that labs can use to compare across their MIA models.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Validation of inflammation in maternal immune activation (MIA) models using microsampling or satellite animal designs. a The microsampling method consists of taking small volumes of blood, plasma, and/or serum from the main experimental animals of the study in order to measure proinflammatory cytokine levels (e.g., interleukin-6) so that inflammation can be confirmed. Confounds of handling stress are an issue for this design. b In a satellite animal design, a second cohort of animals, separate from the main experimental subjects, is employed. Biological samples are collected from the satellite group only, limiting handling of the experimental group. In this design, it may be possible to miss individual differences in that some animals may not mount a measurable immune response, potentially leading to variability in the data.

Another possibility to consider is the passive evaluation of cytokine-induced sickness behaviors in the home cage, the expression of which is associated with peripheral and central cytokine activation [101, 112, 113]. This option offers a non-invasive approach to confirm immune system activation without the added confound of handling stress. It is a simple, straightforward method where blinded observers can assess animals for the presence of specific sickness behaviors (e.g., ptosis, piloerection, lethargy/reduced motor activity, and huddling) across time. In one protocol, the magnitude of each specific sickness behavior is evaluated [114], whereas another recommended protocol strictly assesses the number of sickness behaviors present [115]. In both procedures, rats and mice are then assigned a total composite sickness score. These protocols have been successfully applied to male and female rodents administered LPS and Poly IC, and have been effective in pubertal, adult, and pregnant populations [115]. Similar observational methods have been developed and applied to studies using larger animals such as non-human primates [116]. Reductions in locomotor activity following immunogen challenge can be non-invasively measured in an animals’ home cage using specialized recording and monitoring software to verify sickness, compared to vehicle-treated control animals. For singly housed animals, specialized cages can measure food and water intake, and body weight change [117], known to be affected by immunogens as part of the inflammatory sickness response [102].

Another method for validating immunogens is to measure the inflammatory fever response. This may be done by using either implantable telemetry/data logger systems or temperature probes. A potential problem with the former option is that it requires surgery for animals that will be bred after recovery (in the case of the MIA model), which can result in high per diem expenditures. While the use of radio-frequency identification (RFID) transponders, which are implanted subcutaneously with quick recovery times, can be a reliable measure of temperature [117], the required equipment and software may be costly for laboratories with limited funds. Moreover, one-time use RFID tags can put a toll on the environment. On the other hand, reusable rectal probes or tail cuffs introduce handling, which itself can be a stressor to animals [118], eliciting hyperthermia in addition to elevated levels of hormones and cytokines [118, 119]. Less invasive infrared thermometers are improving in accuracy, but again may be cost prohibitive to some laboratories.

Another critical issue related to experimental design of MIA studies is the use of pilot experiments [120, 121]. Although it seems obvious, it may be difficult to “predict” in advance what dose, administration route, and regimen are needed for one’s specific goals. Reinstating the use of dose response trials implemented in early MIA models may prove useful [76, 77, 96, 122, 123]. If, for example, one uses MIA with additional genetic or environmental manipulations, one would need to determine a dose or a range of doses that would not produce “ceiling” effects in order to be able to evaluate interactions between different factors [73, 124]. Pilot studies could also help with consideration of what would constitute a positive control for ones’ specific investigation. While negative controls usually include animals treated with vehicle and/or untreated animals, it does not seem trivial to determine what would constitute positive controls with immune stimulation. “Historical” controls based on the published and personal reports are widely used but one’s specific conditions may call for additional considerations of positive controls.

At this point, there is no consensus or guidance on how best to validate the immunogens used in our inflammatory models, or how to standardize which sources and types of immunogens we employ. Therefore, we recommend the use of our reporting chart (Table 1) so that we can collect and inform the scientific community on the most acceptable validation protocols and identify appropriate immunogens, doses, and sources.

Table 1 MIA model reporting guidelines checklist

II. Animals and environment

Species, strain, and vendor considerations

The Poly IC MIA model has attracted investigators with expertise in mouse, rat, and NHP models, which in turn has allowed the field to capitalize on the unique advantages of each species [125]. Cross-species comparisons allow preclinical researchers to compare the effects of prenatal immune challenge on evolutionarily conserved behavioral and biological outcome measures [126, 127].

Mouse models have laid the foundation for understanding the effects of MIA on fetal brain development and will undoubtedly continue to be an important species in MIA research, especially in models that incorporate genetic susceptibility. Rat models offer many of the advantages of mouse models in terms of cost, short gestational period, and the potential for genetic modifications, but also have more complex brains and display an enriched repertoire of social behavior [128]. Given that rodents comprise the vast majority of MIA model studies, it is also important to consider how strain and vendor can influence experimental outcomes. It is well established that strains of mice and rats exhibit strain-specific biological and behavioral profiles, and not unexpected that different strains may yield different outcomes in the MIA model [129]. However, mounting evidence suggests that mice from the same strain, but different vendors, may also respond differently to prenatal immune challenge due to composition of maternal gut bacteria [130]. The field of MIA research may also benefit from species that allow for a more sophisticated evaluation of social and cognitive abilities and underlying neural systems, such as the rhesus monkey [131, 132].

Although the NHP model may provide a bridge from rodent models to human disease [133], the increased costs and ethical considerations constrain the use of NHPs in research. We therefore suggest careful evaluation of which species and/or strain is most appropriate for the MIA model outcome measures of interest and reporting species, strain, and vendor details in methods. It is also important to note that species differ in maternal cytokine response, placenta function, litter size, and length of gestation. Extrapolating gestational timing across species is not always straightforward, as the gestational period of rhesus monkeys (165 days) and humans (280 days) is much longer than that of mice/rats (18–23 days) [134]. Moreover, the timing of implantation and development may be delayed following copulation due to the length of sperm viability and embryonic diapause [135, 136]. For these reasons, the method of confirming gestation should also be reported (see Table 1), whether through identification of a mating plug or by estrous cycle tracking to confirm a sustained diestrus phase, for example.

Sample size, sex determination, and group allocation

Similar to all animal studies, experiments with MIA face issues with experimental design, including the determination of appropriate group size, and important decisions on statistical analyses. Most concerns are related to the lack of use of power calculations, inadequate or non-blind randomization and group allocations, and limited consideration of baseline differences between animals. It has been suggested that animal studies follow the established practices of the human clinical trials. These issues have been extensively discussed elsewhere and we refer our readers to several excellent reviews on this topic [137,138,139].

Randomization must always be used to make sure that each animal is allocated to an experimental group strictly by chance. Since MIA models involve two quite different sets of experimental animals, namely, pregnant and nursing dams and their offspring, random group allocation should be done for both groups. While randomly allocating pregnant or to be pregnant females to different experimental conditions, each potential condition (i.e., factor) should be taken into account, including source of animals, mating partners, housing conditions, diets, etc. It is highly desirable that pregnant dams be either single housed after establishing the pregnancy or be randomly allocated to different cages that would include both vehicle-treated and an immune inducer-treated dams. If all dams in a cage receive the same treatment, one may have to consider this cage as the experimental unit. Relatedly, littermates share similar in utero and postnatal environments which can influence offspring physiology and behavior, resulting in litter effects [140]. Therefore, each litter, as opposed to individual offspring, should be considered an experimental unit [141]. A more detailed discussion of this issue has been reviewed elsewhere [142]. Ultimately, a very careful plan should be devised in advance to deal with offspring, including randomization for cross-fostering or testing offspring after birth. As a standardization of all possible experimental approaches involving MIA model is very difficult, investigators are encouraged to provide a detailed description of the randomization and group allocation design (even if a nonstandard design was used) to enable other investigators in the field to reproduce the exact conditions of the published study.

Rearing environment

The prenatal and rearing environment includes the home cage, caging system (see below), as well as the broader environment of the housing room and animal facility. The microbiome of the mother and her offspring undoubtedly can influence behavior, but this is difficult to control. Within the home cage, one of the key influences is the role of the dam. In MIA models, the mother exerts a combination of genetic, environmental, and behavioral influences on the postnatal characteristics of her offspring. These various maternal influences are relevant to the purported downstream effects of prenatal immune stimulation on the offspring’s neurobehavioral characteristics, but they are often difficult to disentangle [81, 82, 123, 143,144,145,146]. One technique used to separate these influences is “cross-fostering”, in which pups are removed from one dam and “transferred” to another lactating dam [147,148,149,150]. In most current behavioral studies, cross-fostering is performed within the same strain, but there is a long history of cross-fostering between strains (and even between species) to examine maternal influences on the offspring’s behavior and physiology [151] (see McCarty for an excellent review [147]). In some transgenic mouse studies, inter-strain cross-fostering is also used as a breeding strategy to develop or maintain a line, or to account for genetic differences in maternal behavior [152].

The advantages of cross-fostering can be considered to partially control for a number of potential confounds in the MIA model such as litter size, sex ratio, lactation, and maternal behavior [147, 153]. However, the exact procedures used for cross-fostering are often missing from publications, and fostering per se can introduce its own set of confounding factors [152, 154, 155]. To control for effects of handling and maternal-pup disruption in cross-fostering experiments, litters may be transferred between mothers within the same treatment groups (e.g., pups from an LPS-treated mother are switched to another LPS-treated mother). As fostering paradigms increase in complexity so does the cost and time commitment for a given scientific question. Despite attempts to control for the plethora of unintended effects in MIA experiments, emerging data continue to yield unexpected and intriguing findings, such as the ability of immune molecules in milk to program hippocampal development and memory in the offspring [156].

Possible influence of caging systems

Another possible confounding factor in MIA models is the precise type of animal caging system. While researchers generally report cage sizes and the number of animals that are housed per cage [123, 124], information regarding other cage parameters is mostly omitted in published articles. A number of rodent studies, however, have provided compelling evidence suggesting that the precise type of animal caging system can affect the animals’ hygiene status, welfare, and behavior [157,158,159,160,161,162], which in turn may significantly influence the outcomes of MIA. In this regard, we have seen a continuous shift from the use of conventional caging systems such as open-top cages (OTC) or filter-top cages (FTC) to individually ventilated cage (IVC) systems [163]. Reducing the potential spreading of airborne infections and protecting the personnel from aeroallergens have been major drivers of the increasing use of IVCs in preclinical rodent research. While the possible benefits of IVCs for laboratory staff seem reasonable, it remains a matter of debate whether preclinical rodent research benefits from IVCs or not [163]. Indeed, IVCs can alter or mask neurobehavioral phenotypes that are typically seen in more conventional caging systems such as OTCs or FTCs. One illustrative example of this are the findings of Logge et al. [164], who showed that schizophrenia-related phenotypes of neuregulin 1 (Nrg1) mutant mice were no longer detectable when the animals were raised in IVCs as compared to FTCs [164].

A wide range of factors may account for the noticeable impact of the caging systems on neurobehavioral phenotypes in preclinical rodent research. For example, IVCs differ from OTCs and FTCs in regard to airflow and intra-cage oxygen (O2), carbon dioxide (CO2), and ammonia (NH3) levels [159, 165], and they also diverge with respect to climbing opportunities [166] and inter-cage olfactory and acoustic cues [167]. IVCs have further been reported to impose cold stress on laboratory mice [168] and to modulate basal immune parameters [159]. All these factors may exert a significant influence on brain development and functions [160, 169, 170], which in turn may modify the expected research outcomes and/or undermine the reproducibility of findings across different laboratories.

Acknowledging (and dealing with) the potential impact of differences in animal caging systems seems particularly relevant for MIA models, for three main reasons: first, the precise type of caging system can affect the animals’ hygiene status, thereby influencing their basal immune profiles and sensitivity to infectious or immune-stimulating agents [159, 171]. Second, offspring of MIA-exposed and control mothers are raised and kept in their home cages for a substantial amount of time, typically ranging from birth to adult stages of life. Hence, the confounding effects induced by cage-specific parameters (e.g., presence or absence of climbing opportunities or inter-cage olfactory and acoustic cues in IVCs; environmental enrichment) can be expected to be maximal in such developmental models [114, 172]. Third, the nature and/or severity of neurobehavioral and molecular changes induced by MIA are known to be dependent on pre-pubertal and post-pubertal maturational processes [78, 124, 173, 174]. External influences brought on by cage-specific factors may modify these maturational processes and may differentially shape the MIA phenotypes as the offspring mature [114, 175].

Thus far, systematic efforts toward investigating the potential impact of differences in laboratory caging systems are lacking in MIA models. Because of the reasons mentioned above, however, we deem such efforts highly warranted. Moreover, we believe it is important to consider and report the precise type of caging system and enrichment devices used in current and future studies of MIA to increase reproducibility across different research laboratories.

III. Outcome measures

Brain and behavior outcome measures

There is increasing evidence that MIA is an environmental risk factor shared across several neurological disorders, such as ASD, schizophrenia, epilepsy, depression, and neurodegenerative diseases [48, 176]. Consistent with this, in animal models, MIA alone or combined with additional environmental or genetic susceptibility, yields offspring that display subsets of various neurochemical, neurophysiological and behavioral abnormalities, as well as immunological, gastrointestinal, or metabolic disruptions [125]. This diversity in phenotypic consequences of MIA requires comprehensive and accurate reporting of experimental methods and results for outcome measures. Relatedly, the consequences of MIA challenges appear to be enduring; disruptions in brain and behavior can be transmitted transgenerationally [81, 82, 92, 177]. Although beyond the scope of these particular reporting guidelines, additional reporting measures will likely need to be established in order to address the implications of these ancestral influences.

The specific experimental methods used to induce MIA should be selected to maximize the physiological relevance of the perturbation and outcomes to the disease or condition of interest. Given that there are many variations of the MIA model that each yield differences in the types, severity and onset of physiological abnormalities seen in MIA offspring, an assessment of evidence supporting face, construct and predictive validity of the specific model of MIA to disease is needed. Consideration should be given to NIMH’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework for investigating mental disorders, which incorporates units of analyses from genes, molecules and cells to circuits, physiology, and behavior. Systems approaches to consider the molecular and cellular bases of neurobehavioral changes are applied to psychological constructs belonging to key behavioral domains, such as emotion, cognition, motivation, and social behavior. Numerous constructs, such as anxiety, sensory perception, cognitive control, and social communication, are relevant to the MIA model and can be used as a framework for outcome measurement and reporting.

Primary and secondary experimental outcomes should be clearly defined. In particular, procedures should indicate at which time point each measurement occurs as well as the age of animals tested. In addition, offspring age should align with the clinical endophenotypes that are being modeled. For example, with relevance to ASD, vocalizations or reciprocal social interactions can be assessed beginning in early postnatal ages, whereas abnormalities in sensorimotor gating seen in schizophrenia may be better examined in adolescent animals and disruptions in learning and memory that are relevant to neurodegenerative disease may be evaluated in adult and aged animals. Consistent with the importance of considering sex as a biological variable [178], whether results for each outcome assessment are sexually dimorphic should be evaluated.

Careful consideration should be given to biological and technical variables that can alter experimental outcomes. Whether each animal undergoes a battery of tests or whether there are separate cohorts that each undergo a subset of tests should be clarified. Where possible, the order of outcome testing should be justified. For example, exposures to physical and psychological stressors can profoundly alter animal behavior [179]. As such, consideration may be given to performing behavioral assessments in order of increasing severity of stress response, to minimize effects of substantial prior exposure to stress on performance in subsequent assays. In addition, whether maternal care is evaluated in the study, or whether maternal care is expected to be altered based on existing literature, should be addressed.

Importantly, statistical methods should be clearly described and justified. In particular, consideration should be given to whether the biological sample size (N) refers to number of mothers (litters) if the primary variable is administered maternally, or if the biological sample size (N) refers to the number of offspring tested (e.g., when testing effects of a postnatal intervention) [180]. The biological sample size of sires should also be considered as paternal effects may also contribute to the offspring phenotype [181]. Overall, dedicated efforts to promote rigorous experimental reporting will enable improved cross-comparisons of different MIA models and assessment of reproducibility across independent studies.

Summary of reporting guidelines and future directions

Translational impact and future directions

MIA studies in pregnant animals indicate that the maternal immune response is one of the critical links between exposure to infection during pregnancy and subsequent changes in brain and behavioral development of offspring. Mice, rat, and NHP offspring born to MIA-treated dams demonstrate significant alterations in brain and behavior development, which parallel features of human neurodevelopmental disorders [34, 35, 38,39,40,41, 176]. The translation of epidemiologic findings to MIA animal models allows for a better understanding of pathogenic mechanisms, reduces the potential for bias and confounding, and allows for cross-validation of the associations by investigating phenotypes observed in schizophrenia and other neuropsychiatric disorders. The use of animal models also allows for study of the interactions of infection with genetic factors as well as with other environmental factors such as diet, alcohol, and drug exposures, which may be important mediators of the neural consequences of maternal infection. Future epidemiologic studies aimed at “back-translating” findings from animal models may facilitate the identification of new risk factors, mechanistic pathways, and interacting genetic mutations. Moreover, further work in both disciplines offers the potential to better understand relationships between MIA and structural and functional brain phenotypes in neuropsychiatric disorders.

The original rationale for using Poly IC in MIA models was based on the assumption that it is not so much the specific viral agent but rather a universal immune response to all types of viruses that really contributes neurodevelopmental abnormalities. Although this appealing hypothesis has never been tested directly, MIA models have significantly advanced our understanding of how Poly IC or LPS produced immune challenge in utero could increase the risk for neurodevelopmental disorders. Since the introduction of the MIA model in early 2000, there has been a significant improvement in our understanding of the mechanisms whereby various microbial infections during pregnancy could contribute to psychopathology in offspring. It is now clear that different microbial pathogens may activate dissimilar immune pathways via activating different TLR receptors. While it is plausible that various microbes could activate a limited number of immune mechanisms, the potential differences in underlying mechanisms could be significant. Thus, an alternative approach is to use inactivated microbes that would produce an agent-specific immune activation by avoiding experimental complications of working with live pathogens. Critically, the latter fact has been historically the main attractive feature of MIA model for many neurobiologists who have limited if any skills and/or lab settings to work with infectious microbes.

Reporting guidelines

Given that preclinical research is under increasing pressure to improve reproducibility [138, 182], the MIA model will undoubtedly benefit from renewed interest in refining experimental design standards [183]. However, MIA models are also faced with a series of unique challenges that has made it increasingly difficult to replicate outcomes, compare across studies, and establish standard protocol guidelines. The need to establish reporting guidelines is of paramount importance for the field and has lead our group to propose the following checklist (Table 1). Modeled after the ARRIVE Guidelines and CONSORT (scientific reporting guidelines for animal and clinical research, respectively), the proposed reporting checklist is a living document tailored for the MIA model, but can easily be adapted and applied to other animal models (e.g. postnatal immune challenge models, early life stress models) as well. At this moment, we are still in the initial stages of identifying key variables central to maintaining the rigor and reproducibility of data that stem from research focused on MIA. While we include specific reporting points, we also have an “other disclosures” section for authors to provide additional information, or communicate other variables, that they deem relevant to the literature. This is a call-to-action requesting that investigators complete the 'fillable' form of Table 1 (see Supplementary Table 2 for downloadable version) and include it as part of the supplementary materials that accompany their manuscripts. Adherence to these reporting guidelines will allow for the future collation and evaluation of reported information, ultimately leading to an established consensus on the best practices to be implemented in our studies. This will also allow for the identification of remaining research gaps, leading to the evolution of evidence-based practice design and the transmission of these findings to the research community. It is our intention to solicit endorsement of this checklist from journals through its implementation as part of the manuscript submission process in order to increase the transparency of data reporting as a standard within the MIA model. With these changes, the MIA model may help us to understand which pregnancies are most vulnerable to prenatal immune challenge, which gestational time points are most sensitive, and ultimately how to safely manage the maternal immune response during pregnancy to prevent deleterious effects on fetal brain development.