Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Frequency-specific hippocampal-prefrontal interactions during associative learning

Abstract

Much of our knowledge of the world depends on learning associations (for example, face-name), for which the hippocampus (HPC) and prefrontal cortex (PFC) are critical. HPC-PFC interactions have rarely been studied in monkeys, whose cognitive and mnemonic abilities are akin to those of humans. We found functional differences and frequency-specific interactions between HPC and PFC of monkeys learning object pair associations, an animal model of human explicit memory. PFC spiking activity reflected learning in parallel with behavioral performance, whereas HPC neurons reflected feedback about whether trial-and-error guesses were correct or incorrect. Theta-band HPC-PFC synchrony was stronger after errors, was driven primarily by PFC to HPC directional influences and decreased with learning. In contrast, alpha/beta-band synchrony was stronger after correct trials, was driven more by HPC and increased with learning. Rapid object associative learning may occur in PFC, whereas HPC may guide neocortical plasticity by signaling success or failure via oscillatory synchrony in different frequency bands.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Paired-associate learning task.
Figure 2: Prefrontal and hippocampal recording locations.
Figure 3: Prefrontal neurons reflect learned associations and hippocampal neurons reflect trial outcome.
Figure 4: Hippocampal-prefrontal oscillatory synchrony carries learning-related information about trial outcome.
Figure 5: Stronger theta PFC→HPC directional influence; stronger HPC→PFC alpha/beta influence.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Scoville, W.B. & Milner, B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 11–21 (1957).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Cohen, N.J. & Squire, L.R. Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210, 207–210 (1980).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Squire, L.R., Stark, C.E.L. & Clark, R.E. The medial temporal lobe. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 279–306 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Gutnikov, S.A., Ma, Y.-Y. & Gaffan, D. Temporo-frontal disconnection impairs visual-visual paired association learning but not configural learning in macaca monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9, 1524–1529 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Farovik, A., Dupont, L.M., Arce, M. & Eichenbaum, H. Medial prefrontal cortex supports recollection, but not familiarity, in the rat. J. Neurosci. 28, 13428–13434 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Sperling, R.A. et al. Encoding novel face-name associations: a functional MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 14, 129–139 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Kim, H. Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: a meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies. Neuroimage 54, 2446–2461 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Jones, M.W. & Wilson, M.A. Theta rhythms coordinate hippocampal–prefrontal interactions in a spatial memory task. PLoS Biol. 3, e402 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Hyman, J.M., Zilli, E.A., Paley, A.M. & Hasselmo, M.E. Medial prefrontal cortex cells show dynamic modulation with the hippocampal theta rhythm dependent on behavior. Hippocampus 15, 739–749 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Siapas, A.G. & Wilson, M.A. Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21, 1123–1128 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Eichenbaum, H., Dudchenko, P., Wood, E., Shapiro, M. & Tanila, H. The hippocampus, memory, and place cells: Is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron 23, 209–226 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Rainer, G., Rao, S.C. & Miller, E.K. Prospective coding for objects in primate prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 19, 5493–5505 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Colgin, L.L., Moser, E.I. & Moser, M.-B. Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping. Trends Neurosci. 31, 469–477 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Schultz, W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1–27 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Matsumoto, M. & Hikosaka, O. Lateral habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neurons. Nature 447, 1111–1115 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Abe, M. et al. Reward improves long-term retention of a motor memory through induction of offline memory gains. Curr. Biol. 21, 557–562 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Bethus, I., Tse, D. & Morris, R.G.M. Dopamine and memory: modulation of the persistence of memory for novel hippocampal NMDA receptor–dependent paired associates. J. Neurosci. 30, 1610–1618 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Wirth, S. et al. Single neurons in the monkey hippocampus and learning of new associations. Science 300, 1578–1581 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bunsey, M. & Eichenbaum, H. Conservation of memory function in rats and humans. Nature 379, 255–257 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Murray, E.A., Gaffan, D. & Mishkin, M. Neural substrates of visual stimulus-stimulus association in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 13, 4549–4561 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Sakai, K. & Miyashita, Y. Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates. Nature 354, 152–155 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Erickson, C.A. & Desimone, R. Responses of macaque perirhinal neurons during and after visual stimulus association learning. J. Neurosci. 19, 10404–10416 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Messinger, A., Squire, L.R., Zola, S.M. & Albright, T.D. Neuronal representations of stimulus associations develop in the temporal lobe during learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 12239–12244 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Eichenbaum, H., Sauvage, M., Fortin, N., Komorowski, R. & Lipton, P. Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1597–1608 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Wirth, S. et al. Trial outcome and associative learning signals in the monkey hippocampus. Neuron 61, 930–940 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Histed, M.H., Pasupathy, A. & Miller, E.K. Learning substrates in the primate prefrontal cortex and striatum: sustained activity related to successful actions. Neuron 63, 244–253 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Jutras, M.J., Fries, P. & Buffalo, E.A. Gamma-band synchronization in the macaque hippocampus and memory formation. J. Neurosci. 29, 12521–12531 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Düzel, E., Penny, W.D. & Burgess, N. Brain oscillations and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 143–149 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Fell, J. & Axmacher, N. The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 105–118 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Fries, P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 474–480 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Buschman, T.J. & Miller, E.K. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Siegel, M., Warden, M.R. & Miller, E.K. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 21341–21346 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Kopell, N., Ermentrout, G.B., Whittington, M.A. & Traub, R.D. Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97, 1867–1872 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Buzsáki, G. & Draguhn, A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Luu, P., Tucker, D.M. & Makeig, S. Frontal midline theta and the error-related negativity: neurophysiological mechanisms of action regulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1821–1835 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Buzsáki, G. & Moser, E.I. Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 130–138 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Engel, A.K. & Fries, P. Beta-band oscillations—signaling the status quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 156–165 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Dudek, S.M. & Bear, M.F. Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89, 4363–4367 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Skaggs, W.E. et al. EEG Sharp waves and sparse ensemble unit activity in the macaque hippocampus. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 898–910 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Nelson, M.J., Pouget, P., Nilsen, E.A., Patten, C.D. & Schall, J.D. Review of signal distortion through metal microelectrode recording circuits and filters. J. Neurosci. Methods 169, 141–157 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Miller, E.K., Gochin, P.M. & Gross, C.G. Habituation-like decrease in the responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the macaque. Vis. Neurosci. 7, 357–362 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Xiang, J.-Z. & Brown, M.W. Neuronal responses related to long-term recognition memory processes in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 42, 817–829 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Yanike, M., Wirth, S., Smith, A.C., Brown, E.N. & Suzuki, W.A. Comparison of associative learning-related signals in the macaque perirhinal cortex and hippocampus. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1064–1078 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Manly, B.F.J. Randomization, Bootstrap, and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology (Chapman & Hall/CRC, 2007).

  45. Torrence, C. & Compo, G. A practical guide to wavelet analysis. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 79, 61–78 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Cui, J., Xu, L., Bressler, S.L., Ding, M. & Liang, H. BSMART: a Matlab/C toolbox for analysis of multichannel neural time series. Neural Netw. 21, 1094–1104 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E. & Schoffelen, J.-M. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011, 1–9 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Bokil, H., Andrews, P., Kulkarni, J.E., Mehta, S. & Mitra, P.P. Chronux: a platform for analyzing neural signals. J. Neurosci. Methods 192, 146–151 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Gallistel, C.R., Fairhurst, S. & Balsam, P. The learning curve: implications of a quantitative analysis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 13124–13131 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Zar, J.H. Biostatistical Analysis (Prentice-Hall/Pearson, 2010).

  51. Olejnik, S. & Algina, J. Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychol. Methods 8, 434–447 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Meyers, E.M. & Kreiman, G. Tutorial on pattern classification in cell recording. in Visual Population Codes (eds. Kriegskorte, N. & Kreiman, G.) 517–538 (MIT Press, 2012).

  53. Kalcher, J. & Pfurtscheller, G. Discrimination between phase-locked and non-phase-locked event-related EEG activity. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 94, 381–384 (1995).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Ding, M., Bressler, S.L., Yang, W. & Liang, H. Short-window spectral analysis of cortical event-related potentials by adaptive multivariate autoregressive modeling: data preprocessing, model validation, and variability assessment. Biol. Cybern. 83, 35–45 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J. & Varela, F.J. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 194–208 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Vinck, M., van Wingerden, M., Womelsdorf, T., Fries, P. & Pennartz, C.M.A. The pairwise phase consistency: A bias-free measure of rhythmic neuronal synchronization. Neuroimage 51, 112–122 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Fisher, N.I. Statistical Analysis of Circular Data (Univ. Press, 1995).

  58. Baccalá, L.A., Sameshima, K. & Takahashi, D.Y. Generalized partial directed coherence. 15th Int. Conf. Digit. Signal Process 163–166 (2007).

  59. Granger, C.W. Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econ. J. Econ. Soc. 37, 424–438 (1969).

    Google Scholar 

  60. Morf, M., Vieira, A., Lee, D.T. & Kailath, T. Recursive multichannel maximum entropy spectral estimation. Geosci. Electron. IEEE Trans. 16, 85–94 (1978).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Geweke, J. Measurement of linear dependence and feedback between multiple time series. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 77, 304–313 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Hess, E.H. & Polt, J.M. Pupil size as related to interest value of visual stimuli. Science 132, 349–350 (1960).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Kennerley, S.W. & Wallis, J.D. Reward-dependent modulation of working memory in lateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 29, 3259–3270 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Nassar, M.R. et al. Rational regulation of learning dynamics by pupil-linked arousal systems. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1040–1046 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Shepherd, S.V., Lanzilotto, M. & Ghazanfar, A.A. Facial muscle coordination in monkeys during rhythmic facial expressions and ingestive movements. J. Neurosci. 32, 6105–6116 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Funahashi, S., Bruce, C.J. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 331–349 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Cromer, J.A., Machon, M. & Miller, E.K. Rapid association learning in the primate prefrontal cortex in the absence of behavioral reversals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1823–1828 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Merletti, R. & Di Torino, P. Standards for reporting EMG data. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 9, 3–4 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  69. Schoffelen, J.-M. Neuronal coherence as a mechanism of effective corticospinal interaction. Science 308, 111–113 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank E. Antzoulatos, A. Bastos, J. Donoghue, N. Kopell, S. Kornblith, R. Loonis, M. Lundqvist, M. Moazami, V. Puig, J. Rose, J. Roy, A. Salazar-Gómez, L. Tran and M. Wilson for helpful comments and suggestions, and D. Altschul, B. Gray, M. Histed, D. Ouellette and the MIT veterinary staff for technical assistance. This work was supported by NIMH Conte Center grant P50-MH094263-03 (E.K.M.), NIMH fellowship F32-MH081507 (S.L.B.), and The Picower Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.L.B. and E.K.M. designed the experiments. S.L.B. trained the monkeys, performed the experiments and analyzed the data. S.L.B. and E.K.M. wrote the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Earl K Miller.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral measures of motor function, motivation, and arousal do not change appreciably with associative learning

In all panels in this figure, behavioral metrics were calculated in identical sliding trial windows (width = 10%, step = 0.5% of each total session length) on the same set of sessions (except for e), are plotted in a similar relative scale (~10× average s.d. across trial windows), and are assayed for changes with learning using the same statistical test (2-sided permutation test on means of early vs. late learning stage [first vs. last third of trials]). (a) Learning performance. Across-session (all sessions meeting learning criteria; n = 61) mean ± s.d. of logit-transformed percent of correct trials, plotted as a function of the percentile of each session’s trials. Performance robustly increases across trials (P ≤ 10−4). This difference is not due to restricting analysis to sessions with successful learning, as it remains significant when all sessions (n = 87) are included (P ≤ 10−4; dashed curves). Note that this is the same data plotted in main text Fig. 1c, but with learning curves pooled (averaged) across all four associations in each session, to match the number of observations for other data in this figure. Also plotted is the across-session mean ± s.d. (“MTS” to right of main plot) performance for an identity match-to-sample control task (i.e., matching an object to itself, rather than to a learned associate), which was significantly better than for the associative learning task (P ≤ 10−4). (b) Reaction time. Across-session (n = 61) mean ± s.d. of log-transformed reaction times to response targets. This metric—which may reflect both motor preparatory and motivational factors—does not change with learning (P = 0.49). Note this null result is likely due in part to the enforced delay in our task between choice object onset and response (cf. Fig. 1b), though the fact that match-to-sample task reaction times are significantly faster (P = 0.008) indicates that reliable reaction time modulations are possible with this task structure. (c) Saccade traces from a typical session. Eye position is plotted for each trial in the early, middle, and late learning stages (top to bottom) to left (green) and right (red) targets, for -130–130 ms relative to saccade onset. Dashed circles indicate fixation and saccade windows. Scale bar indicates 1 degree of visual angle. (d) Saccadic endpoint variability. Across-session (n = 61) mean ± s.d. of the variability of saccade endpoints (across-trial standard deviation of position 30–130 ms post-saccade, when the eyes were typically stable on the target), for vertical (top) and horizontal (bottom) dimensions and saccades to left (green) and right (red) targets. Though saccades do become significantly more variable with learning (all P < 2×10−4), the magnitude of this change is rather small (compare maximum increase of ~0.1 deg. to 1 deg. scale bar in panel c; see also panel g). (e) Pupil size. Across-session (n = 61) mean ± s.d. of pupil diameter during delay period (100–850 ms after start of delay), when pupil size is least influenced by external factors. Within each session, pupil size is expressed as a z-score relative to the fixation period mean and s.d. This metric—which is strongly linked to global arousal—does not significantly change with learning (P = 0.07), but is significantly decreased for the match-to-sample task (P ≤ 10−4). (f) Lip EMG. Across-session mean ± s.d. of lip EMG during outcome feedback period (100–1350 ms after outcome feedback), normalized by its mean value for each session. EMG was obtained from two animals performing a working memory–guided saccade task (purple; 4 sessions) or a visuomotor associative learning task (yellow; 6 sessions). Lip EMG—a proxy for reward-related orofacial movements—also shows little change with learning for either the working memory (P = 0.1) or learning (P = 0.43) tasks. (g) Summary of behavioral results. To compare behavioral changes across all reported metrics, relatively independent of the number of observations, we calculated a d′ statistic between the early and late learning stages: |meanearly–meanlate|/SDpooled. These results reiterate that across-trial changes in motor behavior, motivation, and arousal are relatively minor compared with learning-related changes in performance.

Supplementary Figure 2 Population effects are also observed in single neurons

(a) Rasters showing spike times for an exemplary PFC neuron on trials in which the monkey is cued to recall associate object A1 (top panel, gray) or A2 (bottom, red). Within each panel, learning trials progress from bottom to top. (b) Spike density functions (computed with 75 ms Hann window) summarizing the PFC neuron’s firing rate when recalling associate A1 (gray) or A2 (red) within the early, middle, and late learning stages (light-to-dark colors). Its activity is stronger when associate A2 is recalled, and this preference increases with learning, reflecting population-level PFC signals for learned associations. (c) Rasters showing spike times for an exemplary hippocampal neuron following correct (top panel, green) and incorrect (bottom, brown) trials. Within each panel, learning trials progress from bottom to top. (d) Spike density functions showing the HPC neuron’s firing rate for correct (green) and incorrect (brown) trials within the early, middle, and late learning stages (light-to-dark colors). Its activity is stronger following incorrect trials, but this preference diminishes with learning, reflecting the robust population-level HPC outcome signals and their shift from error-preferring toward correct-preferring bias with learning.

Supplementary Figure 3 Hippocampus and PFC carry neural information about the retrieval cue

(a) Mean percent of variance in PFC (left) and HPC (right) spiking activity explained by retrieval cue, plotted across time after cue onset and learning trials. Activity reflecting the cues is present in both areas, in contrast to activity reflecting the learned associates (Fig. 3 in main text), which is only found in PFC. This indicates the lack of associate signals in HPC is not due to a lack of selective visual responses to the stimuli used.

Supplementary Figure 4 Trial outcome signals in hippocampal subregions

(a) Mean selectivity (PEV) for trial outcome in hippocampal local-projection subregions (top; Dentate gyrus/CA3, n = 104 neurons) and output subregions (bottom; CA1/Subiculum, n = 93) neurons, plotted across learning stages (light-to-dark colors). Outcome signals are significantly stronger overall in the HPC output subregions (P ≤ 10−4, 2-way subregion × learning-stage ANOVA in outcome feedback epoch). (b) Mean bias (signed PEV) in HPC local-projection (top) and output subregion (bottom) neurons for correct (positive values) vs. incorrect (negative values) outcomes. With learning, there was a significant shift from stronger signals for incorrect to correct trials in HPC output subregions (P = 0.005, 2-sided permutation test on ITI epoch signals in early vs. late learning stages), but not local-projection subregions (P = 0.83, P = 0.04, interaction in 2-way subregion × learning-stage ANOVA).

Supplementary Figure 5 HPC-PFC synchrony across all trial periods

(a) Mean synchrony (PLV) between HPC and PFC LFPs on correct trials, plotted as a spectrogram across time and frequency. Separate plots show time periods during the trial (left; time referenced to retrieval cue onset), and after outcome feedback is given (right; referenced to outcome feedback onset). (b) Mean HPC-PFC synchrony on incorrect trials (same conventions and color scale as a). (c) Mean z-scored difference in HPC-PFC synchrony between correct and incorrect trials, for same time periods as above (right: replot of main text Fig. 4b). Though there are clear periods of band-specific synchrony during trial performance (panels a and b), they are nearly identical for correct and incorrect trials, and thus convey little information about trial outcome.

Supplementary Figure 6 Learning-related synchrony in hippocampal subregions

(a) Mean z-scored difference in synchrony (dPLV) between correct and incorrect trials, calculated between electrodes in PFC and distinct hippocampal subregions (left: PFC & HPC local-projection subregions [dentate gyrus and CA3], n = 558 electrode pairs; right: PFC & HPC output subregions [CA1 and subiculum], n = 407), plotted across learning stages. (b) Summary of synchrony learning effects—mean (± s.e.m.) dPLV pooled within the alpha/beta-band (top) and theta-band (bottom) regions of interest, as a function of learning stage. While the theta-band decrease with learning is similar for synchrony between PFC and all HPC subregions (P ≤ 10−4 for both, 2-sided permutation test on early vs. late learning), the alpha/beta-band increase with learning is only present for synchrony between PFC and HPC output subregions (CA1/Sub.; P ≤ 10−4), but not for synchrony between PFC and HPC local-projection subregions (dentate/CA3; P = 0.52) despite their greater numbers of observations. Synchrony between hippocampal subregions (not shown) is nearly identical to synchrony averaged across all pairs of hippocampal electrodes (Supplementary Fig. 7); small numbers of observations precluded meaningful analysis of synchrony between sites within each subregion.

Supplementary Figure 7 Learning-related information about trial outcome in oscillatory synchrony between all area pairs

(a) Mean synchrony (PLV) spectrograms between pairs of electrodes in PFC (left; n = 648), in HPC (right; n = 694), and between HPC and PFC (center; n = 970), following correct (top) and incorrect (bottom) trials. (b) Mean z-scored difference in synchrony (dPLV) between correct and incorrect trials, plotted across learning stages, for all pairs of studied areas (HPC-PFC data replotted from main text Fig. 4c). (c) Summary of synchrony learning effects—mean (± s.e.m.) synchrony difference pooled within the alpha/beta-band (top) and theta-band (bottom) regions of interest, as a function of learning stage. Synchrony between distinct sites within PFC (red) follows a similar pattern to the cross-area synchrony (purple)—theta decreases (P = 3×10−4), while alpha/beta increases with learning (P ≤ 10−4, 2-sided permutation test on early vs. late learning). In contrast, intra-hippocampal synchrony increases with learning for both the theta (P = 2×10−4) and alpha/beta bands (P ≤ 10−4), indicating the observed learning effects do not reflect global state changes that are invariant across all brain areas.

Supplementary Figure 8 Learning-related information about trial outcome in oscillatory power in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus

(a) Mean raw LFP signals (evoked potentials) in PFC (red; n = 250 electrodes) and HPC (blue; n = 166), following correct (top) and incorrect (bottom) trials. LFPs are time-locked to the onset of trial-outcome feedback (solid line; dashed line indicates behavioral response time). (b) Mean normalized LFP power spectrograms in PFC (left) and HPC (right), following correct (top) and incorrect (bottom) trials. To highlight components not time-locked to trial events (i.e., induced, rather than evoked, signals), mean raw LFPs were subtracted off each electrode prior to calculation of spectral power. To enhance visualization of band-specific signals relative to the well-known 1/frequency distribution of LFP power, power at each frequency was normalized by 1/frequency for display purposes only. (c) Mean z-scored difference in induced power between correct and incorrect trials across learning stages, for PFC (left) and HPC (right). While there is a strong alpha/beta-band signal for correct trials, the theta-band signal for incorrect trials observed in the cross-electrode synchrony results is not as robust in local power. (d) Summary of power learning effects—mean (± s.e.m.) induced power difference pooled within the alpha/beta-band (top) and theta-band (bottom) regions of interest, as a function of learning stage. Theta power exhibits a significant positive shift (from incorrect toward correct bias) with learning (P ≤ 10−4 for both areas), and alpha/beta power also shows a positive trend (significant only for HPC: P ≤ 10−4; PFC: P = 0.06, 2-sided permutation test on early vs. late learning). These results indicate a similar change with learning for both cross-area synchrony and within-area power.

Supplementary Figure 9 Balancing band-limited power does not eliminate observed HPC-PFC synchrony and causality effects

(a) Mean z-scored difference in HPC-PFC synchrony (dPLV) between correct and incorrect trials for the full dataset (left), and for data subsets where power pooled within the theta-band (middle) or alpha/beta-band (right) regions of interest was balanced across trial outcomes. Though power balancing reduced the effect of trial outcome on neural synchrony, both frequency bands remained significantly different from zero (P ≤ 10−4 for both, 1-sample bootstrap test). This confirms there is a specific effect of outcome on HPC-PFC synchrony, beyond any possible artifactual effects due to differences in power. (b) Frequency-domain directional influences (GPDC) from PFC to HPC (left) and from HPC to PFC (right), following correct (top) and incorrect (bottom) trials for full dataset. (c) GPDC for data subsets where power pooled within the theta-band (bottom) or alpha/beta-band (top) regions of interest was balanced across trial outcomes. For the power-balanced controls, alpha/beta-band influences remain stronger from HPC to PFC, and theta-band influences remain stronger from PFC to HPC (P ≤ 10−4 for both; direction factor in 2-way causal direction × trial outcome permutation ANOVA). This confirms that the observed directionality effects are not due to any differences in local power within these areas.

Supplementary Figure 10 PFC and HPC neuronal trial outcome signals are distinct from subcortical reward prediction error signals

(a) Roughly equal numbers of neurons show stronger activity for correct and incorrect outcomes. Plots show the percent of neurons in PFC (red; n = 319) and HPC (blue; n = 199) with a significant preference for correct (Cor) or incorrect (Inc) outcomes (P < 0.05, 2-sided permutation test). Values are plotted separately for early, mid, and late learning stages (bottom to top), and time epochs capturing transient (left; 100–500 ms) and sustained (right; 600–1350 ms) response components. These results are in contrast to previous results from the ventral tegmental area and lateral habenula, which show strong biases toward positive and negative reward prediction errors (roughly, uncertain correct and incorrect outcomes), respectively. (b) No robust transfer of outcome signals to earlier trial events. Population mean percent variance explained by trial outcome (correct vs. incorrect) is plotted as a function of time during (left) and after (right) the trial, separately for PFC and HPC and learning stages (see legend). Tick marks at bottom indicate time points with significant explained variance during the late learning stage (P < 0.05, uncorrected, 1-sample bootstrap test). In the ventral tegmental area and lateral habenula, as reward becomes more predictable during learning, activation shifts from the post-response outcome feedback epoch to earlier trial events predictive of reward. In contrast, post-response trial outcome information in PFC and HPC is present throughout learning, with little shift to earlier time points. These properties, and the learning-related shift from bias toward encoding incorrect to correct outcomes in HPC (main text Fig. 3c), distinguish the outcome signals we report from the static reward prediction error signals found in areas such as the ventral tegmental area and lateral habenula.

Supplementary Figure 11 Results are similar for neural selectivity measured using mutual information, area under ROC curve, and percent explained variance

(a) Mean mutual information between spike counts and trial outcome (correct vs. incorrect) in PFC (top; n = 319) and HPC (bottom; n = 199) neurons, bias-corrected by subtracting trial-shuffled information. (b) Mean area under receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve for discrimination of spike counts between correct and incorrect trial outcomes in PFC (top) and HPC (bottom) neurons, rectified around 0.5 and bias-corrected by subtracting trial-shuffled area-under-ROC values. Format for both plots is the same as for plots of neural percent explained variance in main text Fig. 3b. All three metrics show highly similar results—consistently showing greater selectivity in HPC than PFC—as we have observed for numerous neural activity contrasts.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brincat, S., Miller, E. Frequency-specific hippocampal-prefrontal interactions during associative learning. Nat Neurosci 18, 576–581 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3954

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3954

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing