Trends in Neurosciences
Volume 37, Issue 8, August 2014, Pages 443-454
Journal home page for Trends in Neurosciences

Review
From molecule to mind: an integrative perspective on odor intensity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.05.005Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Olfaction lacks a working model of odor-intensity coding.

  • Neural correlates of varying odor concentration lead to simplistic models.

  • New models are needed that predict intensity across odors, in mixtures, and after adaptation.

A fundamental problem in systems neuroscience is mapping the physical properties of a stimulus to perceptual characteristics. In vision, wavelength translates into color; in audition, frequency translates into pitch. Although odorant concentration is a key feature of olfactory stimuli, we do not know how concentration is translated into perceived intensity by the olfactory system. A variety of neural responses at several levels of processing have been reported to vary with odorant concentration, suggesting specific coding models. However, it remains unclear which, if any, of these phenomena underlie the perception of odor intensity. Here, we provide an overview of current models at different stages of olfactory processing, and identify promising avenues for future research.

Section snippets

Deciphering the neural code for odor intensity

‘I want to create a cologne that smells like a whisper. It’ll be for all the secret admirers out there.’ Jarod Kintz

Some odors whisper like a secret admirer, others blare like a megaphone. How does the brain encode such a broad spectrum of olfactory intensities? Most research in the field has focused on concentration, rather than intensity. Whereas the intensity of an odorant is clearly related to its concentration [1], odorant concentration also correlates with odor valence [2] and quality [3]

Intensity–concentration relation

Empirically, the perceived intensity of an odorant is a monotonic, sigmoidal function of the logarithm of odorant concentration, although most early observations captured only the linear portion over a middle range of concentrations. In fact, many odorants fail to attain sufficiently high vapor concentrations to saturate the psychophysical function [7], so linear [8] and exponential [9] functions can indeed approximate the intensity–concentration function. However, a comparison of models found

Predicting odor intensity from chemical structure

There is currently no general model predicting the psychophysical intensity function from chemical structure. Certain physicochemical properties of molecules tend to be associated with high-potency odorants, such as high volatility and intermediate water-lipid partitioning, but odor intensity still spans several orders of magnitude for compounds with similar volatility and hydrophobicity.

General models have emerged for a special case of the psychophysical function relating concentration to

Neural encoding: predicting odor intensity from spike patterns

Chemical signals detected by olfactory receptors (OR) in OSNs are transformed into sequences of action potentials or spikes relayed to the brain. Spikes are digital signals whose number, timing, and distribution across neural assemblies contain all of the stimulus information available to the nervous system. A variety of schemes have been proposed for spike encoding of odor intensity at different stages in the olfactory pathway, based on observing changes in neural activity as a function of

Odor intensity from cortical activation

To date, few studies have directly assessed the central neural network responsible for the processing of perceived odor intensity or odor concentration. A major reason for this dearth of knowledge is the aforementioned inherently confounding co-dependency between the three perceptual odor dimensions: intensity, valence, and quality. This interdependence among dimensions renders the task of isolating cognitive processing of intensity perception inherently difficult. Nonetheless, increased

How quickly is intensity computed?

In land-based vertebrates, olfactory perception begins with the inhalation (sniffing) of odorous air into the nasal cavity. A sniff begins with a stimulus-independent inhalation, but is rapidly modulated in response to sniff content. Sniffs of different odorant concentrations are uniform for the first 160–260 ms following sniff onset, but then diverge such that sniffs of a concentrated odorant will have a smaller volume than sniffs of a diluted odorant [73]. This relation is sufficiently robust

Concluding remarks

Most olfactory stimuli encountered in nature are complex mixtures of odorant molecules. When the intensities of the component odorants are known, what is the intensity of the mixture? The intensity of an odorant mixture is rarely a simple linear sum of the intensities of individual components [108], but as noted above, nonlinear models, such as the Hill equation, are often a better fit to the concentration–intensity relation. Thus, mixture interactions that deviate from a linear sum of

Acknowledgments

J.D.M. is supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant DC013339. J.N.L. is supported by the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation (KAW 2012.0141). J.R. is supported by NIH grant DC009613. G.L. is supported by NIH grant DC04208.

Glossary

Amodal neural network
a network that is not primarily dedicated to the processing of information from a specific sensory modality, such as vision, audition, or olfaction.
GCaMP2 imaging
GCaMP2 is a genetically encoded reporter protein whose fluorescence increases as intracellular calcium levels rise when neurons fire action potentials. In the olfactory system, mouse strains expressing GCaMP2 in OSNs, and in mitral/tufted cells, have been used to map patterns of glomerular activity encoding

References (118)

  • I. Fukunaga

    Two distinct channels of olfactory bulb output

    Neuron

    (2012)
  • T.A. Schoenfeld et al.

    The anatomical logic of smell

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • Z. Zhou et al.

    Coding odorant concentration through activation timing between the medial and lateral olfactory bulb

    Cell Rep.

    (2012)
  • T. Bozza

    In vivo imaging of neuronal activity by targeted expression of a genetically encoded probe in the mouse

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • B.D. Rubin et al.

    Optical imaging of odorant representations in the mammalian olfactory bulb

    Neuron

    (1999)
  • H. Spors et al.

    Spatio-temporal dynamics of odor representations in the mammalian olfactory bulb

    Neuron

    (2002)
  • S.H. Fuss

    Odorant receptor gene choice and axonal wiring in mice with deletion mutations in the odorant receptor gene SR1

    Mol. Cell. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • D.D. Stettler et al.

    Representations of odor in the piriform cortex

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • I.G. Davison et al.

    Neural circuit mechanisms for pattern detection and feature combination in olfactory cortex

    Neuron

    (2011)
  • T. Sugai

    Odor-concentration coding in the guinea-pig piriform cortex

    Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • T. Tateyama

    Relation of olfactory event-related potentials to changes in stimulus concentration

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1998)
  • M. Bensafi

    Neural coding of stimulus concentration in the human olfactory and intranasal trigeminal systems

    Neuroscience

    (2008)
  • N.E. Bowman

    Temporal integration of olfactory perceptual evidence in human orbitofrontal cortex

    Neuron

    (2012)
  • J.A. Gottfried

    Remembrance of odors past: human olfactory cortex in cross-modal recognition memory

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • R.A. Frank

    Assessment of the Sniff Magnitude Test as a clinical test of olfactory function

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2003)
  • H.L. Batsel et al.

    Bulbar respiratory neurons participating in the sniff reflex in the cat

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1973)
  • M. Chastrette

    Modelling the human olfactory stimulus-response function

    Chem. Senses

    (1998)
  • K.E. Henion

    Odor pleasantness and intensity: a single dimension?

    J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1971)
  • R. Gross-Isseroff et al.

    Concentration-dependent changes of perceived odor quality

    Chem. Senses

    (1988)
  • H. Eichenbaum

    Selective olfactory deficits in case H.M

    Brain

    (1983)
  • H.R. Moskowitz

    Odor intensity and pleasantness for a diverse set of odorants

    Percept. Psychophysiol.

    (1976)
  • G.T. Fechner

    Elemente der Psychophysik

    (1860)
  • S.S. Stevens

    On the psychophysical law

    Psychol. Rev.

    (1957)
  • V. Audouin

    Limitations in the use of odor activity values to determine important odorants in foods

  • M.H. Abraham

    An algorithm for 353 odor detection thresholds in humans

    Chem. Senses

    (2012)
  • K.M. Hau et al.

    Quantitative structure-activity relationships (QSARs) for odor thresholds of volatile organic compounds (VOCs)

    Indoor Air

    (1998)
  • A. Keller

    Genetic variation in a human odorant receptor alters odour perception

    Nature

    (2007)
  • W.S. Cain

    Odor intensity: differences in the exponent of the psychophysical function

    Percept. Psychophys.

    (1969)
  • R.A. Maue et al.

    Patch-clamp studies of isolated mouse olfactory receptor neurons

    J. Gen. Physiol.

    (1987)
  • J. Reisert et al.

    Adaptation of the odour-induced response in frog olfactory receptor cells

    J. Physiol.

    (1999)
  • R.J. O’Connell et al.

    Quantitative stimulation of frog olfactory receptors

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1969)
  • J. Reisert

    Origin of basal activity in mammalian olfactory receptor neurons

    J. Gen. Physiol.

    (2010)
  • P. Duchamp-Viret

    Peripheral odor coding in the rat and frog: quality and intensity specification

    J. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • A.S. Ghatpande et al.

    Olfactory receptor neuron responses coding for rapid odour sampling

    J. Physiol.

    (2011)
  • K. Nara

    A large-scale analysis of odor coding in the olfactory epithelium

    J. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • H. Spors

    Temporal dynamics and latency patterns of receptor neuron input to the olfactory bulb

    J. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • T. Bozza

    Odorant receptor expression defines functional units in the mouse olfactory system

    J. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • X. Grosmaitre

    Odorant responses of olfactory sensory neurons expressing the odorant receptor MOR23: a patch clamp analysis in gene-targeted mice

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2006)
  • M. Smear

    Multiple perceptible signals from a single olfactory glomerulus

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • D.G. Laing

    Relationship between molecular structure, concentration and odor qualities of oxygenated aliphatic molecules

    Chem. Senses

    (2003)
  • Cited by (66)

    • Olfaction in the context of obesity and diabetes: Insights from animal models to humans

      2022, Neuropharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      also observed that many patients developed aversions to sweet foods and meats and that food preferences changed right after surgery (in 68% of 82 RYGB patients). Sensory aversion is a complex emotional reaction implicating at least the amygdala and the insular cortex (Miranda, 2012; Münzberg et al., 2015), two brain areas that receive massive olfactory information (Mainland et al., 2014). Extended effects of BS on the overall sensory-limbic networks should be studied by brain imaging techniques to give access to the brain activity underlying changes in olfactory aversion after surgery.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text