Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 55, Issue 4, 15 April 2011, Pages 1548-1565
NeuroImage

An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.055Get rights and content

Abstract

Phase-synchronization is a manifestation of interaction between neuronal groups measurable from LFP, EEG or MEG signals, however, volume conduction can cause the coherence and the phase locking value to spuriously increase. It has been shown that the imaginary component of the coherency (ImC) cannot be spuriously increased by volume-conduction of independent sources. Recently, it was proposed that the phase lag index (PLI), which estimates to what extent the phase leads and lags between signals from two sensors are nonequiprobable, improves on the ImC. Compared to ImC, PLI has the advantage of being less influenced by phase delays. However, sensitivity to volume-conduction and noise, and capacity to detect changes in phase-synchronization, is hindered by the discontinuity of the PLI, as small perturbations turn phase lags into leads and vice versa. To solve this problem, we introduce a related index, namely the weighted phase lag index (WPLI). Differently from PLI, in WPLI the contribution of the observed phase leads and lags is weighted by the magnitude of the imaginary component of the cross-spectrum. We demonstrate two advantages of the WPLI over the PLI, in terms of reduced sensitivity to additional, uncorrelated noise sources and increased statistical power to detect changes in phase-synchronization. Another factor that can affect phase-synchronization indices is sample-size bias. We show that, when directly estimated, both PLI and the magnitude of the ImC have typically positively biased estimators. To solve this problem, we develop an unbiased estimator of the squared PLI, and a debiased estimator of the squared WPLI.

Research highlights

►New measure of phase‐synchronization, the Weighted Phase-Lag-Index. ►Reduced sensitivity to addition of uncorrelated sources. ►Increased sensitivity to detect differences in phase‐synchronization. ►New, unbiased estimator procedure for Phase-Lag‐Index. ►Debiased estimator for Weighted Phase‐Lag‐Index.

Introduction

Oscillatory neuronal activity has been implied in numerous functions (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004, Fries, 2009, Gray et al., 1989, Pesaran et al., 2002, Salinas and Sejnowski, 2001), such as attention, spatial navigation, perceptual binding and memory. Oscillatory activity in different areas can be phase-coupled, i.e., display systematic phase-delays, a phenomenon called phase-synchronization, which has been hypothesized to be an important mechanism for creating a flexible communication structure between brain areas (Engel et al., 2001, Fries, 2005, Varela et al., 2001). In support of this hypothesis, correlations between cognitive functions and long-range phase-synchronization have been demonstrated in many different areas and species, e.g. (Benchenane et al., 2010, Buschman and Miller, 2009, Gregoriou et al., 2009, Pesaran et al., 2008, Roelfsema et al., 1997, Siapas et al., 2005, von Stein et al., 2000, Womelsdorf et al., 2007).

Traditionally, spectral coherence has been used to quantify phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data (EEG and MEG) (Adey et al., 1961, Mitra and Pesaran, 1999, Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006, Walter, 1963). Since coherence merely indicates linear correlation between signals, intermingling phase and amplitude correlations, Lachaux et al. (1999) proposed to use only the relative phase between signals to index phase-synchronization, resulting in an index called the phase locking value (PLV).

However, it is well-known that indexing phase-synchronization can be complicated by four problems: (i) the presence of a common reference, (ii) volume-conduction of source activity, (iii) the presence of noise sources, and (iv) sample-size bias. Volume-conduction of source activity, and, in case of EEG (but not MEG) data, the use of a common reference, can spuriously inflate phase-synchronization indices (Fein et al., 1988, Nolte et al., 2004, Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006, Stam et al., 2007). The problem of volume-conduction is especially large for scalp EEG and MEG data, because of their low spatial resolution. However, it can still be significant when the EEG is measured intracranially (ECoG) or from electrode tips within the tissue (Local Field Potentials — LFPs) if a common reference is used and/or the cross-spectrum is defined over data from spatially close sensors.

To overcome these problems, Nolte et al. (2004) proposed the imaginary component of the coherency (ImC) as a conservative index of phase-synchronization, and showed that volume conduction of uncorrelated sources cannot ‘create’ a non-zero ImC, based on the conventional assumption that, for typical EEG and MEG frequencies of interest, the quasi-stationary description of the Maxwell equations holds (Maxwell, 1865, Plonsey and Heppner, 1967, Stinstra and Peters, 1998), implying that the conducted electric activity of a single source affects spatially separate sensors with negligible time delay.

Stam et al. (2007) argued that one disadvantage of the ImC lies in the fact that it can be strongly influenced by the phase of the coherency, so that it is most effective in detecting synchronization with a phase lag corresponding to a quarter cycle, and breaks down if the two sources of interest are in phase or in phase opposition. As a potential improvement on the ImC, Stam et al. (2007) therefore proposed the phase lag index (PLI). The PLI estimates, for a particular frequency, to what extent the phase leads and lags between signals from two sensors are nonequiprobable, irrespective of the magnitude of the phase leads and lags. In simulations, the PLI performed better than the ImC in detecting true changes in phase-synchronization, and was less sensitive to the addition of volume-conducted noise sources (Stam et al., 2007). Still, PLI's sensitivity to noise and volume conduction may be hindered by the discontinuity in this index as small perturbations turn phase lags into leads and vice versa, a problem that becomes more serious for synchronization effects of small magnitude.

To increase the capacity to detect true changes in phase-synchronization, to reduce the influence of common noise sources (and for EEG data, a common reference) and to reduce the influence of changes in the phase of the coherency, we add a new member to the family of phase-synchronization indices that are based on the imaginary component of the cross-spectrum, namely the weighted phase lag index (WPLI). The WPLI extends the PLI in that it weights the contribution of observed phase leads and lags by the magnitude of the imaginary component of the cross-spectrum; in this way it alleviates the discontinuity mentioned above. We will demonstrate two main advantages of the WPLI over the PLI, in terms of their sensitivity to additional, uncorrelated noise sources and their capacity to detect true changes in phase-synchronization.

To bridge the analysis in terms of population parameters to the practical case of data obtained under limited sampling, we will address a final confounding factor that complicates the estimation of phase-synchronization, namely sample-size bias, usually increasing with smaller sample sizes. We will show that the direct PLI estimator (Stam et al., 2007) is positively biased, and that the direct estimator of the ImC's magnitude is typically positively biased, but can be negatively biased as well. To solve this problem, we will introduce an unbiased estimator of the squared PLI and a debiased estimator of the squared WPLI.

The paper is organized as follows. We commence by introducing a conventional linear mixture model (The linear mixture model section), and existing indices of phase-synchronization (Existing indices of phase-synchorinization section). This sets the stage for the definition of the WPLI (The weighted phase-lag index section). The comparison of the WPLI with previous statistics will proceed as follows. First, for two correlated sources of interest, we transition from the case of ideal sensors (i.e., neglecting the reference) without volume-conduction to the more realistic case of ideal sensors where the two sources of interest are volume-conducted (Volume-conducting correlated sources of interest section). Second, we continue with a more realistic case where uncorrelated, volume-conducted noise sources have been added (Addition of uncorrelated, volume-conducted noise reduction section). Third, we study the effect of changes in the coherency between the activities of two sources of interest (Detecting changes in the coherency between sources' activities section). After this comparison, we continue with the practical case where population parameters have to be estimated from small sample sizes, (The problem of sample-size bias section), and finish our theoretical section by comparing the statistical powers of the WPLI and PLI estimators (Comparison between the statistical power of the squared PLI and WPLI estimators section). Finally, we apply the proposed techniques to actual LFP data recorded from the rat orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (Application to experimental LFP data: methods, Application to experimental LFP data: results sections).

Section snippets

The linear mixture model

Suppose we observe real-valued signals from two recording sensors, for N trials, and T samples per trial. For every j-th trial (j = 1, …, N), we receive a 2 × T data matrix Sj where each row of Sj corresponds to data from one of the two sensors. We model the observed data as a linear mixture of K source activities, represented by the K × T matrix Vj. Without loss of generality, we model the observed data in the frequency domain, by using the linearity of the Discrete Fourier Transform (DFT), aszj(f)=Ayj

Existing indices of phase-synchronization

Before introducing the WPLI and conducting an in-depth comparison of the various statistics, we first provide definitions of conventional indices of phase-synchronization in terms of population parameters (for estimation problems, see The problem of sample-size bias section).

The complex-valued coherency is defined asCE{X}E{M12}E{M22},where M1  |Z1|, and M2  |Z2|. The coherence is defined as |C|, and is always less than or equal to 1.

The phase-locking value (PLV) Lachaux et al. (1999) defined it

The weighted phase-lag index

As will be argued in more detail below, PLI's sensitivity to noise and volume conduction is hindered by the discontinuity in this measure as small perturbations turn phase lags into leads and vice versa, a problem that becomes more serious for synchronization effects of small magnitude (Addition of uncorrelated, volume-conducted noise sources section). Further, improvement can be made in detecting changes in phase-synchronization (Detecting changes in the coherency between sources' activities

Volume-conducting correlated sources of interest

We start by considering the case where we have no noise sources added and two ideal sensors, i.e., where we have a neutral reference (as would be ideal for LFP/EEG) or no reference (as is the case for MEG); the latter reduction is justified because the problem caused by a common reference is identical to the problem of adding a volume-conducted noise source, since referencing can be described as a linear superposition of the potential of the channel of interest with the negative potential of

Addition of uncorrelated, volume-conducted noise sources

Suppose that, in addition to our two sources of interest, we start adding L  2 noise sources (Y3, …,YL) that are (i) uncorrelated to each other and (ii) uncorrelated to the two sources of interest Y1 and Y2.

For EEG and LFP recordings, a common reference can be thought of as transitioning from the case of an ideal sensor (with electro-neutral or no reference, as with MEG) to adding a single source to the linear mixture with coefficients (aref, aref). As far as uncorrelated noise sources whose

Detecting changes in the coherency between sources' activities

Having investigated the effect of adding noise and volume-conduction, we will now investigate to what extent the descriptive statistics are affected by changes in (i) the phase and (ii) the magnitude of the coherency (or, the PLV (7)). The influence of components (i) and (ii), which are the typical parameters of interest in neuroscience experiments, are treated separately in this section.

The problem of sample-size bias

We will now study another factor that complicates the use of phase-synchronization indices, namely sample-size bias, which is a serious problem when, in practice, the population parameters have to be estimated from only a relatively small sample of trials.

Comparison between the statistical power of the squared PLI and WPLI estimators

To further characterize the sampling distribution of the unbiased PLI-square estimator and the debiased WPLI-square estimator, we determined the ratio of the sample mean over the sample standard deviation, i.e., the standardized mean. The relative phase was von Mises distributed. Fig. 10 shows the standardized mean as a function of sample size, for different values of the phase consistency (colors) and the mean phase (subpanels A and B), separate for the PLI (dotted) and WPLI (solid).As

Application to experimental LFP data: methods

To demonstrate the usefulness of the developed phase-synchronization indices, we applied them to actual neuronal data. Methods on the behavioral paradigm and recordings techniques have been described in detail in (van Wingerden et al., 2010a, van Wingerden et al., 2010b, Vinck et al., 2010b). We recorded LFP activity from the rat orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) by using closely separated (< 1 mm, smallest horizontal separation between electrodes < 200 μm, although the exact distance for a specific

Application to experimental LFP data: results

As described in van Wingerden et al. (2010b), LFP ‘theta’-band (here about 6 Hz) power in OFC was higher during the correct go waiting (for sucrose) period than during the incorrect go waiting (for quinine) period.

However, the latter study did not investigate whether (i) the activities of spatially separate OFC populations become more theta-band coherent during the correct go waiting (for sucrose) period than during the incorrect go waiting (for quinine) period and the movement period, and (ii)

Discussion

We introduced the WPLI, a novel measure of phase-synchronization. Similar to the PLI (Stam et al., 2007), the WPLI estimates to what extent the phase leads and lags between signals from two sensors are nonequiprobable. In contrast with the PLI, the WPLI weighs more contribution of observations based on the magnitude of the imaginary component. WPLI is also tightly related to the ImC, in that they only differ for the normalization. The main advantage of the WPLI above ‘volume-conduction

Acknowledgments

MV was supported by European Union Grant FP7 #217148. RO gratefully acknowledges the support of the BrainGain Smart Mix Programme of the Netherlands Ministry of Economic Affairs and the Netherlands Ministry of Education, Culture and Science.

References (40)

  • A. Sirota et al.

    Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm

    Neuron

    (2008)
  • M. Vinck et al.

    The pairwise phase consistency: a bias-free measure of rhythmic neuronal synchronization

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • D. Walter

    Spectral analysis for electroencephalograms: mathematical determination of neurophysiological relationships from records of limited duration

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1963)
  • V. Benignus

    Estimation of the coherence spectrum and its confidence interval using the fast fourier transform

    I. E. E. E. Trans. Audio Electroacoust.

    (1969)
  • D. Best et al.

    Efficient simulation of the von mises distribution

    J. R. Stat. Soc. C Appl. Stat.

    (1979)
  • G. Buzsáki et al.

    Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks

    Science

    (2004)
  • A.K. Engel et al.

    Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2001)
  • N. Fisher

    Statistical Analysis of Circular Data

    (1993)
  • P. Fries

    Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • C.M. Gray et al.

    Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties

    Nature

    (1989)
  • Cited by (998)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text