Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 50, Issue 4, 1 May 2010, Pages 1599-1605
NeuroImage

Conflict processing in the anterior cingulate cortex constrains response priming

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.095Get rights and content

Abstract

A prominent function of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is to process conflict between competing response options. In this study, we investigated the role of conflict processing in a response-priming task in which manual responses were either validly or invalidly cued. Examining electrophysiological measurements of oscillatory brain activity on the source level, we found response priming to be related to a beta power decrease in the premotor cortex and conflict processing to be linked to a theta power increase in the ACC. In particular, correlation of oscillatory brain activities in the ACC and the premotor cortex showed that conflict processing reduces response priming by slowing response time in valid trials and lowering response errors in invalid trials. This relationship emerged on a between subjects level as well as within subjects, on a single trial level. These findings suggest that conflict processing in the ACC constrains the automatic priming process.

Introduction

The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) plays an important role in the adjustment of cognitive control in conflict resolution between relevant and irrelevant information (for a review, see Mansouri et al., 2009). Such cognitive control is needed when goal-irrelevant automatic or prepotent information interferes with goal-relevant information in the selection of the appropriate behavior. For example, the automatic response to extinguish fire is to pour water on the fire source. However, this automatic response is best overridden by throwing table salt, baking soda, or a damp towel on the fire source in the case of an oil fire in the kitchen.

Two current theories of how the ACC processes conflict are the conflict-monitoring theory and the regulatory theory of ACC function. The former theory asserts that the ACC detects and signals conflict to other areas, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which then implement cognitive control (Botvinick et al., 2004, Carter and van Veen, 2007). The conflict-monitoring theory assumes a monitoring role for the ACC in a broad range of situations in which conflict might occur from early perceptual to late motor processing. On the other hand, the latter theory asserts that the ACC exerts cognitive control itself and, thus, has a more direct response-regulatory function in resolving conflict (Paus et al., 1998, Posner and DiGirolamo, 1998). The regulatory theory is primarily based on evidence that indicates an important role for the ACC in response selection at the late motor stage. Actually, although the ACC has been shown to signal conflict already from the early perceptual stage of information processing, it is most strongly responsive to conflict at the late motor stage (Milham et al., 2001, van Veen et al., 2001) and post-response conflict due to response errors (Braver et al., 2001, Carter et al., 1998). Consistently, the ACC with its strong connectivity to motor areas has been suggested to be in part a motor structure (Matsumoto et al., 2003, Paus, 2001).

Examining the effects of response conflict and error processing on event-related brain potentials (ERPs), ACC activity has been found to be related to the frontocentral N2 component and error-related negativity (ERN) (Debener et al., 2005, van Veen and Carter, 2002). In addition, induced frontocentral ACC theta oscillations have been shown to indicate error detection (Luu et al., 2004, Marco-Pallarés et al., 2008, Trujillo and Allen, 2007) and to signal conflict of prepotent responses in the classical Stroop task (Hanslmayr et al., 2008) and the Eriksen flanker task (Cavanagh et al., 2009). Moreover, during conflict in prepotent responding, a cross-talk between the ACC and the DLPFC as reflected by long-range theta phase coupling between these brain areas has been found suggesting that when conflict of a prepotent response arises the ACC engages the DLPFC to implement control (Cavanagh et al., 2009, Hanslmayr et al., 2008). In the present response-priming study, we sought to examine the role of theta oscillations in the selection of validly and invalidly primed motor responses.

In response priming, response processing is faster when a target to respond with one hand is shortly preceded by a valid cue that primes the same-hand response and more error-prone when it is preceded by an invalid cue that primes the other-hand response (Sterr and Dean, 2008). Response priming has been studied with neuroimaging (Deiber et al., 1996, Lee et al., 1999) and ERPs (Leuthold and Jentzsch, 2002, Mathews et al., 2006). Consistently, lateralized activity in the premotor cortex has been found to be related to response priming. For example, Dehaene et al. (1998) reported that subliminal primes trigger lateralized readiness potentials (LRP) indicating a preparatory activation of the primed response. In addition to response-related evoked activity, there is good evidence that induced beta oscillations in the motor cortex are linked to movement preparation and movement execution (Kaiser et al., 2001, Pfurtscheller and Neuper, 1997). Consistently, in an inhibition-of-return task, beta power in the motor cortex has been found to be responsive both to cue and target presentation (Pastötter et al., 2008). Thereby, induced beta oscillations and evoked readiness potentials are suggested to arise from different neuronal mechanisms providing different information in movement preparation (Shibasaki and Hallett, 2006). In the present study, we sought to examine the role of beta oscillations in response priming.

In the present EEG experiment, a localization-discrimination task was used in which, half-and-half, manual responses were primed by cues or not. Cuing was exogenous (peripheral cues) and uninformative (50% cue validity). Conflict arose when an actual response (e.g., a left-hand response) had been preceded by an invalid cue (e.g., a cue that primed a right-hand response). We examined induced brain oscillations on the source level and hypothesized that beta power in the premotor cortex is linked to response priming and theta power in the ACC is related to conflict processing. To examine whether conflict processing in the present task affects the priming process, we analyzed the relation of theta activity in the ACC and beta activity in motor sources both between and within subjects.

Section snippets

Participants

Twenty-four right-handed volunteers (15 females and 9 males) participated in the study. All participants reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Mean age was 23.1 years with a range of 20 to 29 years (SD = 2.0). No participant reported any history of neurological disease. All participants gave their written informed consent and received credit points or monetary reward for participation.

Materials and experimental design

Participants viewed a computer monitor from a distance of 150 cm and were instructed to maintain

Reaction times

Analyzing mean median RT of participants' responses (Table 1), a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the within-subject factors CUE (no cue, above, below) and TARGET (above, below) revealed a main effect of CUE (F (2, 46) = 25.6, p < .001) and a CUE × TARGET interaction (F (2, 46) = 75.0, p < .001), but no main effect of the factor TARGET (F (1, 23) < 1), the latter showing that right and left hand responses did not differ in RT. Importantly, the interaction arose from faster responses in valid

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to examine the role of conflict processing in response priming with electrophysiological measurements of oscillatory brain activity. Consistent with prior behavioral work, we found response processing to be faster after valid cuing and more error-prone after invalid cuing. In line with prior physiological work, response processing was related to oscillatory beta activity in the motor cortex and conflict processing was linked to theta activity in the ACC. Going

Acknowledgments

The authors thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions on a previous version of the manuscript.

References (41)

  • PfurtschellerG. et al.

    Event-related cortical desynchronization detected by power measurements of scalp EEG

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1977)
  • PfurtschellerG. et al.

    Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1997)
  • SalmelinR. et al.

    Functional segregation of movement-related rhythmic activity in the human brain

    NeuroImage

    (1995)
  • ShibasakiH. et al.

    What is the Bereitschafts potential?

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2006)
  • TrujilloL.T. et al.

    Theta EEG dynamics of the error-related negativity

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2007)
  • van VeenV. et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • Bogacz, R., Wagenmakers, E.J., Forstmann, B.U., Nieuwenhuis, S., 2010. The neural basis of the speed-accuracy tradeoff....
  • BotvinickM.M. et al.

    Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex

    Nature

    (1999)
  • BraverT.S. et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2001)
  • CarterC.S. et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: an update of theory and data

    Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (24)

    • On the locus of the effect of alerting on response conflict: An event-related EEG study with a speed-accuracy tradeoff manipulation

      2019, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      A body of evidence suggests that this effect reflects a neural oscillatory mechanism underlying the processes of conflict detection and resolution (for review see Cavanagh & Frank, 2014; Cohen, 2014a). The theta modulation originates from the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), including the midcingulate cortex (MCC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Nigbur, Ivanova, & Stürmer, 2011; Cohen, 2011; Pastötter, Hanslmayr, & Bauml, 2010), and presumably forms a prefrontal-theta network of functional local communication between mPFC and the areas of dorsal and ventral lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) (Cavanagh, Cohen, & Allen, 2009; Cohen & Ridderinkhof, 2013), which constitutes the brain's executive control system (cf. Petersen & Posner, 2012; Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, 2004). If the interaction between phasic alerting and response conflict is due to alerting-triggered enhancement of the scope of visual processing (the global processing hypothesis), the interactive effects should presumably be observed beginning from the visual N1 component.

    • Cortical hypoactivation during resting EEG suggests central nervous system pathology in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome

      2018, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The orbitofrontal gyrus is involved in self-initiated behavior and disruption to this region—or connections between—is associated with a clinical reduction in volition (Levy, 2012; Ropper & Samuels, 2009). Likewise, the anterior cingulate plays a pivotal role in attention, working memory, and conflict resolution, and regulation of emotional/motivational responses (Pastotter, Hanslmayr, & Bauml, 2010). In patients with CFS, abnormality in the ventral portion of the anterior cingulate was linked to slower reaction times and fewer correct responses (de Lange et al., 2004).

    • Mechanisms and neuronal networks involved in reactive and proactive cognitive control of interference in working memory

      2014, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Transient right IFG activation was also found in reactive control in the n-back task, and was supposed to reflect executive control processes corresponding to response inhibition rather than interference resolution per se (e.g. Marklund and Persson, 2012). In that study, analysis was restricted to specific regions, and ACC activation was not studied, but earlier studies underline the role of the ACC in reactive control of response conflict (e.g. Braver et al., 2001; Pastötter et al., 2010). The right IFG was associated earlier with both preparation for response conflict resolution (e.g. in cued stop-signal paradigms), and the actual inhibition of prepotent response tendencies once they threaten to interfere with successful task performance (e.g. Chikazoe et al., 2009; Jahfari et al., 2010).

    • A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling

      2014, Trends in Neurosciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, due to uncertainties in the precise interpretation of the BOLD response [32–35], findings from electrophysiological investigations provide firmer grounding upon which to develop a neuroscientific account of conflict. Time-frequency-based data analyses of electroencephalography (EEG) data allow inferences regarding neural oscillations [36], and have revealed that the neural response to conflict is an increase in theta band (4–8 Hz) power over midfrontal electrodes [37–41] (Figure 1C). Despite being a temporally brief response (several hundreds of ms), the conflict-modulated theta power is non-phase-locked to stimulus onset or to button press [42].

    • The neural oscillations of conflict adaptation in the human frontal region

      2013, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the present study, activity in the theta-band TF-ROI (6–7 Hz, 0.2–1.00 s) does not display the general pattern involved in conflict processing in the ‘look’ condition. Using the novel look-to-do transition design, we found the opposite pattern (increased theta-band ERS for the congruent type relative to the incongruent type) This finding is novel but disagrees with previous studies (Cavanagh et al., 2009; Cohen & Cavanagh, 2011; Cohen et al., 2008; Hanslmayr et al., 2008; Pastötter et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2005). The ERS of the EEG oscillations in the theta-band frequency range has been thought of as an index of conflict in the incongruent type when compared with the congruent type (Hanslmayr et al., 2008; van Steenbergen et al., 2012).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text