Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 25, Issue 4, 1 May 2005, Pages 1279-1291
NeuroImage

Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038Get rights and content

Abstract

Adolescents' propensity for risk-taking and reward-seeking behaviors suggests a heightened sensitivity for reward, reflected by greater feedback-related activity changes in reward circuitry (e.g., nucleus accumbens), and/or a lower sensitivity to potential harm reflected by weaker feedback-related activity changes in avoidance circuitry (e.g., amygdala) relative to adults. Responses of nucleus accumbens and amygdala to valenced outcomes (reward receipt and reward omission) were assayed using an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging procedure paired with a monetary reward task in 14 adults and 16 adolescents. Bilateral amygdala and nucleus accumbens showed significantly greater activation when winning than when failing to win in both groups. Group comparisons revealed stronger activation of left nucleus accumbens by adolescents, and of left amygdala by adults. When examining responses to reward receipts and to reward omissions separately, the most robust group difference was within the amygdala during reward omission. The reduction of the fMRI BOLD signal in the amygdala in response to reward omission was larger for adults than for adolescents. Correlations showed a close link between negative emotion and amygdala decreased BOLD signal in adults, and between positive emotion and nucleus accumbens activation in adolescents. Overall, these findings support the notion that the signal differences between positive and negative outcomes involve the nucleus accumbens more in adolescents than in adults, and the amygdala more in adults than in adolescents. These developmental differences, if replicated, may have important implications for the development of early-onset disorders of emotion and motivation.

Introduction

Adolescence is a critical period characterized by high levels of risk-taking, impulsivity, and novelty-seeking (Arnett, 1999). Adolescents present vulnerability for gambling (Chambers and Potenza, 2003) and addiction (Chambers et al., 2003). Their propensity for risk-taking may relate to increased responses to reward (Laviola et al., 2003), and reduced harm avoidance (Maggs et al., 1995, Wills et al., 1994). Consummatory processing (responses to outcomes) influences the motivation to select specific courses of actions, and subsequently can indirectly affect pattern of decision-making.

A growing body of work in animals is being conducted to better understand the biological basis of reward-related processing. Single-cell recording studies reveal that dopamine neurons in non-human primates are activated at the receipt of unpredicted rewards, emit no signal after a predicted reward, and emit a negative signal (decreased spike production from baseline) when a predicted reward fails to occur (Schultz et al., 1997).

Similarly, neuroimaging studies in human adults (Breiter et al., 2001, Delgado et al., 2000, Knutson et al., 2001, O'Doherty et al., 2001) reveal that reward receipt enhances activity of subcortical structures (i.e., striatum and amygdala), and of cortical regions (i.e., mesial prefrontal cortex), whereas reward omission, or exposure to losses, tends to reduce the fMRI BOLD signal in these same structures.

At present, two neuroimaging studies have examined reward circuitry in adolescents (Bjork et al., 2004, May et al., 2004), only one with an adult comparison group (Bjork et al., 2004). Bjork et al. (2004) found less activation of the right nucleus accumbens (NAcc) during anticipation of gains in adolescents than in adults. They interpreted this finding as indicating a selectively weaker recruitment of motivational, but not consummatory, components of reward-directed behavior in adolescents. Here, we propose that age-related differences in consummatory behavior and underlying neural substrates may contribute to risk-taking in adolescence.

Whereas increased risk-taking in adolescents can relate to greater reward responses, through enhanced appetitive value of positive outcomes (Arnett, 1992, Gardner and Herman, 1991, Laviola et al., 2003), it can also indicate decreased response to danger or threat, through a decreased aversiveness of negative outcomes, leading to reduced harm avoidance behavior (Maggs et al., 1995, Wills et al., 1994, Wilson and Daly, 1985). The amygdala circuitry is critically involved in anticipation of negative outcomes but also in the response to negative stimuli (LeDoux, 2000). Although participating in the emotional coding of both appetitive and aversive stimuli (Baxter and Murray, 2002, Zald, 2003), the amygdala can function as a behavioral “brake” (Zald, 2003). Its functional characteristics during adolescence are virtually unknown (Killgore et al., 2001, Yurgelun-Todd et al., 2003), although neuroimaging studies suggest distinct patterns of amygdala modulation in affective paradigms during adolescence and adulthood (Monk et al., 2003). These studies have focused on various aspects of the amygdala, including its modulation by attention during processing of fearful facial expression (Monk et al., 2003, Thomas et al., 2001), and its response to passive viewing of fearful and neutral faces (Thomas et al., 2001). Adolescents seemed to be more sensitive to the emotional content than to the attentional demands of the task in Monk et al.'s (2003) study, and to be more responsive to neutral vs. fearful expressions than adults in Thomas et al.'s (2001) study. None of these studies have examined reward-related processes per se.

Here, we test whether adults and adolescents differ in the NAcc's and amygdala's involvement in reward-related processes, specifically in response to reinforcers. Converging data emerge from three different perspectives: the approach/avoidance model of adolescent behavior (Chambers et al., 2003, Spear, 2000), the available neuroimaging findings in adults (Breiter et al., 2001, Delgado et al., 2000, Knutson et al., 2001, O'Doherty et al., 2001), and the electrophysiological work in primates (Schultz, 2002, Schultz et al., 1997). Taken together, this work generates the hypotheses that (1) responses to outcome will affect the NAcc more in adolescents than in adults, and (2) the amygdala more in adults than in adolescents, and (3) that self-ratings of affective responses will correlate with regional activation.

Section snippets

Sample

Eighteen healthy adolescents and 16 healthy adults participated in the study. Data from four subjects (2 adolescents and 2 adults) were excluded from the analysis because of excessive head movement (greater than 1.5 mm in any one direction). Subjects were recruited through newspaper advertisements and were financially compensated for their participation. Inclusion criteria were right-handedness, age between 9 and 17 years for adolescents and 20 and 40 years for adults, absence of past and

Sample and task performance

Fourteen adults (6 females/8 males; 26.7 ± 5.0 years old) and sixteen adolescents (9 females/7 males; 13.3 ± 2.1 years old) completed the study. Mean IQ (adults 109.1 ± 12.1; adolescents 107.6 ± 12.4) and mean socioeconomic status (adults 69.4 ± 31.2; adolescents 55.3 ± 17.8) measured using the Hollingshead's Four Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975) were similar between groups.

On the 5-point self-rating scales (feel: 1 ‘neutral,’ 5 ‘very good’ in win outcome, and ‘very bad’ in

Discussion

We examined, in adolescents and in adults, brain regions sensitive to reward receipt/reward omission. Based on the propensity for risk-taking and novelty-seeking in adolescents, we predicted a more active reward-related system (i.e., NAcc), and a less active harm avoidance-related system (i.e., amygdala) during responses to outcomes in adolescents than in adults. Overall findings supported this prediction.

Acknowledgments

We thank the staff of the NIH MR Center for making this study possible, Harvey Iwamoto for his assistance in programming the task, and Suzanne Munson, Andrea Hoberman, Leanne Montgomery, and Alison Merikangas for their help in running the study.

References (69)

  • W.D. Killgore et al.

    Activation of the amygdala and anterior cingulate during nonconscious processing of sad versus happy faces

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • B. Knutson et al.

    fMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task

    NeuroImage

    (2000)
  • G. Laviola et al.

    Risk-taking behavior in adolescent mice: psychobiological determinants and early epigenetic influence

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2003)
  • J.C. May et al.

    Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward-related brain circuitry in children and adolescents

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • S.M. McClure et al.

    Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • C.S. Monk et al.

    Adolescent immaturity in attention-related brain engagement to emotional facial expressions

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • A.G. Phillips et al.

    Amygdalar control of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system: parallel pathways to motivated behavior

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2003)
  • R.D. Rogers et al.

    Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • W. Schultz

    Getting formal with dopamine and reward

    Neuron

    (2002)
  • D.L. Segal et al.

    Reliability of the structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R: an evaluative review

    Compr. Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • L.P. Spear

    The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2000)
  • K.M. Thomas et al.

    Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • T.A. Wills et al.

    Novelty seeking, risk taking, and related constructs as predictors of adolescent substance use: an application of Cloninger's theory

    J. Subst. Abuse

    (1994)
  • M. Wilson et al.

    Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: the young male syndrome

    Ethol. Sociobiol.

    (1985)
  • D.H. Zald

    The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli

    Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev.

    (2003)
  • E. Zarahn

    Testing for neural responses during temporal components of trials with BOLD fMRI

    NeuroImage

    (2000)
  • J.J. Arnett

    Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered

    Am. Psychol.

    (1999)
  • M.G. Baxter et al.

    The amygdala and reward

    Nat. Rev., Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • G.S. Berns et al.

    Predictability modulates human brain response to reward

    J. Neurosci.

    (2001)
  • J.M. Bjork et al.

    Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults

    J. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • R.J.R. Blair

    The neurobiology of antisocial behavior and psychopathy

  • R.L. Buckner

    Event-related fMRI and the hemodynamic response

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (1998)
  • G. Bush et al.

    Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2002)
  • R.N. Cardinal et al.

    Effects of selective excitotoxic lesions of the nucleus accumbens core, anterior cingulate cortex, and central nucleus of the amygdala on autoshaping performance in rats

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text