Intracranial self-stimulation as a positive reinforcer to study impulsivity in a probability discounting paradigm

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.04.025Get rights and content

Abstract

Probability discounting is used to study risky decision-making in humans and rodents. In these paradigms, the subject chooses between a small reward that is always delivered and a large reward that is delivered with varying probabilities. Risk-taking is defined as a preference for the large, uncertain reward. The aversive consequence associated with this task involves choosing the large reward and not obtaining it. To study this form of impulsivity in rodents, food reinforcement is commonly used. Using this reinforcer, and the need to food-deprive rodents to enhance task performance, may be problematic in rodent models that exhibit eating disorders, in pharmacological assessments that alter feeding, and for assessments of the neurocircuitry that is engaged by both feeding and risk-taking. We reveal here that electrical intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) can be used as the positive reinforcer in risk assessments (i.e., probability discounting). ICSS was selected as it is rapidly acquired, the operant procedures are retained for months, and no tolerance or satiety develops to the reinforcer; thus, ICSS can be used in multiple test sessions in a repeated measures design. We developed an efficient, standardized, six phase ICSS-mediated protocol that allowed for the assessments of risk-taking in a probability discounting task. We demonstrated that the discounting behavior remained stable for several weeks. The value of this protocol is discussed in terms of practical as well as theoretical advantages of using ICSS-mediated reinforcement.

Highlights

► We developed a new, efficient protocol to measure risk-taking in rats. ► We use intracranial self-stimulation as the reward in probability discounting. ► We discuss advantages of the ICSS discounting protocol over food reinforcement.

Introduction

Impulsivity can be regarded as “actions that appear poorly conceived, prematurely expressed, unduly risky, or inappropriate to the situation” (Daruna and Barnes, 1993). While some beneficial aspects of impulsivity are known (Dickman, 1990), it is generally recognized as a dysfunctional trait that is frequently associated with numerous neurological and psychiatric disorders including frontal lobe damage, schizophrenia, attention deficit-hyperactive disorder and substance abuse disorders. According to the American Psychiatric Association, impulse control disorders (ICDs) are a form of psychiatric disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). ICDs include trichotillomania, intermittent explosive disorder, pathological gambling, kleptomania, pyromania, hypersexuality, compulsive shopping and others.

To understand impulsivity and ICDs and to subsequently develop therapies targeted to particular aspects of the disorder, laboratory protocols that model attributes of impulsivity are required. Risky decision-making is one facet of impulsivity. A common method used to study risky choice in both humans and laboratory rodents is the probability discounting paradigm (Mobini et al., 2000, Rachlin et al., 1991, Richards et al., 1999). In this task, the subject can choose between a small reward that is always delivered and a large reward that is delivered with varying probabilities. Risky behavior is defined as a preference for the large uncertain reward. The aversive consequence associated with this task involves choosing the large reward and not obtaining it (Cardinal and Howes, 2005). In rodent testing of probability discounting, food is often used as the positive reinforcer and to motivate the animal, salience of the food is enhanced by food-deprivation. This approach presents several disadvantages which can potentially confound outcomes. First, internal factors, such as hunger or thirst, can themselves lead to a change in impulsive behavior in animals (Minamimoto et al., 2009, Schuck-Paim et al., 2004). Second, chronic food restriction can lead to adaptations in dopaminergic (Carlson et al., 1988, Carr et al., 2003, Carr et al., 2009, Collins et al., 2008) and serotonergic signaling (Haleem and Haider, 1996, Huether et al., 1997, Kohsaka et al., 1980). These neurotransmitters also play a role in impulsivity (Adriani et al., 2009, Mehlman et al., 1994, Mobini et al., 2000, Soubrié, 1986, Winstanley et al., 2005). Moreover, this reward option may not be possible for assessments of risky choice in rat models of human neuropathologies that present eating disorders or for testing pharmacologics that alter feeding behaviors. Thus, we sought to design a probability discounting paradigm that utilized a positive reinforcer that avoided such shortcomings. To be broadly applicable to a range of laboratory assessments, we determined that criteria for this reinforcer should include the following: (i) It should more directly engage brain “reward centers” than is possible with food reward. (ii) It should be conducive to robust operant task testing. (iii) It should demonstrate a range of reward values that can be discriminated by the rat. (iv) Finally, it should support stable responding for several weeks. We reveal here that intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) meets these criteria. In ICSS procedures, rats perform an operant task to obtain a positive reinforcing current delivered via an electrode implanted in reward regions of the brain (Olds and Milner, 1954). For the current study, we selected the medial forebrain bundle (MFB) at the level of the lateral hypothalamus (LH) as the stimulation target. This structure is well known to readily support ICSS with a large range of stimulation parameters. We detail how this reward parameter can be successfully implemented for probability discounting paradigms, and we verify performance stability and persistence.

Section snippets

Methods

Male Sprague–Dawley rats weighing 250–274 g upon arrival (Harlan, Indianapolis, IN) were housed in pairs under environmentally controlled conditions (7:00 AM/7:00 PM light/dark cycle, temperature maintained at 23–25 °C) with access to rat chow and water ad libitum. All rats were handled according to established procedures in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Research Council, Washington, DC); specific protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use

Phase 1: shaping

For this Phase, rats had to learn the association between a lever press and receiving an experimenter-applied EBS. Rats with stimulation electrode tips placed in the LH (n = 6; Fig. 1) met the minimum criteria of eight lever presses/min on both levers within three sessions (corresponding to protocol test days 7–8). Rats with electrode tips outside the LH (n = 2; Fig. 1), did not successfully shape after six sessions and subsequently were removed from the study. For the six rats with proper the tip

Discussion

The current report revealed that ICSS can be used as a positive reinforcer to study risk-taking in a probability discounting paradigm. With this paradigm, rats rapidly learned to lever press for EBS, and they reliably chose to receive a larger EBS more than a smaller EBS, regardless if the variable EBS modality was intensity or frequency, indicating the larger EBS was a stronger reinforcer. As the delivery probability of the large reinforcer decreased, rats decreased their selection for the

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Michael J. Fox Foundation, the Parkinson's Disease Foundation, and the Rice Foundation.

References (42)

  • S.M. Bell et al.

    Food-deprivation increases cocaine-induced conditioned place preference and locomotor activity in rats

    Psychopharmacology

    (1997)
  • H.A. Berlin et al.

    Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions

    Brain

    (2004)
  • S. Cabeza de Vaca et al.

    Food restriction enhances the central rewarding effect of abused drugs

    J Neurosci

    (1998)
  • R.N. Cardinal et al.

    Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core on choice between small certain rewards and large uncertain rewards in rats

    BMC Neurosci

    (2005)
  • K.D. Carr et al.

    Reward-potentiating effects of D-1 dopamine receptor agonist and AMPAR GluR1 antagonist in nucleus accumbens shell and their modulation by food restriction

    Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    (2009)
  • G.T. Collins et al.

    Food restriction alters N′-propyl-4,5,6,7-tetrahydrobenzothiazole-2,6-diamine dihydrochloride (pramipexole)-induced yawning, hypothermia, and locomotor activity in rats: evidence for sensitization of dopamine D2 receptor-mediated effects

    J Pharmacol Exp Ther

    (2008)
  • J.H. Daruna et al.

    A neurodevelopmental view of impulsivity

  • S.J. Dickman

    Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: personality and cognitive correlates

    J Pers Soc Psychol

    (1990)
  • J.L. Evenden et al.

    The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats: the effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement

    Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    (1996)
  • C.R. Gallistel et al.

    Measuring the subjective magnitude of brain stimulation reward by titration with rate of reward

    Behav Neurosci

    (1991)
  • S. Ghods-Sharifi et al.

    Fundamental contribution by the basolateral amygdala to different forms of decision making

    J Neurosci

    (2009)
  • Cited by (19)

    • Mirtazapine and ketanserin alter preference for gambling-like schedules of reinforcement in rats

      2017, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      All experimental data were recorded by a PC connected to the operant chambers via an interface. The ICSS testing protocol was modified from those previously established in our laboratory using probability discounting (Rokosik and Napier, 2011; Rokosik and Napier, 2012; Holtz et al., 2016) and delay discounting (Tedford et al., 2015) tasks. Rats were trained to associate a reinforcing electrical stimulation of the brain with pressing a lever using a forepaw.

    • Decision-making and impulse control disorders in parkinson's disease

      2017, Decision Neuroscience: An Integrative Perspective
    • Pharmacologically distinct pramipexole-mediated akinesia vs. risk-taking in a rat model of Parkinson's disease

      2016, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      DLS lesion-imposed retrograde degeneration of dopaminergic neurons is time-dependent, and we verified that the number of TH+ neurons in the dorsolateral pars compacta is reduced to 50% by 90 days post-lesion (Tedford et al., 2015). Our protocols result in remarkably consistent DLS TH reductions and the associated akinesia, and probability and delayed discounting are extremely stable for at least 90 days post-lesion (Hendrickson and Rasmussen, 2013; Rokosik and Napier, 2011). Thus, behavioral outcomes of our prior and current studies do not reflect the extent of dopaminergic cell loss, and the current observations suggest a general higher sensitivity to DAs by risk-takers that involves other factors.

    • Rodent models of impulsive-compulsive behaviors in Parkinson's disease: How far have we reached?

      2015, Neurobiology of Disease
      Citation Excerpt :

      To the best of our knowledge, this food-based test paradigm has not yet been applied to any rodent model of PD. Rokosik and Napier have developed a probability discounting paradigm based on ICSS (Rokosik and Napier, 2011), and then applied this paradigm to rats with bilateral 6-OHDA-lesions in the dorsolateral striatum (Rokosik and Napier, 2012). In addition to exploring differences between lesioned and intact animals at baseline, the authors examined the effects of Pramipexole (Rokosik and Napier, 2012).

    • Dyskinesias and impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease: From pathogenesis to potential therapeutic approaches

      2015, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, a rat model of parkinsonism by bilateral 6-OHDA injection in the striatum has been used to test the effect of DA agonists on impulsivity, with some interesting findings obtained. A study developed to measure probability discounting, an aspect of risk taking, and using intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) for positive reinforcement, has shown that parkinsonian animals, as control animals, display an increase in risky choices after chronic administration of pramipexole (Rokosik and Napier, 2011). In addition, they are more sensitive to the rewarding effects of pramipexole than control animals, as evaluated using conditioned place preference (Riddle et al., 2012).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text