Neural basis of sensorimotor learning: modifying internal models

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.11.003Get rights and content

The neural basis of the internal models used in sensorimotor transformations is beginning to be uncovered. Sensorimotor learning involves the modification of such models. Different stages of sensory-motor processing have been explored with a continuum of experimental tasks, from learning arbitrary associations of sensory cues to movements, to adapting to altered kinematic and dynamic environments. Several groups have been studying changes in neuronal activity in cortical and subcortical areas that may be related to the acquisition and consolidation processes. We discuss the progress and challenges in understanding how these learning-related neural changes are involved in the modification of internal models, and offer future directions.

Introduction

Sensorimotor learning is essential for daily behavior. It is used for maintaining known behavioral abilities and for learning new skills. For example, it is used to learn how to move a cursor on a computer screen or to assign behavioral relevance to some arbitrary stimulus, such as the instruction ‘stop the car’ when the traffic light is red. Current opinion holds that sensorimotor actions are produced by transformations that utilize internal models of the mechanics of the body and the world [1] (Figure 1b). Inverse models work in the obvious direction: transforming desired goals into a plan to accomplish thema [2] (Figure 1a). These transformations are undoubtedly a major part of sensorimotor control. However, since action based solely on sensory feedback is slow and risky, the brain  like efficient artificial control systems  has developed the power of prediction. Predictive abilities can be achieved by using forward models that simulate the outcomes of a given plan (Figure 1a). There are compelling theoretical arguments why forward models are an essential feature of sensorimotor control, perception, and learning [3]. First, forward models can be used to give instantaneous predicted feedback that provides an estimate of the state of the controlled effector, and thus overcome the significant delays of real sensory feedback. Therefore, forward models are a necessary ingredient for the contemporary description of the sensorimotor system as an optimal feedback controller [4]. When real sensory feedback does arrive, it can be combined with such predicted feedback to compensate for limitations and noise of the sensory systems [5]. A second proposed role for forward models involves anticipating and cancelling the sensory effects of self initiated actions from the incoming sensory stimuli (reafference). Finally, during learning itself, forward models may be used to generate sensory error signals (predicted feedback minus real feedback) which can guide learning of inverse models (distal teacher) [6]. An increasing number of psychophysical experiments support the notion that humans make use of forward models (e.g. [7, 8, 9, 10], for a review see [11]). Classic experiments on fish [12, 13], bats [14], and cats [15] have demonstrated the existence of corollary discharges (the outputs of the forward models) and their effects on reafference. More recently, two elegant series of electrophysiological experiments in monkeys have culminated in showing how forward models are used in the saccadic eye movement system [16••], and in sensing of head motion in the vestibular system [17]. Mulliken et al. [18] showed that during hand movements a subpopulation of neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of monkeys may encode a forward estimate of the cursor direction on the screen. They found neurons whose maximal encoding evolved too late to subserve feedforward motor processing, but too early to result from sensory feedback.

The extent that prediction is used by different brain functions remains an open question. The theory of active perception proposes that in order to perceive the world, predictions can direct active exploratory movements (e.g. of the eyes, or the vibrissa [19]) and top-down attention to the informative features of the stimulus. Going even further, the proposal that perception itself is none other than the process of integrating our internally generated predictions with the incoming sensory stream is an attractive one. In this vain, Noë argues that ‘the experience of seeing occurs when the organism masters what we call the governing laws of sensorimotor contingency’ [20]. Thus, forward models may be a more fundamental and pervasive feature of brain organization than just a component of motor control [21] (Figure 1c).

Different sensorimotor learning tasks modify the relevant internal models. Depending on the context, a red light may be associated with different behaviors. Likewise, we may need to learn different sensorimotor transformations when we control a cursor by using different devices, such as a mouse, a touch-pad, or an electronic-pen. When the design of such devices merges well with our internal models, we learn to control novel devices more quickly and naturally.

Section snippets

Neural basis of sensorimotor learning

Different sensorimotor learning tasks involve different learning strategies. As a result, distinct tasks affect internal models at different stages of sensory-motor processing (Figure 1b). Here we discuss the challenges and review the progress in studying the neural basis of sensorimotor learning.

Summary and future directions

While there is a growing body of physiological data, our knowledge of how the brain learns to control sensorimotor actions is still in its infancy. Where, when, and how does the brain implement the internal models underlying the sensory-motor transformations that guide both our actions and perceptions remain open questions. To this end, theoretical models which yield experimental predictions are of key value. Specifically, computational approaches from engineering need to be converted into

References and recommended reading

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:

  • • of special interest

  • •• of outstanding interest

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank several of our collaborators and the students in the lab; some of their results we have cited herein. Our thanks especially go to Dr Hagai Bergman, Dr Rony Paz, Dr Neta Zach, Dorrit Inbar, and Yael Grinvald.

The study was supported in part by the Israeli Science Foundation (ISF) and the American Israeli Bi-National Foundation (BSF).

References (68)

  • M. Zacksenhouse et al.

    Cortical modulations increase in early sessions with brain–machine interface

    PLoS ONE

    (2007)
  • D.M. Wolpert et al.

    Computational principles of movement neuroscience

    Nat Neurosci

    (2000)
  • E. Todorov

    Optimality principles in sensorimotor control

    Nat Neurosci

    (2004)
  • D.M. Wolpert et al.

    An internal model for sensorimotor integration

    Science

    (1995)
  • M.J. Jordan et al.

    Forward models: supervised learning with a distal teacher

    Cognit Sci

    (1990)
  • S.J. Blakemore et al.

    Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli

    J Cogn Neurosci

    (1999)
  • S. Vaziri et al.

    Why does the brain predict sensory consequences of oculomotor commands? Optimal integration of the predicted and the actual sensory feedback

    J Neurosci

    (2006)
  • J. McIntyre et al.

    Does the brain model Newton's laws?

    Nat Neurosci

    (2001)
  • P.R. Davidson et al.

    Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review

    J Neural Eng

    (2005)
  • B.L. Roberts et al.

    The activity of lateral-line efferent neurones in stationary and swimming dogfish

    J Exp Biol

    (1972)
  • B. Zipser et al.

    Interaction of electrosensory and electromotor signals in lateral line lobe of a mormyrid fish

    J Neurophysiol

    (1976)
  • N. Suga et al.

    Site of neural attenuation of responses to self-vocalized sounds in echolocating bats

    Science

    (1974)
  • C. Ghez et al.

    Inhibition of afferent transmission in cuneate nucleus during voluntary movement in the cat

    Brain Res

    (1972)
  • M.A. Sommer et al.

    Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements

    Annu Rev Neurosci

    (2008)
  • K.E. Cullen et al.

    Signal processing in the vestibular system during active versus passive head movements

    J Neurophysiol

    (2004)
  • G.H. Mulliken et al.

    Forward estimation of movement state in posterior parietal cortex

    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A

    (2008)
  • D. Kleinfeld et al.

    Active sensation: insights from the rodent vibrissa sensorimotor system

    Curr Opin Neurobiol

    (2006)
  • A. Noe

    Action in Perception

    (2005)
  • M. Kawato et al.

    Internal forward models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling

    Prog Brain Res

    (2003)
  • C.S. Li et al.

    Neuronal correlates of motor performance and motor learning in the primary motor cortex of monkeys adapting to an external force field

    Neuron

    (2001)
  • D. Marr

    A theory of cerebellar cortex

    J Physiol

    (1969)
  • Y.W. Tseng et al.

    Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching

    J Neurophysiol

    (2007)
  • J. Diedrichsen et al.

    Dissociating timing and coordination as functions of the cerebellum

    J Neurosci

    (2007)
  • D.M. Wolpert et al.

    Internal models in the cerebellum

    Trends Cogn Sci

    (1998)
  • Cited by (60)

    • Opposing Influence of Top-down and Bottom-up Input on Excitatory Layer 2/3 Neurons in Mouse Primary Visual Cortex

      2020, Neuron
      Citation Excerpt :

      It is still unclear how the nervous system learns and represents the relationships between movement and sensory feedback. One brain structure involved in complex sensorimotor learning processes is the neocortex (Lalazar and Vaadia, 2008; Makino et al., 2016). Cortical areas receive both bottom-up sensory-driven input and top-down input that is thought to signal contextual and motor-related information (Engel et al., 2001).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text