Elsevier

Cognitive Brain Research

Volume 25, Issue 2, October 2005, Pages 459-471
Cognitive Brain Research

Research Report
Cortical activation during Pavlovian fear conditioning depends on heart rate response patterns: An MEG study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.07.006Get rights and content

Abstract

In the present study, we examined stimulus-driven neuromagnetic activity in a delayed Pavlovian aversive conditioning paradigm using steady state visual evoked fields (SSVEF). Subjects showing an accelerative heart rate (HR) component to the CS+ during learning trials exhibited an increased activation in sensory and parietal cortex due to CS+ depiction in the extinction block. This was accompanied by a selective orientation response (OR) to the CS+ during extinction as indexed by HR deceleration. However, they did not show any differential cortical activation patterns during acquisition. In contrast, subjects not showing an accelerative HR component but rather unspecific HR changes during learning were characterized by greater activity in left orbito-frontal brain regions in the acquisition block but did not show differential SSVEF patterns during extinction. The results suggest that participants expressing different HR responses also differ in their stimulus-driven neuromagnetic response pattern to an aversively conditioned stimulus.

Introduction

Learning that certain events in an individual's environment signal potential threat or danger is an important ability to ensure survival of the organism. Pavlovian fear conditioning [69] as a laboratory model of the aforementioned ability represents a simple form of associative learning that is part of most mammalian defensive behavior systems [21].

The neural systems underlying fear conditioning have been elucidated by animal models as well as human imaging studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) [13], [58]. The amygdala has been regarded as the key structure processing aversive stimuli via afferents from sensory thalamus [55], [84], enabling fast evaluation of noxious stimuli without complex computations of the cortex. As the amygdaloidal complex is highly interconnected with the temporal, orbito-frontal, and insular cortices [1], [2], [64], [74], the amygdala is anatomically well placed for stimulus association learning in various sensory domains. Given its fast access to sensory information and its neuroanatomical location, the amygdala has been assigned a central role in mediating synaptic changes at the cortical level [59] necessary for the association between the conditioned (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US). For example, Buchel and colleagues [14] demonstrated in an fMRI study that the amygdala activation was most prominent during acquisition of the conditioned response (CR) and LaBar and collaborators [50] showed a decline of amygdala activation over trials possibly reflecting that amygdala activation is only necessary until associations between the CS and US have been formed.

In addition to amygdala circuitry and highly interconnected with it, a distributed network of cortical areas seems to be involved in classical fear conditioning as well. For example, standard fear conditioning paradigms using auditory CS not only reported enhanced spike firing rates in neurons of the amygdala and hippocampus, but also of neurons in auditory cortex [5], [52], [75]. In the visual system of primates, the amygdala projects back to various stages in the ventral visual path and receives itself highly processed sensory information [3]. This neural architecture may be the basis of a mechanism, which enhances stimulus processing in visual sensory systems. In line with this notion, imaging studies in humans have shown greater activations for the reinforced visual CS (CS+) as compared to the nonreinforced CS (CS−) in temporal and occipital cortices [15], [39].

Additional support to the involvement of cortical areas in fear conditioning comes from cognitive theories of Pavlovian conditioning that emphasize anticipation or attentional processes [70]. For instance, Rescorla [77] has emphasized that the CS+ in a Pavlovian conditioning paradigm gains predictive value that enables the organism to anticipate an aversive event. The ability to obtain such a predictive value has been associated with the salience of a stimulus [57]. Thus, at a neurophysiological level, cortical systems mediating attentional and evaluative processes should come into play.

There is good evidence for the involvement of a widespread fronto-parietal cortical network in orienting to a stimulus and integrating its features [16], [54], [68], [80]. Human imaging studies of Pavlovian conditioning have consistently reported enhancement of cortical activity for visual reinforced CSs in brain regions like the frontal, temporal, parietal cortices, and the anterior cingulate [14], [22], [23], [39], [47], [50] possibly reflecting the involvement of a neural network mediating attention/emotion aspects of that stimulus. In a recent fMRI study, Armony and Dolan [4] demonstrated that a visual context stimulus modulated conditioned BOLD responses in auditory cortex associated with activity in parietal cortex, further illustrating the importance of cortical structures being part of a cortical attention network.

In the present work, we investigated changes of cortical processes over time using the steady state visual evoked field (SSVEF) technique. SSVEFs (a counterpart of the SSVEP in EEG research) reflect widely distributed functional networks oscillating coherently at the driving stimulus frequency and are sensitive to attentional and complex cognitive processes [61], [66], [71], [85].

As expectancy and attentional processes may be involved in Pavlovian conditioning as outlined above, the SSVEP/SSVEF paradigm seems to be an adequate tool in order to track ongoing neural activity associated with processes of aversive conditioning. A recent SSVEP study by Gray and coworkers [26] investigated cortical oscillatory activity during the anticipation of an electric shock and revealed the involvement of frontal, temporal, and occipital electrode sites during anticipatory anxiety. Recently, Moratti and collaborators [60] have identified a fronto-parietal network during visual affective stimulus processing using magnetoencephalography (MEG) and the SSVEF technique. This finding supports the involvement of networks traditionally related to attention processes in processing of emotional information. In the same vein, it has been demonstrated that cued spatial attention and motivated attention to affective stimuli possibly interact within the same neuronal networks of attention and stimulus processing [44]. This raises the question whether similar neurophysiological processes are associated with viewing arbitrary visual stimuli which are associated with aversive responses as a consequence of classical fear conditioning. There is, however, no study to date using the SSVEP/SSVEF technique in combination with a discriminative aversive conditioning design which allows the direct comparison between the stimulus-driven neuromagnetic response of a reinforced (CS+) and nonreinforced (CS−) stimulus during acquisition and extinction blocks.

Therefore, the aim of the present study was to examine cortical sources of the SSVEF as modulated by the predictive value of a visual CS during acquisition and extinction, using MEG. Since the SSVEF response is a stimulus-driven ongoing oscillatory response in cortical networks responsible for processing the visual CS+ and CS−, estimating the underlying sources should elucidate the involvement of relevant cortical structures in Pavlovian conditioning. Applying the minimum norm estimate (MNE) [34], [35] to the SSVEF response to determine the cortical sources of the neuromagnetic field, we hypothesized that after an association between the CS+ and the US has been established, the motivationally relevant CS+ will generate greater amplitudes in fronto-parietal and extrastriate cortex responsible for allocating attention and feature extraction, respectively. As the MEG has sufficient temporal resolution, we were interested at what time interval during CS depiction a differential activation pattern in the aforementioned cortical structures could be observed for the CS+ and CS−, respectively.

Heart rate (HR) was recorded to validate the effectiveness of the conditioning procedure. Further, we wanted to evaluate the brain responses of subjects who reacted with accelerative or decelerative HR change components in response to the CS+ during acquisition as has been observed by various authors [28], [32], [38]. Hodes et al. [38] hypothesized that HR accelerators learn a fear response whereas HR decelerators just learn that two events are related in time. Hamm and Vaitl [28] demonstrated that only subjects responding with an accelerative HR component displayed potentiated startle responses to the CS+. We wanted to explore if attentional and sensory cortical networks during Pavlovian conditioning are involved in HR accelerators and decelerators to the same extent. Subjective judgments of the US were obtained in order to assess the aversiveness of the US. Further, we collected questionnaire data in order to evaluate state/trait anxiety and depressiveness of the participants.

Section snippets

Participants

Nineteen paid volunteers (10 females, 9 males, all right handed [67]) gave written consent to participate in the study. Their mean age was 27.1 years (range: 20–49 years). They had normal or corrected to normal vision and no family history of photic epilepsy. They did not report a recent critical live event or any history of psychotherapy. On the German version of the state–trait anxiety inventory (STAI) [53], subjects reported a mean state anxiety score of 31.1 (±1.1) and a mean trait anxiety

Delineation of different HR change responders by cluster analysis

The cluster analysis on HR change in acquisition trials during CS+ presentation revealed three groups. One group of subjects showed a defensive accelerative HR component in response to the CS+ (N = 8) and the other two subgroups depicted a decelerative pattern (N = 6 and N = 4, respectively). In order to obtain samples of sufficient size for further analysis, the latter two subgroups were collapsed into one decelerative group (N = 10). The obtained grouping structure was applied to the

Discussion

Examining MEG correlates of Pavlovian fear conditioning by means of steady state visual evoked fields (SSVEF), we studied the modulation of stimulus-driven magnetocortical responses to visual stimuli differing in their motivational significance (reinforced CS+ vs. nonreinforced CS−) after one stimulus had been paired with an aversive event. Using the techniques of complex demodulation and minimum norm estimation, we aimed to investigate the changes in cortical processing over stimulus

Acknowledgments

We like to thank Leonie Koban and Ursula Lommen for help with data acquisition. Research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgesellschaft (DFG) and the Volkswagen Stiftung.

References (89)

  • K.S. LaBar et al.

    Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: a mixed-trial fMRI study

    Neuron

    (1998)
  • S. Laroche et al.

    Linear relation between the magnitude of long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus and associative learning in the rat. A demonstration using commissural inhibition and local infusion of an N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor antagonist

    Neuroscience

    (1989)
  • S. Moratti et al.

    Motivated attention in emotional picture processing is reflected by activity modulation in cortical attention networks

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • E.J. Mufson et al.

    Insular interconnections with the amygdala in the rhesus monkey

    Neuroscience

    (1981)
  • M.M. Müller et al.

    Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20–28 Hz range

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (1998)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • W.M. Perlstein et al.

    Steady-state visual evoked potentials reveal frontally-mediated working memory activity in humans

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2003)
  • E.A. Phelps et al.

    Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • G.J. Quirk et al.

    Fear conditioning enhances different temporal components of tone-evoked spike trains in auditory cortex and lateral amygdala

    Neuron

    (1997)
  • D. Sabatinelli et al.

    Parallel amygdala and inferotemporal activation reflect emotional intensity and fear relevance

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • R.B. Silberstein et al.

    Steady state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) topography in a graded working memory task

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (2001)
  • D.G. Amaral

    Amygdalohippocampal and amygdalocortical projections in the primate brain

    Adv. Exp. Med. Biol.

    (1986)
  • D.G. Amaral et al.

    Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex

  • J.L. Armony et al.

    Modulation of auditory neural responses by a visual context in human fear conditioning

    NeuroReport

    (2001)
  • J.L. Armony et al.

    Differential effects of amygdala lesions on early and late plastic components of auditory cortex spike trains during fear conditioning

    J. Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • E. Bagiella et al.

    Mixed-effects models in psychophysiology

    Psychophysiology

    (2000)
  • J.D. Baker et al.

    The NMDA antagonist MK-801 blocks the extinction of Pavlovian fear conditioning

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1996)
  • Y. Benjamini et al.

    Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing

    J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser., B

    (1995)
  • M. Bertero et al.

    Linear inverse problems with discrete data: II. Stability and regularization

    Inverse Probl.

    (1988)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Emotion and motivation II: sex differences in picture processing

    Emotion

    (2001)
  • D.T. Cheng et al.

    Functional MRI of human amygdala activity during Pavlovian fear conditioning: stimulus processing versus response expression

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • M. Corbetta

    Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems?

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (1998)
  • R.J. Davidson et al.

    Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation—A possible prelude to violence

    Science

    (2000)
  • F. Di Russo et al.

    Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention

    Cereb. Cortex.

    (2003)
  • F. Esteves et al.

    Nonconscious associative learning: Pavlovian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fear-relevant facial stimuli

    Psychophysiology

    (1994)
  • W.A. Falls et al.

    Extinction of fear-potentiated startle: blockade by infusion of an NMDA antagonist into the amygdala

    J. Neurosci.

    (1992)
  • M.S. Fanselow

    Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear

    Psychon. Bull. Rev.

    (1994)
  • H. Fischer et al.

    Right-sided human prefrontal brain activation during acquisition of conditioned fear

    Emotion

    (2002)
  • M. Fredrikson et al.

    Affective and attentive neural networks in humans: a PET study of Pavlovian conditioning

    NeuroReport

    (1995)
  • F.K. Graham et al.

    Heart-rate change as a component of the orienting response

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1966)
  • M.S. Hamalainen et al.

    Interpreting magnetic fields of the brain: minimum norm estimates

    Med. Biol. Eng. Comput.

    (1994)
  • A.O. Hamm et al.

    Affective learning: awareness and aversion

    Psychophysiology

    (1996)
  • A.O. Hamm et al.

    Fear and the startle reflex: blink modulation and autonomic response patterns in animal and mutilation fearful subjects

    Psychophysiology

    (1997)
  • A.O. Hamm et al.

    Affective blindsight: intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in a cortically blind patient

    Brain

    (2003)
  • Cited by (61)

    • Neutral is negative enough—Peer victimization influences cardiac, facial-muscular and experiential reactions to both negative evaluative and neutral social stimuli

      2018, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This generalization may reflect hypersensitivity, overarousal or hypervigilance, which is in line with studies reporting that exposure to adverse childhood experiences is associated with hypo- as well as hyperresponsivity (Heim et al., 2000; Kendall-Tackett, 2000; Obradović et al., 2012; Voellmin et al., 2015). Accordingly, enhanced cardiac reactions to the CS-neu may indicate a hyper-responsive disposition for the learning of fear responses to indiscriminate classes of stimuli including neutral faces (Hodes et al., 1985; Moratti & Keil, 2005). Interestingly, a similar pattern of a pronounced indiscriminate orienting response to all classes of stimuli including neutral pictures was reported for a sample of healthy subjects with a history of severe past traumatic events (Adenauer, Catani, Keil, Aichinger, & Neuner, 2010).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text