Elsevier

International Congress Series

Volume 1250, October 2003, Pages 347-370
International Congress Series

The contribution of the amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and prefrontal cortex to emotion and motivated behaviour

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0531-5131(03)01013-6Get rights and content

Abstract

Emotion and motivation depend on the assessment of the value of environmental stimuli. Multiple representations of stimulus value are created in the brain by Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning procedures. The basolateral amygdala (BLA) appears necessary for a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus (CS) to gain access to the current value of the specific unconditioned stimulus (US) that it predicts, while the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) controls brainstem arousal and response systems and subserves some forms of stimulus–response Pavlovian conditioning. The nucleus accumbens (Acb) appears not to be required to represent instrumental action–outcome contingencies, but influences instrumental behaviour strongly by mediating the impact of Pavlovian CSs on instrumental responding, and is required for the normal ability of animals to choose delayed rewards. Prelimbic cortex is required for action–outcome contingency detection, while insular cortex may allow rats to remember the sensory properties of foodstuffs and thereby retrieve their specific values. The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) may represent aspects of reinforcer value governing instrumental choice behaviour. Finally, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) may play a role in responding to the emotional significance of stimuli and preventing responding to inappropriate conditioned stimuli.

Introduction

Emotions and motivation both depend on our ability to assign value to events, objects, and states of the world. Such values drive our lives—thus, we seek out stimuli that are appetitive or rewarding and avoid those that are aversive. Though emotion and motivation are complex, the analysis of the neural basis of emotions has benefited from considering those brain systems that assign value to stimuli. Even within the rat there are many such systems, learning and representing different kinds of information about the world, as associative learning (including the acquisition of emotional value by a stimulus, context or event) is not a simple or unitary phenomenon. Overt behaviour is determined by the interaction of many learning and memory systems, some complementary, some competitive. In this review, the psychological and neural representations that govern two major classes of behaviour, Pavlovian and instrumental conditioned responding, will be summarized. Within this framework, the contributions to emotional and motivated behaviour of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex will be considered, in each case relating neural systems to the psychological representations to which they may correspond.

The term ‘Pavlovian conditioning’ (or classical conditioning) refers to a set of experimental procedures in which an experimenter arranges a contingency between stimuli in the world by presenting those stimuli independent of an animal's behaviour [1]. In a Pavlovian conditioning study, an initially neutral stimulus (such as a bell) is paired with a biologically relevant, unconditioned stimulus (US) (such as food) that normally elicits a reflexive or unconditioned response (UR) such as salivation. As a result of such pairings, the bell becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS), now capable of evoking salivation as a conditioned response (CR). Pavlovian conditioning (CS–US pairings) creates multiple associative representations in the brain [2], [3], [4], [5], [6]. Firstly, and most simply, the CS may become directly associated with the unconditioned response (UR), a stimulus–response association that carries no information about the identity of the US (e.g. Ref. [7]). However, a single US may elicit several responses; for example, a US such as a puff of air delivered to the eye may elicit a simple motor act such as blinking, and a ‘central’ process such as an enhancement of arousal or attention. Therefore, the CS may enter into stimulus–response associations with several kinds of response. Secondly, the CS can evoke a representation of affect—such as fear or the expectation of reward [8]. This embodies the concept of an emotional ‘tone’ that is tagged to a stimulus. Thirdly, the CS can become associated with the specific sensory properties of the US [4], [9], [10]—including its visual appearance, sound, feel, and smell, but also ‘consummatory’ qualities such as its taste and nutritive value.

Multiple representations are also found following instrumental conditioning, in which the experimenter arranges a contingency between the animal's behaviour and a reinforcing outcome [11]. It is apparent that at least six psychological processes contribute to learning and performance of instrumental behaviour [6], [12], [13]. Just as humans can be aware of the goals they seek, rats can conceptualize goals and actions symbolically and such representations can be in the form of declarative or semantic knowledge. Thus, goal-directed action in the rat depends on the twin representations of the instrumental contingency between an action and a particular outcome, and a representation of the outcome as a goal—termed the (instrumental) incentive value of the goal [13], [14]. Simply put, a goal-directed organism presses a lever for food because it knows (1) that lever-pressing produces food and (2) that it wants the food. What is not at all obvious intuitively is that this value, governing instrumental responding, differs from the ‘hedonic’ value that the rat experiences as it actually consumes the food. Though the instrumental incentive value normally tracks the ‘hedonic’ value closely, Dickinson and colleagues have shown in an elegant series of experiments that these values can be dissociated, being different from each other under certain circumstances [6], [12], [13]. A fourth factor governing instrumental performance is the presence of discriminative stimuli (SDs). When responding is rewarded in the presence of a stimulus but not in its absence, that stimulus is established as an SD. SDs inform the subject that a particular instrumental (response–reinforcer) contingency is in force [15], [16], [17], though SDs also have properties of Pavlovian conditioned stimuli [18].

In addition to these sophisticated goal-directed systems, the rat can form stimulus–response habits, as long theorized [11], [19], [20], [21]. S–R habits are the archetype of ‘procedural’ learning: as the S–R association is direct, there is no representation of the reinforcer. If a rat is responding via a goal-directed system for food, and that food is devalued (for example, by pairing it with lithium to induce nausea), the rat will subsequently respond less for that food. However, a rat responding using its habit system, which possesses no information about the reinforcer, will not alter its rate of responding following reinforcer devaluation. Habits appear to develop with extended training; thus, overtraining can cause instrumental responses to become ‘habitual’ and resistant to reinforcer devaluation [12], [22], [23], [24], [25].

Finally, Pavlovian CSs can modulate instrumental performance [12], [13], an effect termed Pavlovian–instrumental transfer (PIT). For example, a stimulus that predicts the arrival of sucrose solution will enhance lever-pressing for sucrose [26], [27]. PIT operates both by providing general conditioned motivation [28], [29] and by selectively potentiating actions that share an outcome with the CS [18]. This Pavlovian motivational process is distinguishable from the values governing instrumental responding, psychologically (e.g. Ref. [30]) and pharmacologically [31]. PIT is functionally important as it probably plays a major role in CS-precipitated reinstatement of instrumental responding, exemplified by cue-induced relapse in drug addiction (see e.g. [32], [33], [34]).

Having summarized the psychological representations known to contribute to Pavlovian and instrumental behaviour in the rat, we will now review the contributions of the amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and prefrontal cortex to emotion and motivated behaviour, using the learning-theory framework outlined above.

Section snippets

The amygdala comprises nuclei involved in emotional learning and expression

Since the demonstration that monkeys with amygdala lesions were ‘fearless’—part of the Klüver–Bucy syndrome [35]—it has been recognized that the amygdala is a key part of the brain's emotional system. Damage to the amygdala in humans may lead to an increase in threshold of emotional perception and expression [36], [37], [38]; amygdala lesions cause impairments in emotional learning [39], deficits in the perception of emotions in facial expressions [40], [41], and impaired memory for emotional

The nucleus accumbens

Motivational effects of emotionally significant stimuli are mediated in part by the ventral striatum, specifically the nucleus accumbens (Acb) [96]. While the Acb conforms broadly to the pattern of the cortico-striatal-pallido-thalamo-cortical ‘loop’ typical of the striatum [97], [98], it is a recipient of information from a considerable array of limbic structures (including the amygdala, hippocampal formation, and regions of the prefrontal cortex) [97] that also projects to structures known to

The prefrontal cortex and its interactions with the amygdala and nucleus accumbens

In the rat, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is a heterogeneous region of the brain that includes the prelimbic, anterior cingulate, agranular insular and orbitofrontal areas [143], [144]. Each of these regions makes a distinct contribution to emotional or motivated behaviour. This final section will review studies that have examined the contribution of the PFC to simple conditioning tasks, primarily in the rat, and will of necessity omit a great deal of research into complex functions of the PFC

Conclusions

Emotion, motivation and reinforcement are not unitary. Pavlovian conditioning creates multiple representations whose neural bases are dissociable (see also Ref. [6]); these include CS–US (sensory) or S–S associations, dependent at least in part on the gustatory neocortex for food USs; CS–US (motivational) associations, suggested to depend on the BLA for both appetitive and aversive conditioning; direct CS–affect associations, which are poorly understood; and CS–response associations, whose

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by an MRC Programme Grant (to B.J.E., T.W. Robbins, and A. Dickinson) and a Wellcome Trust programme grant (to T.W. Robbins, B.J.E., A.C. Roberts, and B.J. Sahakian), and was conducted within the MRC Centre for Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience. R.N.C. was supported by the UK Medical Research Council and the University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine. J.H. was supported by Trinity College, Cambridge and the University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine.

References (195)

  • P.C. Holland

    Amount of training affects associatively-activated event representation

    Neuropharmacology

    (1998)
  • B.J. Everitt et al.

    Interactions between the amygdala and ventral striatum in stimulus–reward associations: studies using a second-order schedule of sexual reinforcement

    Neuroscience

    (1989)
  • M. Cador et al.

    Involvement of the amygdala in stimulus–reward associations: interaction with the ventral striatum

    Neuroscience

    (1989)
  • L.H. Burns et al.

    Differential effects of excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala, ventral subiculum and medial prefrontal cortex on responding with conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity potentiated by intra-accumbens infusions of d-amphetamine

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (1993)
  • B.S. Kapp et al.

    Amygdala central nucleus lesions: effect on heart rate conditioning in the rabbit

    Physiology and Behavior

    (1979)
  • C.G. Gentile et al.

    The role of amygdaloid central nucleus in the retention of differential Pavlovian conditioning of bradycardia in rabbits

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (1986)
  • J. Iwata et al.

    Intrinsic neurons in the amygdaloid field projected to by the medial geniculate body mediate emotional responses conditioned to acoustic stimuli

    Brain Research

    (1986)
  • P.C. Holland et al.

    Amygdala circuitry in attentional and representational processes

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (1999)
  • J.A. Parkinson et al.

    Limbic cortical–ventral striatal systems underlying appetitive conditioning

    Progress in Brain Research

    (2000)
  • G.J. Mogenson et al.

    From motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system

    Progress in Neurobiology

    (1980)
  • L. Zaborszky et al.

    Cholecystokinin innervation of the ventral striatum—a morphological and radioimmunological study

    Neuroscience

    (1985)
  • I.P. Pavlov

    Conditioned Reflexes

    (1927)
  • N.J. Mackintosh

    The Psychology of Animal Learning

    (1974)
  • A. Dickinson

    Contemporary Animal Learning Theory

    (1980)
  • N.J. Mackintosh

    Conditioning and Associative Learning

    (1983)
  • E.R. Kandel

    Cellular mechanisms of learning and the biological basis of individuality

  • A. Dickinson et al.

    Appetitive-aversive interactions and inhibitory processes

  • W.J. Brogden

    Sensory pre-conditioning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1939)
  • P.C. Holland et al.

    Differential effects of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after Pavlovian appetitive conditioning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes

    (1979)
  • E.L. Thorndike

    Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies

    (1911)
  • A. Dickinson et al.

    Motivational control of goal-directed action

    Animal Learning & Behavior

    (1994)
  • E.C. Tolman

    Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men

    (1932)
  • R.M. Colwill et al.

    Evidence for the hierarchical structure of instrumental learning

    Animal Learning & Behavior

    (1990)
  • R.A. Rescorla

    Evidence for an association between the discriminative stimulus and the response-outcome association in instrumental learning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes

    (1990)
  • R.A. Rescorla

    The role of information about the response-outcome relation in instrumental discrimination learning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes

    (1990)
  • R.M. Colwill et al.

    Associations between the discriminative stimulus and the reinforcer in instrumental learning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes

    (1988)
  • G.C. Grindley

    The formation of a simple habit in guinea pigs

    British Journal of Psychology

    (1932)
  • E.R. Guthrie

    The Psychology of Learning

    (1935)
  • C.L. Hull

    Principles of Behavior

    (1943)
  • C.D. Adams

    Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (1982)
  • A. Dickinson et al.

    The effect of the instrumental training contingency on susceptibility to reinforcer devaluation

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (1983)
  • A. Dickinson

    Actions and habits—the development of behavioral autonomy

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences

    (1985)
  • A. Dickinson et al.

    Motivational control after extended instrumental training

    Animal Learning & Behavior

    (1995)
  • W.K. Estes

    Discriminative conditioning: II. Effects of a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus upon a subsequently established operant response

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1948)
  • P.F. Lovibond

    Facilitation of instrumental behavior by a Pavlovian appetitive conditioned stimulus

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes

    (1983)
  • R.A. Rescorla et al.

    Two-process learning theory: relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning

    Psychological Review

    (1967)
  • A. Dickinson et al.

    Pavlovian processes in the motivational control of instrumental performance

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (1987)
  • A. Dickinson

    Re-examination of the role of the instrumental contingency in the sodium-appetite irrelevant incentive effect

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (1986)
  • A. Dickinson et al.

    Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists

    Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • F.H. Gawin

    Cocaine addiction: psychology and neurophysiology

    Science

    (1991)
  • Cited by (45)

    • Altered behaviour, dopamine and norepinephrine regulation in stressed mice heterozygous in TPH2 gene

      2021, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
    • Natural-enriched environments lead to enhanced environmental engagement and altered neurobiological resilience

      2016, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further, the natural-enhanced animals exhibited less activation in the amygdala (along with the artificial-enriched rats), a brain area implicated in fear processing and learning, while, at the same time, exhibited more activity in the nucleus accumbens core, a brain area known for its role in motivation and contingency-driven responses. The increased activation of the nucleus accumbens core via fos-immunoreactivity in the natural habitat animals corroborated prior evidence that this brain area is important for an animal’s ability to suppress actions directed to immediate, small rewards for larger, delayed rewards (Cardinal et al., 2001, 2003). In the current study, solving the swim challenge task required the inhibition of floating and swimming in order to dive down into the tank to find the escape route, ultimately a larger reward than merely keeping afloat.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text