Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 2, Issue 4, December 1981, Pages 267-280
Appetite

Release of Feeding by the Sweet Taste in Rats: The specificity of Oral Satiety

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(81)80016-5Get rights and content

Saccharin intake in hungry rats occurs in discrete “meals”, indicating that a period of saccharin drinking produces a temporary “oral satiety”. We show that such satiety does not suppress the intake of solid food (powdered rat chow), of liquid food (milk), or even of a nutritive sweet solution (glucose) unless it is very dilute. Therefore, oral satiety does not reflect the reduction of a generalized hunger state or of a specific carbohydrate hunger. Having drunk one concentration of saccharin to satiety, the rat will resume drinking if offered a higher concentration but not if offered a lower one, even if the lower one is more palatable than the original concentration. Therefore, in the saccharin-satiated rat, the resumption of drinking requires an increase in stimulus intensity, even if it means a decrease in palatability of the solution offered. We suggest that it is gustatory adaptation that terminates a saccharin meal in the hungry rat.

References (15)

  • D.G. Mook et al.

    Release of feeding by the sweet taste in rats: oropharyngeal satiety

    Appetite: Journal for Intake Research

    (1980)
  • D.G. Mook et al.

    Release of feeding by the sweet taste in rats: the influence of body weight

    Appetite: Journal for Intake Research

    (1981)
  • E.F. Adolph

    Urges to eat and drink in rats

    American Journal of Physiology

    (1947)
  • E.M. Blass et al.

    Drinking termination: interactions among hydrational, orogastric, and behavioral control in rats

    Psychological Review

    (1976)
  • G.H. Burke et al.

    Hyperreactivity to quinine associated with osmotic thirst in the rat

    Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (1972)
  • V.G. Dethier

    Feeding behavior of the blowfly

  • D.H. McBurney

    Gustatory cross adaptation between sweet-tasting compounds

    Perception and Psychophysics

    (1972)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (21)

  • Memory and eating: A bidirectional relationship implicated in obesity

    2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    Citation Excerpt :

    In these experiments, animals are given limited exposure to saccharin to reduce the likelihood of any conditioned responses to the taste. Under such conditions, saccharin has minimal postingestive consequences (Foletto et al., 2016; Mook et al., 1980; Renwick, 1985, 1986; Sclafani and Nissenbaum, 1985) and unlike sucrose, saccharin meal size and meal frequency are controlled primarily by oropharyngeal satiety (Kushner and Mook, 1984; Mook et al., 1980, 1981; Renwick, 1985, 1986; Sclafani and Nissenbaum, 1985). Nonetheless, the possibility remains that dHC inhibition disrupts saccharin intake by interfering with the processing of any mechanical stimulation produced by the saccharin solution in the gut (Waise et al., 2018).

  • The effect of economy type on reinforcer value

    2019, Behavioural Processes
    Citation Excerpt :

    The half-life of saccharin in rats is only about 30 min (Sweatman and Renwick, 1980; Renwick, 1985), which would mean that any saccharin consumed on one day would have been eliminated well before the start of the next day’s work session. Additionally, earlier research indicates that saccharin satiety in rats is controlled by immediate oral sensory stimulation rather than by longer-term post-ingestive consequences such as fullness of the stomach or overhydration (Mook et al., 1981). The short-half life and the lack of post-ingestive consequences for saccharin drinking suggest that the effect of economy type on saccharin reinforcer value observed by Kim et al. (2018) was not due to differences in satiation across economies.

  • The effect of economy type on demand and preference for cocaine and saccharin in rats

    2018, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
    Citation Excerpt :

    It should be noted, however, that the half-life of saccharin in rats is also short enough (∼30 min; Sweatman and Renwick, 1980; Renwick, 1985) so that saccharin consumed on one day would have been eliminated prior to the start of the next day’s work session. Further, saccharin satiety in rats is controlled by immediate oral sensory stimulation rather than by any longer-term post-ingestive consequences such as fullness of the stomach or overhydration (Mook et al., 1981). Nonetheless, the neurobiological mechanisms involved in saccharin satiety are poorly understood and it is possible that residual satiety-like effects of prior saccharin exposure could have been responsible for the OpenSac group’s results.

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text