Characterization of prediction in the primate visual smooth pursuit system

https://doi.org/10.1016/0303-2647(94)01446-EGet rights and content

Abstract

To define predictive behavior and mechanisms in visual smooth pursuit, various target motions were presented to 2 monkeys. Target stimuli included: single sinusoids (1′s), triangle waves (T's), sums of 4 nonharmonically related sinusoids (4′s), bandpass limited white noise (B's), and wideband white noise (N′s). Velocity error was least for 1′s, greatest for N′s, and intermediate for T's, 4′s, and B's. For the bandlimited 4′s and B's, monkeys demonstrated the greatest relative amplitude response at the highest frequencies. Predictive mechanisms are classified as short- and long-term, depending on how much past target motion information is employed. The T's and a modification of this stimulus pattern involving a random perturbation were used to test the hypothesis that prediction is based exclusively on short-term signal processing related to target position and its derivatives. The existence of long-term predictive mechanisms in monkey smooth pursuit was unequivocally demonstrated with the use of the latter stimulus.

References (46)

  • A.T. Bahill et al.

    Model emulates human smooth pursuit system producing zero-latency target tracking

    Biol. Cybern.

    (1983)
  • G.R. Barnes et al.

    Predictive velocity estimation in the pursuit reflex response to pseudorandom and step displacement stimuli in man

    J. Physiol. (Lond.)

    (1987)
  • W. Becker et al.

    Prediction in the oculomotor system: smooth pursuit during transient disappearance of a visual target

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1985)
  • J.R. Carl et al.

    Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1987)
  • D.G. Childers

    Modern Spectral Analysis

    (1978)
  • W.F. Crandall et al.

    Visual and oculomotor signals in nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis in alert monkey

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1985)
  • P.J. Dallos et al.

    Learning behavior of the eye fixation control system

    IEEE Trans. on Auto. Cont.

    (1963)
  • D.C. Deno

    Control theoretic investigations of the visual smooth pursuit system

  • R. Dodge et al.

    Optic nystagmus III. Characteristics of the slow phase

    Arch. Neurol. Psychiat.

    (1930)
  • R. Eckmiller et al.

    Pursuit eye movements and their neural control in the monkey

    Pflugers Arch.

    (1978)
  • D.H. Fender

    Techniques of systems-analysis applied to feedback pathways in the control of eye movements

  • D.H. Fender et al.

    An investigation of the mechanisms of eye movement control

    Kybernetik

    (1961)
  • K.A. Flowers

    The predictive control of behavior: appropriate and inappropriate and inappropriate actions beyond the input in a tracking task

    Ergonomics

    (1978)
  • Cited by (41)

    • Synchronizing the tracking eye movements with the motion of a visual target: Basic neural processes

      2017, Progress in Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Because of the visuomotor delays, the ability to generate eye movements which are locked onto the current target location is obviously not possible unless the upcoming trajectory is already known. Several studies have shown that the velocity of slow eye movements hardly match the target speed when an unpredictable target is being tracked (e.g., Bahill et al., 1980; Barnes and Collins, 2015; Deno et al., 1995; Miall et al., 1986; Michael and Melvill-Jones, 1966; Mrotek et al., 2006; St-Cyr and Fender, 1969). The presence of numerous catch-up saccades makes the tracking look intermittent.

    • Active inference and oculomotor pursuit: The dynamic causal modelling of eye movements

      2015, Journal of Neuroscience Methods
      Citation Excerpt :

      In terms of modelling, we have also shown that it is possible to use empirical data to inform (invert) relatively sophisticated Bayesian or normative models of behaviour. There are many carefully constructed and validated descriptive SPEM models in the literature (e.g. Barnes, 2008; Deno et al., 1995; Krauzlis and Lisberger, 1989; Krauzlis, 2004; Lisberger, 2010; Robinson et al., 1986; Shibata et al., 2005): however, the generative model that we used is distinguished in the sense that it is a special case of generic (predictive coding) models that conform to normative (Bayesian) principles. We have previously shown that formally similar generative models can reproduce both control and schizophrenic subjects’ pursuit of targets whose occlusion is either expected or unexpected, and of targets that unpredictably change direction (Adams et al., 2012).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text